Full text of "Robert Whitaker - Anatomy Of An Epidemic
Full text of "Robert Whitaker - Anatomy Of An Epidemic: Magic Bullets, Psychiatric Drugs, And The Astonishing Rise Of Mental Illness In America"
“lucid, pointed, and important, Anatomy of tin Epidemic should be required reading lor anyone
considering extended use ol psychiatric medicine. Whitaker is at the height ol his powers.”
— Greg Critser, author oe Generation Rx
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Magic Bullets, Psychiatric Drugs, and the
Astonishing Rise of Mental Illness in America
ROBERT WHITAKER
Author of Mad in America
$26.00
(Canada: $32.00)
I n this astonishing and startling book, award¬
winning science and history writer Robert Whita¬
ker investigates a medical mystery: Why has the
number of disabled mentally ill in the United States
tripled over the past two decades? Every day 1,100
adults and children are added to the government dis¬
ability rolls because they have become newly disabled
by mental illness, with this epidemic spreading most
rapidly among our nation's children. What is going on?
Anatomy of an Epidemic challenges readers to think
through that question themselves. First, Whitaker
investigates what is known today about the biological
causes of mental disorders. Do psychiatric medications
fix "chemical imbalances" in the brain, or do they, in
fact, create them? Researchers spent decades studying
that question, and by the late 1980s, they had their
answer. Readers will be startled—and dismayed—to
discover what was reported in the scientific journals.
Then comes the scientific query at the heart of
this book: During the past fifty years, when investi¬
gators looked at how psychiatric drugs affected long¬
term outcomes, what did they find? Did they discover
that the drugs help people stay well? Function better?
Enjoy good physical health? Or did they find that these
medications, for some paradoxical reason, increase the
likelihood that people will become chronically ill, less
able to function well, more prone to physical illness?
This is the first book to look at the merits of psy¬
chiatric medications through the prism oflong-term
results. Are long-term recovery rates higher for medi¬
cated or unmedicated schizophrenia patients? Does
taking an antidepressant decrease or increase the risk
that a depressed person will become disabled by the
disorder? Do bipolar patients fare better today than
they did forty years ago, or much worse? When the
National Institute ofMental Health (NIMH) studied
the long-term outcomes of children with ADHD, did
they determine that stimulants provide any benefit?
By the end of this review of the outcomes lit¬
erature, readers are certain to have a haunting
question of their own: Why have the results from these
(continued on back flap)
Also by Robert Whitaker
Mad in America
The Maptnaker's Wife
On the Laps of Gods
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Magic Bullets,
Psychiatric Drugs,
and the
Astonishing Rise of
Mental Illness
in America
Robert Whitaker
CROWN PUBLISHERS
New York
Copyright © 2010 by Robert Whitaker
All rights reserved.
Published in the United States by Crown Publishers, an imprint of the Crown Publishing
Group, a division of Random House, Inc., New York.
www.crownpublishing.com
CROWN and the Crown colophon are registered trademarks of Random House, Inc.
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Whitaker, Robert.
Anatomy of an epidemic : magic bullets, psychiatric drugs, and the astonishing rise of
mental illness in America / Robert Whitaker,
p. ; cm.
Includes bibliographical references and index.
1. Mental illness—United States. 2. Psychotropic drugs —Prescribing —United States.
3. Psychiatry —United States. I. Title.
[DNLM: 1. Psychiatry—ethics —United States. 2. Psychiatry —history —United States.
3. Mental Disorders —drug therapy —United States. 4. Mental Disorders —
epidemiology —United States. WM 11 AA1 W578a 2010]
RC443.W437 2010
616.89 —dc22 2009049467
ISBN 978-0-307-45241-2
Printed in the United States of America
Illustrations by Hadel Studio, Westbury NY
10 987654321
First Edition
To Lindsay
May you sing "Seasons of Love" again
and be filled with joy
CONTENTS
Foreword • ix
Part One: The Epidemic
i. A Modern Plague • 3
2. Anecdotal Thoughts • 12
Part Two: The Science of Psychiatric Drugs
3 . The Roots of an Epidemic *39
4. Psychiatry's Magic Bullets *47
5. The Hunt for Chemical Imbalances • 67
Part Three: Outcomes
6 . A Paradox Revealed • 89
7. The Benzo Trap *126
8. An Episodic Illness Turns Chronic • 148
9 . The Bipolar Boom • 1 72
10. An Epidemic Explained • 20 $
V i i i . CONTENTS
ii. The Epidemic Spreads to Children 216
12. Suffer the Children 247
Part Four: Explication of a Delusion
13. The Rise of an Ideology 263
14. The Story That Was ... and Wasn't Told 283
15. Tallying Up the Profits 313
Part Five: Solutions
16. Blueprints for Reform 331
Epilogue 361
Notes 363
Acknowledgments 3 9 5
Index 397
FOREWORD
The history of psychiatry and its treatments can be a contentious
issue in our society, so much so that when you write about it, as I
did in an earlier book, Mad in America, people regularly ask about
how you became interested in the subject. The assumption is that
you must have a personal reason for being curious about this topic,
as otherwise you would want to stay away from what can be such a
political minefield. In addition, the person asking the question is
often trying to determine if you have any personal bias that colors
your writing.
In my case, I had no personal attachment to the subject at all. I
came to it in a very back-door manner.
In 1994, after having worked a number of years as a newspaper
reporter, I left daily journalism to cofound a publishing company,
CenterWatch, that reported on the business aspects of the clinical
testing of new drugs. Our readers came from pharmaceutical com¬
panies, medical schools, private medical practices, and Wall Street,
and for the most part, we wrote about this enterprise in an industry-
friendly way. We viewed clinical trials as part of a process that
brought improved medical treatments to market, and we reported
on the financial aspects of that growing industry. Then, in early
1998, I stumbled upon a story that told of the abuse of psychiatric
X
FOREWORD
patients in research settings. Even while I co-owned CenterWatch, I
occasionally wrote freelance articles for magazines and newspapers,
and that fall I cowrote a series on this problem for the Boston
Globe.
There were several types of "abuses” that Dolores Kong and I
focused on. We looked at studies funded by the National Institute of
Mental Health (NIMH) that involved giving schizophrenia patients
a drug designed to exacerbate their symptoms (the studies were
probing the biology of psychosis). We investigated the deaths that
had occurred during the testing of the new atypical antipsychotics.
Finally, we reported on studies that involved withdrawing schizo¬
phrenia patients from their antipsychotic medications, which we
figured was an unethical thing to do. In fact, we thought it was out¬
rageous.
Our reasoning was easy to understand. These drugs were said to
be like "insulin for diabetes.” I had known that to be "true” for
some time, ever since I had covered the medical beat at the Albany
Times Union. Clearly, then, it was abusive for psychiatric re¬
searchers to have run dozens of withdrawal studies in which they
carefully tallied up the percentage of schizophrenia patients who be¬
came sick again and had to be rehospitalized. Would anyone ever
conduct a study that involved withdrawing insulin from diabetics to
see how fast they became sick again?
That's how we framed the withdrawal studies in our series, and
that would have been the end of my writing on psychiatry except
for the fact that I was left with an unresolved question, one that
nagged at me. While reporting that series, I had come upon two re¬
search findings that just didn't make sense. The first was by Har¬
vard Medical School investigators, who in 1994 announced that
outcomes for schizophrenia patients in the United States had wors¬
ened during the past two decades and were now no better than they
had been a century earlier. The second was by the World Health Or¬
ganization, which had twice found that schizophrenia outcomes
were much better in poor countries, like India and Nigeria, than in
the United States and other rich countries. I interviewed various ex¬
perts about the WHO findings, and they suggested that the poor
outcomes in the United States were due to social policies and cul-
FOREWORD
xi
tural values. In the poor countries, families were more supportive of
those with schizophrenia, they said. Although this seemed plausible,
it wasn't an altogether satisfactory explanation, and after the series
ran in the Boston Globe, I went back and read all of the scientific
articles related to the WHO study on schizophrenia outcomes. It
was then that I learned of this startling fact: In the poor countries,
only 16 percent of patients were regularly maintained on anti¬
psychotic medications.
That is the story of my entry into the psychiatry "minefield.” I
had just cowritten a series that had focused, in one of its parts, on
how unethical it was to withdraw schizophrenia patients from their
medications, and yet here was a study by the World Health Orga¬
nization that seemingly had found an association between good
outcomes and not staying continuously on the drugs. I wrote Mad
in America, which turned into a history of our country's treatment
of the severely mentally ill, to try to understand how that could be.
I confess all this for a simple reason. Since psychiatry is such a
controversial topic, I think it is important that readers understand
that I began this long intellectual journey as a believer in the con¬
ventional wisdom. I believed that psychiatric researchers were dis¬
covering the biological causes of mental illnesses and that this
knowledge had led to the development of a new generation of
psychiatric drugs that helped "balance” brain chemistry. These
medications were like "insulin for diabetes.” I believed that to be
true because that is what I had been told by psychiatrists while writ¬
ing for newspapers. But then I stumbled upon the Harvard study
and the WHO findings, and that set me off on an intellectual quest
that ultimately grew into this book, Anatomy of an Epidemic.
Part One
The Epidemic
I
A Modern Plague
'That is the essence of science: ask an impertinent
question, and you are on the way to
a pertinent answer."
-JACOB BRONOWSKI ( 1973 )'
This is the story of a medical puzzle. The puzzle is of a most curious
sort, and yet one that we as a society desperately need to solve, for
it tells of a hidden epidemic that is diminishing the lives of millions
of Americans, including a rapidly increasing number of children. The
epidemic has grown in size and scope over the past five decades, and
now disables 850 adults and 250 children every day. And those star¬
tling numbers only hint at the true scope of this modern plague, for
they are only a count of those who have become so ill that their
families or caregivers are newly eligible to receive a disability check
from the federal government.
Now, here is the puzzle.
As a society, we have come to understand that psychiatry has made
great progress in treating mental illness over the past fifty years. Scien¬
tists are uncovering the biological causes of mental disorders, and
pharmaceutical companies have developed a number of effective med¬
ications for these conditions. This story has been told in newspapers,
magazines, and books, and evidence of our societal belief in it can be
found in our spending habits. In 2007, we spent $25 billion on anti¬
depressants and antipsychotics, and to put that figure in perspective,
that was more than the gross domestic product of Cameroon, a nation
of 18 million people. 2
4
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
In 1999, U.S. surgeon general David Satcher neatly summed up
this story of scientific progress in a 458-page report titled Mental
Health. The modern era of psychiatry, he explained, could be said
to have begun in 1954. Prior to that time, psychiatry lacked treat¬
ments that could "prevent patients from becoming chronically ill."
But then Thorazine was introduced. This was the first drug that was
a specific antidote to a mental disorder —it was an antipsychotic
medication —and it kicked off a psychopharmacological revolution.
Soon antidepressants and antianxiety agents were discovered,
and as a result, today we enjoy "a variety of treatments of well-
documented efficacy for the array of clearly defined mental and be¬
havioral disorders that occur across the life span," Satcher wrote.
The introduction of Prozac and other "second-generation" psychi¬
atric drugs, the surgeon general added, was "stoked by advances in
both neurosciences and molecular biology" and represented yet an¬
other leap forward in the treatment of mental disorders. 3
Medical students training to be psychiatrists read about this his¬
tory in their textbooks, and the public reads about it in popular
accounts of the field. Thorazine, wrote University of Toronto pro¬
fessor Edward Shorter, in his 1997 book, A History of Psychiatry,
"initiated a revolution in psychiatry, comparable to the introduc¬
tion of penicillin in general medicine." 4 That was the start of the
"psychopharmacology era," and today we can rest assured that sci¬
ence has proved that the drugs in psychiatry's medicine cabinet are
beneficial. "We have very effective and safe treatments for a broad
array of psychiatric disorders,” Richard Friedman, director of the
psychopharmacology clinic at Weill Cornell Medical College, in¬
formed readers of the New York Times on June 19, 2007. 5 Three
days later, the Boston Globe, in an editorial titled "When Kids Need
Meds," echoed this sentiment: "The development of powerful drugs
has revolutionized the treatment of mental illness." 6
Psychiatrists working in countries around the world also under¬
stand this to be true. At the 161st annual meeting of the American
Psychiatric Association, which was held in May 2008 in Washington,
D.C., nearly half of the twenty thousand psychiatrists who attended
were foreigners. The hallways were filled with chatter about schizo¬
phrenia, bipolar illness, depression, panic disorder, attention deficit/
A MODERN PLAGUE
5
hyperactivity disorder, and a host of other conditions described in
the APA's Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,
and over the course of five days, most of the lectures, workshops,
and symposiums told of advances in the field. "We have come a
long way in understanding psychiatric disorders, and our knowl¬
edge continues to expand,” APA president Carolyn Robinowitz told
the audience in her opening-day address. "Our work saves and
improves so many lives." 7
But here is the conundrum. Given this great advance in care, we
should expect that the number of disabled mentally ill in the United
States, on a per-capita basis, would have declined over the past fifty
years. We should also expect that the number of disabled mentally
ill, on a per-capita basis, would have declined since the arrival in
1988 of Prozac and the other second-generation psychiatric drugs.
We should see a two-step drop in disability rates. Instead, as the
psychopharmacology revolution has unfolded, the number of dis¬
abled mentally ill in the United States has skyrocketed. Moreover,
this increase in the number of disabled mentally ill has accelerated
further since the introduction of Prozac and the other second-
generation psychiatric drugs. Most disturbing of all, this modern-
day plague has now spread to the nation's children.
The disability numbers, in turn, lead to a much larger question.
Why are so many Americans today, while they may not be disabled
by mental illness, nevertheless plagued by chronic mental problems —
by recurrent depression, by bipolar symptoms, and by crippling
anxiety? If we have treatments that effectively address these disor¬
ders, why has mental illness become an ever-greater health problem
in the United States?
The Epidemic
Now, I promise that this will not just be a book of statistics. We are
trying to solve a mystery in this book, and this will lead to an ex¬
ploration of science and history, and ultimately to a story with
many surprising twists. But this mystery arises from an in-depth
6
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
analysis of government statistics, and so, as a first step, we need to
track the disability numbers over the past fifty years to make certain
that the epidemic is real.
In 1955, the disabled mentally ill were primarily cared for in state
and county mental hospitals. Today, they typically receive either a
monthly Supplemental Security Income (SSI) or Social Security
Disability Insurance (SSDI) payment, and many live in residential
shelters or other subsidized living arrangements. Both statistics pro¬
vide a rough count of the number of people under governmental
care because they have been disabled by mental illness.
In 1955, there were 566,000 people in state and county mental
hospitals. However, only 355,000 had a psychiatric diagnosis, as
the rest suffered from alcoholism, syphilis-related dementia, Alz-
The Hospitalized Mentally III in 1955
First Admissions
Resident Patients
Psychotic Disorders
Schizophrenia
28,482
267,603
Manic-depressive
9,679
50,937
Other
1,387
1 4,734
Psychoneurosis (Anxiety)
6,549
5,41 5
Personality Disorders
8,730
9,739
All Others
6,497
6,966
Although there were 558,922 resident patients in state and county mental hospitals in 1 955,
only 355,000 suffered from mental illness. The other 200,000 were elderly patients suffering from
dementia, end-stage syphilis, alcoholism, mental retardation, and various neurological syn¬
dromes. Source: Silverman, C. The Epidemiology of Depression (1 968): 1 39.
heimer's, and mental retardation, a population that would not show
up in a count of the disabled mentally ill today. 8 Thus, in 1955, 1 in
every 468 Americans was hospitalized due to a mental illness. In
1987, there were 1.25 million people receiving an SSI or SSDI pay¬
ment because they were disabled by mental illness, or 1 in every 184
Americans.
A MODERN PLAGUE
7
Now it may be argued that this is an apples-to-oranges com¬
parison. In 1955, societal taboos about mental illness may have led
to a reluctance to seek treatment, and thus to low hospitalization
rates. It's also possible that a person had to be sicker to get hos¬
pitalized in 1955 than to receive SSI or SSDI in 1987, and that's why
the 1987 disability rate is so much higher. However, arguments can
be made in the other direction, too. The SSI and SSDI numbers only
provide a count of the disabled mentally ill less than sixty-five years
old, whereas the mental hospitals in 1955 were home to many el¬
derly schizophrenics. There were also many more mentally ill people
who were homeless and in jail in 1987 than in 1955, and that pop¬
ulation doesn't show up in the disability numbers. The comparison
is an imperfect one, but it's the best one we can make to track dis¬
ability rates between 1955 and 1987.
Fortunately, from 1987 forward it's an apples-to-apples compar¬
ison, involving only the SSI and SSDI numbers. The Food and Drug
Administration approved Prozac in 1987, and over the next two
decades the number of disabled mentally ill on the SSI and SSDI
rolls soared to 3.97 million. 9 In 2007, the disability rate was 1 in
every 76 Americans. That's more than double the rate in 1987, and
six times the rate in 1955. The apples-to-apples comparison proves
that something is amiss.
If we drill down into the disability data a bit more, we find a sec¬
ond puzzle. In 1955, major depression and bipolar illness didn't
disable many people. There were only 50,937 people in state and
county mental hospitals with a diagnosis for one of those affective
disorders. 10 But during the 1990s, people struggling with depression
and bipolar illness began showing up on the SSI and SSDI rolls in
ever-increasing numbers, and today there are an estimated 1.4 mil¬
lion people eighteen to sixty-four years old receiving a federal
payment because they are disabled by an affective disorder." More¬
over, this trend is accelerating: According to a 2008 report by the
U.S. General Accountability Office, 46 percent of the young adults
(ages eighteen to twenty-six) who received an SSI or SSDI payment
because of a psychiatric disability in 2006 were diagnosed with an
affective illness (and another 8 percent were disabled by "anxiety
disorder"). 12
8
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
The Disabled Mentally III in the Prozac Era
SSI and SSDI Recipients Under Age 65 Disabled by Mental Illness, 1987-2007
One in every six SSDI recipients also receives an SSI payment; thus the total number of recipients
is less than the sum ofthe SSI and SSDI numbers. Source: Social Security Administration reports,
1 987-2007.
This plague of disabling mental illness has now spread to our
children, too. In 1987, there were 16,200 children under eighteen
years of age who received an SSI payment because they were dis¬
abled by a serious mental illness. Such children comprised only 5.5
percent of the 293,000 children on the disability rolls —mental ill¬
ness was not, at that time, a leading cause of disability among the
country's children. But starting in 1990, the number of mentally ill
children began to rise dramatically, and by the end of 2007, there
were 561,569 such children on the SSI disability rolls. In the short
span of twenty years, the number of disabled mentally ill children
rose thirty-five fold. Mental illness is now the leading cause of dis¬
ability in children, with the mentally ill group comprising 50 per¬
cent of the total number of children on the SSI rolls in 2007. 13
The baffling nature of this childhood epidemic shows up with
particular clarity in the SSI data from 1996 to 2007. Whereas the
number of children disabled by mental illness more than doubled
during this period, the number of children on the SSI rolls for all
other reasons —cancers, retardation, etc . — declined, from 728,110
A MODERN PLAGUE
9
to 559,448. The nation's doctors were apparently making progress
in treating all of those other conditions, but when it came to mental
disorders, just the opposite was true.
A Scientific Inquiry
The puzzle can now be precisely summed up. On the one hand, we
know that many people are helped by psychiatric medications.
We know that many people stabilize well on them and will person¬
ally attest to how the drugs have helped them lead normal lives.
Furthermore, as Satcher noted in his 1999 report, the scientific liter¬
ature does document that psychiatric medications, at least over the
short term, are "effective.” Psychiatrists and other physicians who
prescribe the drugs will attest to that fact, and many parents of chil¬
dren taking psychiatric drugs will swear by the drugs as well. All of
that makes for a powerful consensus: Psychiatric drugs work and
help people lead relatively normal lives. And yet, at the same time,
we are stuck with these disturbing facts: The number of disabled
mentally ill has risen dramatically since 1955, and during the past
two decades, a period when the prescribing of psychiatric medica¬
tions has exploded, the number of adults and children disabled by
mental illness has risen at a mind-boggling rate. Thus we arrive at
an obvious question, even though it is heretical in kind: Could our
drug-based paradigm of care, in some unforeseen way, be fueling
this modern-day plague?
My hope is that Anatomy of an Epidemic will serve as an explor¬
ation of that question. It's also easy to see what we must find if we
are to solve this puzzle. We will need to discover a history of science
that unfolds over the course of fifty-five years, arises from the very
best research, and explains all aspects of our puzzle. The history
must reveal why there has been a dramatic increase in the number
of disabled mentally ill, it must explain why disabling affective dis¬
orders are so much more common now than they were fifty years
ago, and it must explain why so many children are being laid low by
serious mental illness today. And if we find such a history, we
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
l o
should then be able to explain why it has remained hidden and
unknown.
It's also easy to see what is at stake here. The disability numbers
only hint at the extraordinary toll that mental illness is exacting on
our society. The GAO, in its June 2008 report, concluded that one
in every sixteen young adults in the United States is now "seriously
mentally ill." There has never been a society that has seen such a
plague of mental illness in its newly minted adults, and those who
go on the SSI and SSDI rolls at this young age are likely to spend the
rest of their lives receiving disability payments. The twenty-year-old
who goes on SSI or SSDI will receive more than $1 million in bene¬
fits over the next forty or so years, and that is a cost —should this
epidemic continue to grow —that our society will not be able to
afford.
There is one other, subtler aspect to this epidemic. Over the past
twenty-five years, psychiatry has profoundly reshaped our society.
Through its Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, psychiatry draws a
line between what is "normal" and what is not. Our societal under¬
standing of the human mind, which in the past arose from a medley
of sources (great works of fiction, scientific investigations, and
philosophical and religious writings), is now filtered through the
DSM. Indeed, the stories told by psychiatry about "chemical imbal¬
ances" in the brain have reshaped our understanding of how the
mind works and challenged our conceptions of free will. Are we
really the prisoners of our neurotransmitters? Most important, our
children are the first in human history to grow up under the con¬
stant shadow of "mental illness.” Not too long ago, goof-offs, cut¬
ups, bullies, nerds, shy kids, teachers' pets, and any number of other
recognizable types filled the schoolyard, and all were considered
more or less normal. Nobody really knew what to expect from such
children as adults. That was part of the glorious uncertainty of life —
the goof-off in the fifth grade might show up at his high school's
twenty-year reunion as a wealthy entrepreneur, the shy girl as an
accomplished actress. But today, children diagnosed with mental
disorders —most notably, ADHD, depression, and bipolar illness-
help populate the schoolyard. These children have been told that
they have something wrong with their brains and that they may
A MODERN PLAGUE
I
have to take psychiatric medications the rest of their lives, just like a
"diabetic takes insulin.” That medical dictum teaches all of the chil¬
dren on the playground a lesson about the nature of humankind,
and that lesson differs in a radical way from what children used to
be taught.
So here is what is at stake in this investigation: If the conven¬
tional history is true, and psychiatry has in fact made great progress
in identifying the biological causes of mental disorders and in devel¬
oping effective treatments for those illnesses, then we can conclude
that psychiatry's reshaping of our society has been for the good. As
bad as the epidemic of disabling mental illness may be, it is reason¬
able to assume that without such advances in psychiatry, it would
be much worse. The scientific literature will show that millions of
children and adults are being helped by psychiatric medications,
their lives made richer and fuller, just as APA president Carolyn
Robinowitz said in her speech at the APA's 2008 convention. But if
we uncover a history of a different sort —a history that shows that
the biological causes of mental disorders remain to be discovered
and that psychiatric drugs are in fact fueling the epidemic of dis¬
abling mental illness what then? We will have documented a his¬
tory that tells of a society led horribly astray and, one might say,
betrayed.
And if that is so, we will spend the final part of this book looking
at what we, as a society, might do to forge a different future.
2
Anecdotal Thoughts
"If we value the pursuit of knowledge, we must he
free to follow wherever that search may lead us."
— ADLAI STEVENSON ( 1952 )'
McLean Hospital in Belmont, Massachusetts, is one of the oldest
mental hospitals in the United States, as it was founded in 1817,
when a type of care known as moral therapy was being popularized
by Quakers. Their belief was that a retreat for the mentally ill
should be built in a pastoral setting, and even today the McLean
campus, with its handsome brick buildings and shaded lawns, feels
like an oasis. On the evening in August 2008 that I came there, in
order to attend a meeting of the Depression and Bipolar Support Al¬
liance, that sense of tranquility was heightened by the weather. It
was one of the most gorgeous nights of the summer, and as I ap¬
proached the cafeteria where the meeting was to be held, I figured
that attendance that night would be sparse. It was just too nice of a
night to be inside. This was a meeting for people living in the com¬
munity, which meant they would have to leave their homes and
apartments to come here, and given that the McLean group met five
times a week —there was an afternoon session every Monday, Thurs¬
day, Friday, and Saturday, and an evening meeting every Wednes¬
day—I reasoned that most people attached to the group would skip
this one.
I was wrong.
There were a hundred or so people filling the cafeteria, a scene
ANECDOTAL THOUGHTS
13
that, in a small way, bore witness to the epidemic of disabling men¬
tal illness that has erupted in our country over the past twenty
years. The Depression and Bipolar Support Alliance (DBSA) was
founded in 1985 (known initially as the Depressive and Manic-
Depressive Association), with this group at McLean starting up
shortly after that, and today the organization counts nearly one
thousand of its support groups nationwide. There are seven such
groups in the Greater Boston area alone, and most —like the group
that meets at McLean —offer people a chance to get together and
talk several times a week. The DBSA has grown in lockstep with the
epidemic.
The first hour of the meeting was given over to a talk about
"flotation therapy," and at first glance, the audience was really not
identifiable —at least not by an outsider such as myself —as a patient
group. The people here ranged widely in age, the youngest in their
late teens and the oldest in their sixties, and although the women
outnumbered the men, this gender disparity might have been ex¬
pected, given that depression affects more women than men. Most
in the audience were white, which perhaps reflected the fact that
Belmont is an affluent town. Perhaps the one telltale sign that the
meeting was for people diagnosed with a mental illness was that a
fair number were overweight. People diagnosed with bipolar disor¬
der are often prescribed an atypical antipsychotic, such as Zyprexa,
and those drugs regularly cause people to put on the pounds.
After the talk ended, Steve Lappen, one of the DBSA leaders in
Boston, listed the various groups that would now meet. There was
one for "newcomers," another for "family and friends," a third for
"young adults," a fourth for "maintaining stability,” and so on,
with the last of the eight choices an "observer's group,” which Steve
had organized for me.
There were nine in our group (excluding myself), and by way of
introduction, everyone briefly spoke about how he or she had
been doing lately — "I've been having a hard time” was a common
refrain —and told of his or her specific diagnosis. The man to my
right was a former executive who had lost his job because of his
recurring depression, and as we went around the room, such life
stories spilled out. A younger woman told of a troubled marriage to
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
14
a Chinese man who, because of his culture, didn't like to talk about
mental illness. Next to her, a former prosecuting attorney spoke of
how he'd lost his wife two years ago, and since then "I don't feel
like I know who I am.” A woman who was an adjunct professor at
an area college told of how difficult her work was at the moment,
and finally, a nurse who had been recently hospitalized at McLean
for depression explained what drove her to that dark place: She had
the stress of caring for an ailing father, the stress of her job, and
years of living with "an abusive husband.”
The one lighter moment in this round of introductions came from
the oldest member of the group. He had been doing pretty well
lately, and his explanation for his relative happiness was one that
Seinfield's George Costanza would have appreciated. "Usually the
summer is a hard time for me because everybody seems so happy.
But with all the rain we have been having, that hasn't been so much
the case this summer," he said.
Over the course of the next hour, the talk jumped from topic to
topic. There was a discussion of the stigma that the mentally ill face
in our society, particularly in the workplace, and talk too of how
family and friends, after a time, lose their empathy. This was clearly
why many in the group had come —they found the shared under¬
standing to be helpful. The issue of medication came up, and on this
topic, opinions and experiences varied widely. The former execu¬
tive, while still regularly suffering from depression, said that his
medication did "wonders" for him and that his greatest fear was
that it would "stop working.” Others told of having tried one
medication after another before finding a drug regimen that pro¬
vided some relief. Steve Lappen said that medications had never
worked for him, while Dennis Hagler, the other DBSA leader in the
meeting (who also agreed to be identified), said that a high dose of
an antidepressant has made all the difference in the world in his life.
The nurse told of having responded very badly to antidepressants
during her recent hospitalization.
"I had an allergic reaction to five different drugs," she said. "I am
now trying one of the new atypicals [antipsychotics]. I'm hoping
that will work."
After the group sessions ended, people gathered in the cafeteria in
ANECDOTAL THOUGHTS
15
clutches of two and three, sharing small talk. That made for a pleas¬
ant moment; there was a feeling of social warmth in that room, and
you could see that the evening had lifted the spirits of many. It was
all so ordinary that this easily could have been the wrap-up moment
to a PTA meeting or a church social, and as I walked to the car, it
was that ordinariness that struck me most. In the observer's group,
there had been a businessman, an engineer, a historian, an attorney,
a college professor, a social worker, and a nurse (the other two in
the group hadn't spoken of their work histories). Yet, as far as I
could tell, only the college professor was currently employed. And
that was the puzzle: The people in the observer's group were well
educated and they were all taking psychotropic medications, and
yet many were so plagued by persistent depression and bipolar
symptoms that they couldn't work.
Earlier, Steve had told me that about half of the DBSA members
receive either an SSI or SSDI check because they are, in the govern¬
ment's eyes, disabled by their mental illness. This is the patient type
that has been swelling the SSI and SSDI rolls for the past fifteen
years, while the DBSA has grown into the largest mental health pa¬
tient organization in the country during that time. Psychiatry now
has three classes of medications it uses to treat affective disorders —
antidepressants, mood stabilizers, and atypical antipsychotics —but
for whatever reason, an ever greater number of people are showing
up at DBSA meetings around the country, telling of their persistent
and enduring struggles with depression or mania or both.
Four Stories
In medicine, the personal stories of patients diagnosed with a dis¬
ease are known as "case studies," and it is understood that these an¬
ecdotal accounts, while they might provide insight into a disease
and the treatments for it, cannot prove whether a treatment works.
Only scientific studies that look at outcomes in the aggregate can do
that, and even then the picture that emerges is often a cloudy one.
The reason that anecdotal accounts can't provide such proof is that
l 6
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
people may have widely varying reactions to medical treatments,
and that is particularly true in psychiatry. You can find people who
will tell of how psychiatric medications have helped them im¬
mensely; you can find people who will tell of how the drugs have
ruined their lives; and you can find people —and this seems to be the
majority in my experience —who don't know what to think. They
can't quite decide whether the drugs have helped them or not. Still,
as we set out to solve this puzzle of a modern-day epidemic of dis¬
abling mental illness in the United States, anecdotal accounts can
help us identify questions that we will want to see answered in our
search of the scientific literature.
Here are four such life stories.
Cathy Levin
I first met Cathy Levin in 2004, not too long after I had published
my first book on psychiatry, Mad in America. I immediately came to
admire her fierce spirit. The last part of that book explored whether
antipsychotic medications might be worsening the long-term course
of schizophrenia (a topic that is explored in Chapter 6 of this book),
and Cathy, in some ways, objected to that thought. Although she
had initially been diagnosed with bipolar disorder (in 1978), her di¬
agnosis had later been changed to ''schizoaffective,” and she had,
by her own reckoning, been "saved” by an atypical antipsychotic,
Risperdal. The history that I had related in Mad in America threat¬
ened, in some way, her own personal experience, and she called me
several times to tell me how helpful that drug had been to her.
Born in 1960 in a Boston suburb, Cathy grew up in what she re¬
members as a "male-dominated” world. Her father, a professor at a
college in the Boston area, was a veteran of World War II, and her
stay-at-home mom saw such men as the "backbone of the social
order." Her two older brothers, she recalls, "bullied her," and on
more than one occasion, starting when she was quite young, several
boys in her neighborhood molested her. "I cried all the time when I
was a child," she says, and often she pretended to be sick so that she
wouldn't have to go to school, preferring instead to spend her days
alone in her room, reading books.
ANECDOTAL THOUGHTS
17
Although she did fine academically in high school, she was "a
difficult teenager, hostile, angry, withdrawn." During her second
year in college, at Earlham in Richmond, Indiana, her emotional
troubles worsened. She began partying with the young men on the
football team, eager, she says, "to have sex” but, at the same time,
worried about losing her virginity. "I was confused about being in¬
volved with a guy. I went to a lot of parties and I couldn't concen¬
trate anymore on my studies. I started to flunk out of school."
Cathy was smoking a lot of marijuana, too, and soon she began
acting in an eccentric manner. She borrowed other people's clothes
to wear, trekking around campus in "oversized clogs, a pair of over¬
alls thrown over my regular clothes, a bomber jacket, and a funny
hat I got from the Army-Navy store.” One night, on her way home
from a party, she threw away her glasses for no reason. Her
thoughts about sex gradually bloomed into a fantasy about Steve
Martin, the comedian. Unable to sleep through the night, she would
awaken at four a.m. and go for walks, and at times, it seemed that
Steve Martin was there on campus, stalking her. "I thought he was
in love with me and was running through the bushes just out of
sight,” she says. "He was looking for me.”
Mania and paranoia were combining into a volatile mix. The
breaking point came one evening when she threw a glass object
against the wall in her dorm room. "I didn't clean it up, but instead
was walking around in it. I was, you know, taking the glass out of
my feet. I was completely out of my mind.” School officials called
police and she was rushed off to a hospital, and it was then, a few
days before her eighteenth birthday, that Cathy's medicated life
began. She was diagnosed with manic-depressive illness, informed
that she suffered from a chemical imbalance in the brain, and put on
Haldol and lithium.
For the next sixteen years, Cathy cycled in and out of hospitals.
She "hated the meds" —Haldol stiffened her muscles and caused her
to drool, while the lithium made her depressed —and often she
would abruptly stop taking them. "It feels so great to go off med¬
ication,” she says, and even now, when she remembers that feeling,
she seems to get lost in the pure deliciousness of a memory from the
distant past. "When you go off meds it is like taking off a wet wool
18 • ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
coat, which you have been wearing even though it's a beautiful
spring day, and suddenly feeling so much better, freer, nicer.” The
problem was that off the drugs, she would "start to decompensate
and become disorganized.”
In early 1994, she was hospitalized for the fifteenth time. She was
seen as chronically mentally ill, occasionally heard voices now, had
a new diagnosis (schizoaffective), and was on a cocktail of drugs:
Haldol, Ativan, Tegretol, Halcion, and Cogentin, the last drug an
antidote to Haldol's nasty side effects. But after she was released
that spring, a psychiatrist told her to try Risperdal, a new anti¬
psychotic that had just been approved by the FDA. "Three weeks
later, my mind was much clearer," she says. "The voices were going
away. I got off the other meds and took only this one drug. I got
better. I could start to plan. I wasn't talking to the devil anymore.
Jesus and God weren't battling it out in my head.” Her father put it
this way: "Cathy is back."
Although several studies funded by the NIMH and the British
government have found that patients, on the whole, don't do any
better on Risperdal and the other atypicals than on the older anti-
psychotics, Cathy clearly responded very well to this new agent. She
went back to school and earned a degree in radio, film, and televi¬
sion from the University of Maryland. In 1998, she began dating
the man she lives with today, Jonathan. In 2005, she took a part-
time job as editor of Voices for Change, a newsletter published by
M-Power, a consumer group in Massachusetts, a position she held
for three years. In the spring of 2008, she helped lead an M-Power
campaign to get the Massachusetts legislature to pass a law that
would protect the rights of psychiatric patients in emergency rooms.
Still, she remains on SSDI — "I am a kept woman," she jokes —and
although there are many reasons for that, she believes that Risper¬
dal, the very drug that has helped her so much, nevertheless has
proven to be a barrier to full-time work. Although she is usually en¬
ergetic by the early afternoon, Risperdal makes her so sleepy that
she has trouble getting up in the morning. The other problem is that
she has always had trouble getting along with other people, and
Risperdal exacerbates that problem, she says. "The meds isolate
you. They interfere with your empathy. There is a flatness to you,
ANECDOTAL THOUGHTS
19
and so you are uncomfortable with people all the time. They make
it hard for you to get along. The drugs may take care of aggression
and anxiety and some paranoia, those sorts of symptoms, but they
don't help with the empathy that helps you get along with people."
Risperdal has also taken a physical toll. Cathy is five feet, two
inches tall, with curly brown hair, and although she is fairly physi¬
cally fit, she is probably sixty pounds heavier than what would be
considered ideal. She has also developed some of the metabolic
problems, such as high cholesterol, that the atypical antipsychotics
regularly cause. "I can go toe-to-toe with an old lady with a recital
of my physical problems," she says. "My feet, my bladder, my
heart, my sinuses, the weight gain —I have it all.” Even more alarm¬
ing, in 2006 her tongue began rolling over in her mouth, a sign that
she may be developing tardive dyskinesia. When this side effect
appears, it means that the basal ganglia, the part of the brain that
controls motor movement, is becoming permanently dysfunctional,
having been damaged by years of drug treatment. But she can't do
well without Risperdal, and in the summer of 2008, this led to a
moment of deep despair. "I will, of course, look pretty creepy in a
few years, with the involuntary mouth movements," she says.
Such has been her life's course on medications. Sixteen terrible
years, followed by fourteen pretty good years on Risperdal. She be¬
lieves that this drug is essential to her mental health today, and in¬
deed, she could be seen as a local poster child for promoting the
wonders of that drug. Still, if you look at the long-term course of
her illness, and you go all the way back to her first hospitalization at
age eighteen, you have to ask: Is hers a story of a life made better by
our drug-based paradigm of care for mental disorders, or a story of
a life made worse? How might her life have unfolded if when she
suffered her first manic episode in the fall of 1978, she had not
been immediately placed on lithium and Haldol, the doctors instead
trying other means —rest, psychological therapies, etc. —to restore
her sanity? Or if, once she had been stabilized on those medications,
she had been encouraged to wean herself from the drugs? Would
she have spent sixteen years cycling through hospitals? Would she
have gone on SSDI and remained on it ever since? What would her
physical health be like now? What would her subjective experience
2 0
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
of life through those years have been like? And if she had been able
to fare well without drugs, how much more might she have accom¬
plished in her life?
This is a question that Cathy, given her experience with Risper¬
dal, had not thought much about before our interviews. But once I
raised it, she seemed haunted by this possibility, and she brought it
up again and again when we met. "I would have been more produc¬
tive without the meds," she said the first time. "It would break my
heart" to think about that, she said later. Another time she lamented
that with a life on antipsychotics, "you lose your soul and you never
get it back. I got stuck in the system and the struggle to take meds."
Finally, she told me this: "The thing I remember, looking back, is
that I was not really that sick early on. I was really just confused. I
had all these issues, but nobody talked to me about that. I wish I
could go off meds even now, but there is nobody to help me do it.
I can't even start a dialogue.”
There is, of course, no way of knowing what a life without meds
might have been like for Cathy Levin. However, later in this book
we will see what science has to reveal about the possible course her
illness might have taken if, at that fateful moment in 1978, after her
initial psychotic episode, she had not been medicated and told that
she would have to take drugs for life. Science should be able to tell
us whether psychiatrists have reason to believe that their paradigm
of drug-based care alters long-term outcomes for the better or for
the worse. But Cathy believes that this is a question that psychia¬
trists never contemplate.
"They don't have any sense about how these drugs affect you
over the long term. They just try to stabilize you for the moment,
and look to manage you from week to week, month to month.
That's all they ever think about."
George Badillo
Today, George Badillo lives in Sound Beach on Long Island, his
neatly kept home only a short drive away from the water. Nearly
fifty years old, he is physically fit, with slightly graying hair swept
back off his forehead, and he has a quick, warm smile. His
ANECDOTAL THOUGHTS
2 1
thirteen-year-old son, Brandon, lives with him —"He is on the foot¬
ball team, the wrestling team, the baseball team, and the honor
roll,'' George says, with understandable pride —and his twenty-
year-old daughter, Madelyne, who is a student at the College of
Staten Island, is visiting him on this day. Even at first glance, you
can see both are happy to have this time together.
Like many who have been diagnosed with schizophrenia, George
remembers being "different" as a child. As a young boy growing up
in Brooklyn, he felt isolated from the other kids, partly because his
Puerto Rican parents spoke only Spanish. "I remember all the other
kids talking and being so friendly and outgoing, mingling with each
other, and I couldn't do that. I'd want to talk with them, but I was
always apprehensive," he recalls. He also had an alcoholic father
who often beat him, and because of that, he began to think that
"people were always plotting and wanting to hurt me.”
Still, George did okay in school, and it wasn't until his late teens,
when he was a student at Baruch College, that his life began going
awry. "I got into the disco life," he explains. "I started doing am¬
phetamines, marijuana, and cocaine, and I liked it. The drugs re¬
laxed me. Only then it got out of hand and the cocaine started
making me think all crazy. I got real paranoid. I felt there were con¬
spiracies and all that. People were after me, and the government was
in on it." Eventually he ran off to Chicago, where he lived with his
aunt and withdrew from the world that he felt was chasing him.
Alarmed, his family coaxed him back home and took him to the
psychiatric unit at Long Island Jewish Hospital, where he was diag¬
nosed as a paranoid schizophrenic. "They are all telling me that my
brain is broken, and that I will be sick for the rest of my life,” he says.
The next nine years passed in a chaotic whirl. Like Cathy Levin,
George hated Haldol and the other antipsychotics he was told to
take, and partly because of that drug-induced despair, he tried to
kill himself multiple times. He fought with his family about the
medications, went on and off the drugs, cycled through several hos¬
pitalizations, and, in 1987, became a father after his eighteen-year-
old girlfriend gave birth to Madelyne. He married his girlfriend,
intent on being a good father, but Madelyne was a sickly child and
George and his wife both suffered breakdowns trying to care for
2 2
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
her. His grandmother took Madelyne to Puerto Rico, and George
ended up divorced and living in a home for the disabled. There he
met and married a woman also diagnosed with paranoid schizo¬
phrenia, and after a series of adventures and misadventures in San
Francisco, they, too, got divorced. Despondent and paranoid once
again, in early 1991 George landed in Kings Park Psychiatric Cen¬
ter, a run-down state hospital on Long Island.
Now came his descent into total hopelessness. After he tried to
have a pistol smuggled into the hospital so that he could kill him¬
self, he was given a two-year sentence in the locked facility. Then, as
Christmas neared that year, he grew upset when several of his fellow
patients weren't allowed to go home for the holiday, and so he
helped them escape, breaking a window in his room and tying
sheets together so they could clamber to the ground. The hospital
responded by moving him to a ward for people who had been insti¬
tutionalized for decades. "Now I am on a ward with people urinat¬
ing on themselves," he recalls. "Pm a danger to society and drugged
out. You sit down all day and watch television. You can't even go
outside. I thought my life was over."
George spent eight months on that ward for the hopelessly men¬
tally ill, lost in a haze of drugs. However, at last he was moved to a
unit where he could go outside, and suddenly there was blue sky to
be seen and fresh air to breathe. He felt a spark of hope, and then he
took a very risky step: He began tonguing the antipsychotic medica¬
tion and spitting it out when the staff weren't looking. "I could
think again," he says. "The antipsychotic drugs weren't letting me
think. I was like a vegetable, and I couldn't do anything. I had no
emotions. I sat there and watched television. But now I felt more in
control. And it felt great to feel alive again."
Luckily, George didn't experience a return of psychotic symp¬
toms, and with his body no longer slowed by drugs, he began to jog
and lift weights. He fell in love with another patient in the hospital,
Tara McBride, and in 1995, after they were both discharged from
the hospital to a nearby community residence, she gave birth to
Brandon. George, who had never completely lost touch with his
daughter, Madelyne, now had a new goal in life. "I realize I have a
second chance. I want to be a good parent."
ANECDOTAL THOUGHTS
23
At first, it didn't go well. Like Madelyne, Brandon had been born
with health problems —he had an intestinal abnormality that re¬
quired surgery —and Tara broke down from that stress and was
rehospitalized. Since George was still living in a residence for the
mentally ill, the state did not deem him fit to care for Brandon and
he was given to Tara's sister to raise. However, in 1998 George
began working part-time as a peer specialist for the New York State
Office of Mental Health, counseling hospitalized patients about
their rights, and three years later, he was able to present himself in
court as someone who could be a good father to Brandon. "My sis¬
ter Madeline and I got custody," he says. "That was the best feeling.
I was just jumping for joy. It was like the first time that someone in
the system got custody of their kids."
The following year, one of George's sisters bought him the house
he lives in today. Although he still receives SSDI, he does contract
work for the federal Substance Abuse and Mental Services Health
Administration and does volunteer work with hospitalized youth in
Long Island. His is a life filled with meaning, and as Brandon's suc¬
cess in school will attest, he is proving to be the good father he
dreamed of becoming. Madelyne, meanwhile, is unabashedly proud
of him. "He wanted to have Brandon and me in his life," she says.
"That made him want to turn around his situation. He wanted to be
a father to us. He is proof that someone can recover from mental
illness."
Although George's story is clearly an inspiring one, it doesn't
prove anything one way or another about the overall merits of
antipsychotics. But it does prompt a clinical question: Given that his
recovery began when he stopped taking antipsychotics, is it possible
that some people ill with a serious mental disorder, like schizophre¬
nia or bipolar illness, might recover in the absence of medication? Is
his story an anomaly, or does it provide insight into what could be a
fairly common path to recovery? George, who today occasionally
takes Ambien or a low dose of Seroquel to sleep at night, believes
that, at least in his case, getting off the drugs was what enabled him
to get well. "If I had stayed on those drugs, I wouldn't be where I
am today. I would be stuck in an adult home somewhere, or in the
hospital. But I'm recovered. I still have some strange ideas, but now
24
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
I keep them to myself. And I weather whatever emotional stress
comes up. It stays with me for a few weeks and then it goes away."
Monica Briggs
Monica Briggs is a tall, intense woman and, like so many people ac¬
tive in the "peer recovery” movement, immensely likeable. On the
day that I have lunch with her, at a restaurant in South Boston, she
comes hobbling over to the booth leaning on a cane, as she recently
injured herself, and when I ask how she traveled here, she smiles,
slightly pleased with herself. "On my bike," she says.
Monica, who was born in 1967, is from Wellesley, Massachu¬
setts, and as a teenager growing up in that affluent community, she
seemed like the last person who might have a life of mental illness
awaiting her. She came from an accomplished family —her mother
was a professor at Wellesley, while her father taught at several
Boston-area colleges —and Monica was a child who excelled at
whatever she chose to do. She was a good athlete, earned top
grades, and showed a particular talent for art and writing. Upon
graduating from high school, she received several scholarship
awards, and when she entered Middlebury College in Vermont in
the fall of 1985, she believed that her life would follow a very con¬
ventional path. "I thought I'd go to school, marry, have a chocolate
Labrador, and a home in the suburbs, with the SUV. ... I thought it
would all happen like that."
A month into her freshman year at Middlebury, Monica was
blindsided by a severe depressive episode that seemed to have no
cause. She'd never had emotional problems before, nothing bad had
happened at Middlebury, and yet the depression hit her with such
force that she had to leave school and return home. "I was someone
who had never quit anything before," she says. "I thought my life
was over. I thought this was a failure I could never recover from.”
A few months later, she returned to Middlebury. She was taking
an antidepressant (desipramine), and as spring neared, her spirits
began to lift. However, they didn't just rise to a "normal” level. In¬
stead, they soared beyond to what seemed a much better place. She
now had energy to burn. She took long runs and threw herself into
ANECDOTAL THOUGHTS
25
her art, dashing off accomplished self-portraits in charcoals and
pastels. She had so little need for sleep that she started a T-shirt
business. "It was fantastic, great,” she says. "I am not thinking that
I am God, or anything, but I am thinking I am pretty close to God
at that point. This goes on for several weeks, and then I crash for
what seems like forever.”
This was the start of Monica's long battle with bipolar disorder.
Depression had given way to mania followed by worse depression.
Although she managed to complete her freshman year, with an
A-minus average, she began cycling through depressive and manic
episodes, and in May of her sophomore year, she gulped down
handfuls of sleeping spills, intending to kill herself. Over the next
fifteen years, she was hospitalized thirty times. While lithium kept
her mania in check, the suicidal depression always came back, her
doctors prescribing one antidepressant after another in an attempt
to find the magic pill that would help her stay well.
There were times, between the hospitalizations, when she was
fairly stable, and she made the most of them. In 1994, she earned a
bachelor's degree from Massachusetts College of Art and Design,
and after that she worked for various advertising agencies and pub¬
lishing houses. She became active in the Depressive and Manic-
Depressive Association and developed its logo, the "bipolar bear."
But in 2001, after she was fired from her job for having stayed
home for a week due to her depression, her suicidal impulses re¬
turned with a vengeance. She bought a gun, only to have it misfire
six times when she tried to shoot herself. She spent three nights on a
bridge that crossed a highway, desperately wanting to fling herself
onto the roadway below, but refraining from doing so because she
thought she might cause a crash that would hurt others. She was
hospitalized several times, and then, in 2002, her mother died from
pancreatic cancer, and her mental struggles took an even worse
turn. "I am psychotic, hallucinating, seeing things. I think I have
super powers and can change the way time flows. I think I have ten-
feet wings and that I can fly.”
That was the year she went on SSDI. Seventeen years after her
initial manic episode, she had officially become disabled by bipolar
disorder. "I hate it,” she says. "I am a Wellesley girl on welfare, and
26
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
that’s not what Wellesley girls are supposed to do. It is so corrosive
to your self-esteem.”
As might be guessed, given that she arrived at the diner on a
bicycle, having pedaled there during her lunch break at work, Mon¬
ica's life eventually took a turn for the better. In 2006, she stopped
taking an antidepressant, and that triggered a "dramatic change.”
Her depression lifted, and she began working part-time at the
Transformation Center, a Boston peer-run organization that helps
people with psychiatric diagnoses. Although the lithium she has
continued to take has its drawbacks —"my ability to create artwork
is gone,” she says —it hasn't exacted too great a physical toll. While
she has a thyroid problem and suffers from tremors, her kidneys are
fine. "I'm in recovery now," she says, and as we get up to leave the
diner, she makes it clear that she would like to secure a full-time job
and get off SSDI. "Being on welfare is a phase in my life," she says
emphatically, "not an end.”
Such has been the long arc of her illness. As a clinical study, her
story appears to tell simply of the benefits of lithium. That drug ap¬
parently kept her mania in check for decades, and as a monotherapy,
it has helped keep her stable since 2006. Still, after years of drug
treatment, she ended up on SSDI, and as such, her story illustrates
one of the core mysteries of this disability epidemic. How did some¬
one so smart and accomplished end up on that governmental pro¬
gram? And if we wind the clock back to the spring of 1986, a
perplexing question appears: Did she suffer her first manic episode
because she was "bipolar," or did the antidepressant induce the
mania? Is it possible that the drug converted her from someone who
had suffered a depressive episode into a bipolar patient, and thus
put her onto a path of chronic illness? And did the subsequent use
of antidepressants alter the course of her "bipolar illness," for one
reason or another, for the worse?
To put it another way, in the world of people who attend DBSA
meetings, how often do they tell of becoming bipolar after initial
treatment with an antidepressant?
ANECDOTAL THOUGHTS
27
Dorea Vierling-Clausen
If you had met Dorea Vierling-Clausen in 2002, when she was
twenty-five years old, she would have told you that she was "bi¬
polar." She'd been so diagnosed in 1998, her psychiatrist explaining
that she suffered from a chemical imbalance in the brain, and by
2002 she was on a cocktail of drugs that included an antipsychotic,
Zyprexa. But by the fall of 2008, she was off all psychiatric medica¬
tions (and had been for two years), she was thriving in a life that re¬
volved around marriage, motherhood, and postdoctoral research at
Massachusetts General Hospital, and she was convinced that her
"bipolar" years had all been a big mistake. She believes that she was
one of the millions of Americans caught up in a frenzy to diagnose
the disorder, and it very nearly ended with her becoming a mental
patient for life.
"I escaped by the skin of my teeth,” she says.
Dorea tells me her story while sitting in the kitchen of her condo¬
minium in Cambridge, Massachusetts. Her spouse, Angela, is here,
and their two-year-old daughter is sleeping in the next room. With
her freckles and slightly frizzy hair, and evident zest for life, Dorea
seems like someone who might have been a bit of a mischievous
child, and to a certain extent, that is how she remembers herself. "I
was extremely smart, at the far end of that spectrum, and so I was
the geeky kid. But I had friends. I was skillful at social navigation —
I was also the funny kid.” If there was one thing amiss in her life as
a child, it was that she was overly emotional, prone to "angry out¬
bursts” and "crying” jags. "Delightful, but odd” is how she sums
up her seven-year-old self.
Like many bright "odd” kids, Dorea found pursuits she excelled
at. She developed a passion for the trumpet and became an accom¬
plished musician. A top student, she had a particular talent for
mathematics. In high school, she ran on the track team and had
many friends. However, she remained quite emotional —that part of
her personality did not go away —and there was a very real source
of distress in her life: She was coming to understand that she was a
lesbian. Her parents were "extremely conservative Christians," and
while she loved them and deeply admired their devotion to social
2 8
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
justice —her father, a physician, volunteered half of his time to work
in a clinic he'd founded in Denver's tough "Five Points" neighbor¬
hood—she feared that because of their religious beliefs, they
wouldn't accept her homosexuality. After Dorea's freshman year at
Peabody Institute, a prestigious music conservatory in Baltimore,
she took a deep breath and told them her secret. "It went pretty
much as awfully as could be expected," she says. "There were tears,
a gnashing of teeth. It was so desperately ingrained in their religious
thinking.”
Dorea barely spoke to her parents for the next two years. She
dropped out of Peabody and fell in with a punk crowd that lived in
downtown Denver. The once aspiring trumpeter now ran around
town with a shaved head and wearing combat boots. After working
for a year at a shop that restored rugs, she enrolled at Metro State
College, a commuter school. There she struggled constantly with
her emotions, often crying in public, and soon she began seeing a
therapist, who diagnosed her as depressed. Eventually she began to
take an antidepressant, and during finals week in the spring of
1998, she found that she couldn't sleep. When she showed up at her
therapist's office agitated and a little manic, he had a new explan¬
ation for all that bedeviled her: bipolar illness. "I was told it was
chronic and that my episodes would increase in frequency, and that
I would need to be on drugs for the rest of my life," she recalls.
Although this foretold a bleak future, Dorea took comfort in the
diagnosis. It explained why she was so emotional. This also was a
diagnosis common to many great artists. She read Kay Jamison's
book Touched with Fire and thought, "I am just like all these fa¬
mous writers. This is great." She now had a new identity, and as she
resumed her academic career, she arrived at each new institution —
first at the University of Nebraska for an undergraduate degree and
then at Boston University for a Ph.D. in math and biology —with a
"giant box of pills." The cocktail she took usually included a mood
stabilizer, an antidepressant, and a benzodiazepine for anxiety, al¬
though the exact combination was always changing. One drug
would make her sleepy, another would give her tremors, and none
of the cocktails seemed to bring her emotional tranquility. Then, in
ANECDOTAL THOUGHTS
29
2001, she was put on an antipsychotic, Zyprexa, which, in a sense,
worked like a charm.
"You know what?" she says today, amazed by what she is about
to confess. "I loved the stuff. I felt like I finally found the answer.
Because what do you know. I have no emotions. It was great. I
wasn't crying anymore."
Although Dorea did well academically at Boston University, she
still felt "really stupid” on Zyprexa. She slept ten, twelve hours a
day, and like so many people on the drug, she began to blimp up,
putting on thirty pounds. Angela, who had met and fallen in love
with Dorea prior to her going on Zyprexa, felt a sense of loss: "She
wasn't as lively anymore, she didn't laugh," she says. But they both
understood that Dorea needed to be on the medications, and they
began organizing their lives —and their plans for the future —
around her bipolar illness. They attended DBSA meetings, and they
began to think that Dorea should scale back her career goals. She
probably wouldn't be able to handle the stress of postdoctoral re¬
search; her previous work in a rug shop seemed about right. "That
sounds insane now," says Angela, who is a professor of mathe¬
matics at Lesley College. "But at the time, she wasn't a very resilient
person, and she was becoming more and more dependent. I had to
bear the weight of caretaking.''
Dorea's possibilities were diminishing, and she might have con¬
tinued down that path except for the fact that in 2003 she stumbled
across some literature that raised questions about Zyprexa's long¬
term safety and the merits of antipsychotic drugs. That led her to
wean herself from that drug, and while that process was "pure
hell" —she suffered terrible anxiety, severe panic attacks, paranoia,
and horrible tremors —she eventually did get off that medication.
She then decided to see if she could get off the benzodiazepine she
was taking, Klonopin, and that turned into another horrible with¬
drawal experience, as she suffered such severe headaches she'd be in
bed by noon. Still, she was gradually undoing her drug cocktail, and
that caused her to question her bipolar diagnosis. She had first seen
a therapist because she cried too much. There had been no mania —
her sleeplessness and agitation hadn't arisen until after she had been
30
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
placed on an antidepressant. Could she just have been a moody
teenager who had some growing up to do?
"I had always thought before that I was one of those cases where
the illness was clearly biological," she says. "It couldn't have been
situational. Nothing had gone terribly wrong in my life. But then I
thought, well, I came out as a lesbian, and I had no family support.
Duh. That could have been kind of stressful."
The mood stabilizers were the last to go, and on November 22,
2006, Dorea pronounced herself drug free. "It was fabulous. I was
surprised to find out who I was after all these years,” she says,
adding that having shed the bipolar label in her own mind, her sense
of personality responsibility changed, too. "When I was 'bipolar,' I
had an excuse for any unpredictable or unstable behavior. I had per¬
mission to behave in that way, but now I am holding myself to the
same behavioral standards as everyone else, and it turns out I can
meet them. This is not to say that I don't have bad days. I do, and
I may still worry more than the average Joe, but not that much
more.”
Dorea's research at Massachusetts General Hospital focuses on
how vascular activity affects brain function, and given that her
struggles with "mental illness” can seemingly be chalked up as a
case of misdiagnosis —"I have this fantasy of being undiagnosed as
bipolar,” she says —it may seem that her story is irrelevant to this
book. But, in fact, her story raises a possibility that could go a long
way toward explaining the epidemic of disabling mental illness in
the United States. If you expand the boundaries of mental illness,
which is clearly what has happened in this country during the past
twenty-five years, and you treat the people so diagnosed with psy¬
chiatric medications, do you run the risk of turning an angst-ridden
teenager into a lifelong mental patient? Dorea, who is an extremely
smart and capable person, barely escaped going down that path.
Hers is a story of a possible iatrogenic process at work, of an other¬
wise normal person being made chronically sick by diagnosis and
subsequent treatment. And thus we have to wonder: Do we have a
paradigm of care that can, at times, create mental illness?
ANECDOTAL THOUGHTS
• 3 1
The Parents' Dilemma
Early during the course of my reporting for this book, I met with two
families in the Syracuse area who, a few years back, had been faced
with deciding whether to put their children on a psychiatric medica¬
tion. The reason that I had paired these two families up in my mind
was that they had come to opposite conclusions about what was best
for their child, and I was curious to know what information they had
at their disposal when they made their decisions.
I first went to see Gwendolyn and Sean Oates. They live on the
south side of Syracuse, in a pleasant house perched on a slight hill.
A gracious, biracial couple, they have two children, Nathan and
Alia, and as we spoke, Nathan —who was then eight years old —
spent most of the time sprawled out in the living room, drawing
pictures in a sketchbook with colored pencils.
"We began to worry about him when he was three,” his mother
says. "We noticed that he was hyperactive. He couldn't sit through
a meal, he couldn't even sit down. Dinnertime consisted of him run¬
ning around the table. It was the same thing in his preschool —he
couldn't sit still. He wasn't sleeping either. It would take us until
nine thirty or ten p.m. to get him down. He would be kicking and
screaming. These were not normal temper tantrums."
They first took Nathan to his pediatrician. However, she was re¬
luctant to diagnose him, and so they took him to a psychiatrist, who
quickly concluded that Nathan suffered from "attention deficit hy¬
peractivity disorder.” His problem, the psychiatrist explained, was
"chemical" in kind. Although they were nervous about putting
Nathan on Ritalin —"We were going through this on our own,
and we didn't know anything about ADHD," his mother says —
kindergarten was looming, and they reasoned that it would be the
best thing for him. "The hyperactivity was holding him back from
learning,” his mother says. "The school didn't even want us to send
him to kindergarten, but we said, 'No, we are going to.' We made
the decision to keep him moving forward.”
At first, there was a period of "trial and error" with the medica¬
tions. Nathan was put on a high dose of Ritalin, but "he was like a
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
3 2
zombie,” his mother recalls. "He was calm but he didn't move. He
stared off into space.” Nathan was then switched to Concerta, a
long-lasting stimulant, and he stabilized well on it. However, at
some point, Nathan began to exhibit obsessive behaviors, such as
refusing to step on the grass or constantly needing to have some¬
thing in his hands, and he was put on Prozac to control those symp¬
toms. While on that two-drug combo, he started having terrible
"rages.” He kicked out his bedroom window during one episode,
and he repeatedly threatened to kill his sister and even his mother.
He was taken off the Prozac, and although his behavior improved
somewhat, he continued to be quite aggressive, and he was diag¬
nosed as suffering from both bipolar and ADHD.
"They say that ADHD and bipolar go hand in hand," his mother
says. "And now that we know that he is bipolar, too, we think he
will probably be on drugs for the rest of his life.”
Since that time, Nathan has been on a drug cocktail. When I
visited, he was taking Concerta in the morning, Ritalin in the after¬
noon, and three low doses of Risperdal —an antipsychotic —at
various times during the day. This combo, his parents say, works
fairly well for him. While Nathan is still moody, he doesn't fly off
into total rages, and his hostility toward his younger sister has
abated. He does struggle with his schoolwork, but he is moving
ahead from grade to grade, and he gets along fairly well with his
classmates. The biggest worry that his parents have about the med¬
ications is that they may be stunting his growth. Nathan is smaller
than his sister, even though he is three years older. However, the
physician's assistant and others who are treating Nathan don't talk
much about how the drugs may affect him over the long term. "They
don't worry about that," his father says. "It's helping him now.”
At the end of the interview, Nathan shows me his drawings. He is
into sharks and dinosaurs, and after I tell him how much I like his
artwork, he seems almost to blush. He has been quiet most of the
time I have been there, and even a little subdued, but we shake
hands as I get ready to leave, and he seems, at that particular
moment, to be a very sweet and gentle kid.
ANECDOTAL THOUGHTS
33
Jason and Kelley Smith live on the west side of Syracuse, about
thirty minutes distant from the Oates family, and when I knocked
on their door, it was their seven-year-old daughter, Jessica, who an¬
swered. It appeared that she had been waiting for me, and once I
had my tape recorder on, she plunked down on the couch between
her mother and me, ready to pipe in with her side of the story. "Jes¬
sica,” her father says a short while later, "has a lot of charisma.”
Jessica's behavioral problems began at age two when she was
sent to day care. When she got angry, she would hit and bite the
other children. At home, she started having "night terrors" and all-
out meltdowns. "The mildest thing would trigger her and she would
be off," her mother says.
The Smiths turned to their local school district for help. The dis¬
trict recommended that Jessica go to a "special ed” preschool in
north Syracuse, and when she continued to behave aggressively at
that school, they were told to take Jessica to the Health Sciences
Center at the State University of New York for a psychiatric evalua¬
tion. There they saw a nurse practitioner, who immediately con¬
cluded that Jessica was "bipolar.” The practitioner explained that
Jessica had a chemical imbalance and recommended that Jessica be
put on a cocktail of three drugs: Depakote, Risperdal, and lithium.
"It blew my mind, especially the thought of putting her on anti-
psychotics," Jason says. "She was four years old.”
He and his wife left that consultation not knowing what to do.
Kelley works for Oswego County's family service agency, and she
knew of many troubled children who had been put on psychiatric
medications. In that setting, the county expected parents to comply
with medical advice. "There was part of me that thought maybe Jes¬
sica is bipolar, that's what it is," Kelley says. Moreover, SUNY
Health Sciences told the Smiths that the center wouldn't see Jessica
again if she weren't medicated. All of this pointed to following the
center's advice —the "experts are telling you that you need to do
this, and that it is biological," Jason says —but he had previously
worked as a pharmacy technician and knew that drugs could have
powerful side effects. "I was scared out of my mind."
Kelley used the Internet to research the drugs that had been rec¬
ommended. However, she couldn't find any study that told of good
34
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
long-term outcomes for children placed on such drug cocktails, and
even the short-term side effects, she remembers, "were scary."
Meanwhile, Jessica's pediatrician told them she thought it would be
"absurd” to put Jessica on psychiatric drugs; Jason and Kelley's
families also thought it would be a mistake. Jason remembered how
a few years earlier talk therapy had helped him address his own
"anger management" issues, and if he had been able to change
without the use of medications, couldn't Jessica change her behav¬
ior too?
"We just didn't want to accept [the bipolar diagnosis], Jessica is
such an outgoing child, and we like to think she is gifted,” Kelley
says. "And she had made so much progress from the time she was
two years old. We just couldn't see giving her the medications."
They made that decision in 2005, and three years later, they say,
Jessica is doing well. She gets mostly A's in school; her teachers now
think that her earlier bipolar diagnosis was "crazy." While she does
sometimes quarrel with other kids and will lash back verbally if an¬
other child teases her, she knows that she can't hit anyone. At home,
she still has the occasional meltdown, but her emotional outbursts
are not so extreme as before. Jessica even has her own advice on
how all parents should handle such tirades: "They should say [to
their child] 'come here,' and then they should rub them on the back
so they feel better and so they can't have a meltdown, and so when
they stop having a meltdown, that's what they will remember."
Before I leave, Jessica reads to me the book The Little Old Lady
Who Was Not Afraid of Anything, and more than once she jumps
to the floor to act out a scene. "Even with her behavioral issues,
everybody loves her," her father says. "And that's what we were
afraid of, with the medication, was that it would totally change her,
and her personality. We didn't want to impair her faculties. We just
want her to grow up to be healthy and to succeed in life.”
Two different families, two different decisions. Both families now
saw their decision as the right one, and both believed that their child
was on a better path than he or she otherwise would have been.
That was heartening, and I promised to check in with both families
ANECDOTAL THOUGHTS
35
later, toward the end of my reporting for this book. Still, Nathan
and Jessica were clearly on different paths, and as I drove back to
Boston, all I could think about was how both sets of parents had
needed to make their decision, on whether to medicate their child,
in a scientific vacuum. Did their child really suffer from a chemical
imbalance? Were there studies showing that drug treatment for
ADHD or juvenile bipolar illness is beneficial over the long term? If
you put a young child on a drug cocktail that includes an anti¬
psychotic, how will it affect his or her physical health? Can the child
expect to become a healthy teenager, a healthy adult?
Part Two
The Science of
Psychiatric Drugs
3
The Roots of an Epidemic
"Americans have come to believe that science is
capable of almost everything."
-DR. LOUIS M. ORR, AMA PRESIDENT (l958)'
It may seem odd to begin an investigation of a modern-day epi¬
demic with a visit back to one of the great moments in medical his¬
tory, but if we are going to understand how our society came to
believe that Thorazine kicked off a psychopharmacological revolu¬
tion, we need to go back to the laboratory of German scientist Paul
Ehrlich. He was the originator of the notion that "magic bullets"
could be found to fight infectious diseases, and when he succeeded,
society thought that the future would bring miracle cures of every
kind.
Born in East Prussia in 1854, Ehrlich spent his early years as a
scientist researching the use of aniline dyes as biological stains. He
and others discovered that the dyes, which were used in the textile
industry to color cloth, had a selective affinity for staining the cells
of different organs and tissues. Methyl blue would stain one type of
cell, while methyl red stained a different type. In an effort to explain
this specificity, Ehrlich hypothesized that cells had molecules that
protruded into the surrounding environment, and that a chemical
dye fit into these structures, which he called receptors, in the same
way that a key fits into a lock. Every type of cell had a different
lock, and that was why methyl blue stained one type of cell and
methyl red another —they were keys specific to those different locks.
40
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Ehrlich began doing this research in the 1870s, while he was a
doctoral student at the University of Leipzig, and this was the same
period that Robert Koch and Louis Pasteur were proving that mi¬
crobes caused infectious diseases. Their findings led to a thrilling
thought: If the invading organism could be killed, the disease could
be cured. The problem, most scientists at the time concluded, was
that any drug that was toxic to the microbe would surely poison the
host. "Inner disinfection is impossible," declared scientists at an
1882 Congress of Internal Medicine in Germany. But Ehrlich's stud¬
ies with aniline dyes led him to a different conclusion. A dye could
stain a single tissue in the body and leave all others uncolored. What
if he could find a toxic chemical that would interact with the invad¬
ing microbe but not with the patient's tissues? If so, it would kill the
germ without causing any harm to the patient.
Ehrlich wrote:
If we picture an organism as infected by a certain species of
bacterium, it will be easy to effect a cure if substances have
been discovered which have a specific affinity for these bacte¬
ria and act on these alone. (If) they possess no affinity for the
normal constituents of the body, such substances would then
be magic bullets. 2
In 1899, Ehrlich was appointed director of the Royal Institute of
Experimental Therapy in Frankfurt, and there he began his search
for a magic bullet. He focused on finding a drug that would selec¬
tively kill trypanosomes, which were one-celled parasites that
caused sleeping sickness and a number of other illnesses, and he
soon settled on an arsenic compound, atoxyl, as the best magic-
bullet candidate. This would be the chemical he would have to
manipulate so it fit into the parasite's "lock" while not opening the
lock on any human cells. He systematically created hundreds of
atoxyl derivatives, testing them again and again against try¬
panosomes, but time and time again he met with failure. Finally, in
1909, after Ehrlich had tested more than nine hundred compounds,
one of his assistants decided to see if compound number 606 would
THE ROOTS OF AN EPIDEMIC
41
kill another recently discovered microbe, Spirochete! pallida, which
caused syphilis. Within days, Ehrlich had his triumph. The drug,
which came to be known as salvarsan, eradicated the syphilis
microbe from infected rabbits without harming the rabbits at all.
"This was the magic bullet!" wrote Paul de Kruif in a 1926 best¬
seller. "And what a safe bullet!” The drug, he added, produced
"healing that could only be called biblical." 3
Ehrlich's success inspired other scientists to search for magic bul¬
lets against other disease-causing microbes, and although it took
twenty-five years, in 1935 Bayer chemical company provided medi¬
cine with its second miracle drug. Bayer discovered that sulfanil¬
amide, which was a derivative of an old coal-tar compound, was
fairly effective in eradicating staphylococcal and streptococcal in¬
fections. The magic bullet revolution was now truly under way, and
next came penicillin. Although Alexander Fleming had discovered
this bacteria-killing mold in 1928, he and others had found it diffi¬
cult to culture, and even when they'd succeeded in growing it, they
hadn't been able to extract and purify sufficient quantities of the ac¬
tive ingredient (penicillin) to turn it into a useful drug. But in 1941,
with World War II raging, both England and the United States saw
a desperate need to surmount this hurdle, for wound infections had
always been the big killer during war. The United States asked
scientists from Merck, Squibb, and Pfizer to jointly work on this
project, and by D-Day in 1944, British and American sources were
able to produce enough penicillin for all of the wounded in the
Normandy invasion.
"The age of healing miracles had come at last,” wrote Louis
Sutherland, in his book Magic Bullets, and indeed, with the war
over, medicine continued its great leap forward. 4 Pharmaceutical
companies discovered other broad-acting antibiotics —streptomycin,
Chloromycetin, and Aureomycin, to name a few —and suddenly
physicians had pills that could cure pneumonia, scarlet fever, diph¬
theria, tuberculosis, and a long list of other infectious diseases.
These illnesses had been the scourge of mankind for centuries, and
political leaders and physicians alike spoke of the great day at hand.
In 1948, U.S. secretary of state George Marshall confidently pre-
42
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
dieted that infectious diseases might soon be wiped from the face of
the earth. A few years later, President Dwight D. Eisenhower called
for the "unconditional surrender" of all microbes. 5
As the 1950s began, medicine could look back and count numer¬
ous other successes as well. Pharmaceutical firms had developed im¬
proved anesthetics, sedatives, antihistamines, and anticonvulsants,
evidence of how scientists were getting better at synthesizing chem¬
icals that acted on the central nervous system in helpful ways. In
1922, Eli Lilly had figured out how to extract the hormone insulin
from the pancreas glands of slaughterhouse animals, and this pro¬
vided doctors with an effective treatment for diabetes. Although re¬
placement insulin didn't rise to the level of a magic-bullet cure for
the illness, it came close, for it provided a biological fix for what
was missing in the body. In 1950, British scientist Sir Henry Dale,
in a letter to the British Medical Journal, summed up this extra¬
ordinary moment in medicine's long history: "We who have been able
to watch the beginning of this great movement may be glad and
proud to have lived through such a time, and confident that an even
wider and more majestic advance will be seen by those living
through the fifty years now opening." 6
The United States geared up for this wondrous future. Prior to
the war, most basic research had been privately funded, with An¬
drew Carnegie and John D. Rockefeller the most prominent bene¬
factors, but once the war ended, the U.S. government established
the National Science Foundation to federally fund this endeavor.
There were still many diseases to conquer, and as the nation's lead¬
ers looked around for a medical field that had lagged behind, they
quickly found one that seemed to stand above all the rest. Psychia¬
try, it seemed, was a discipline that could use a little help.
Imagining a New Psychiatry
As a medical specialty, psychiatry had its roots in the nineteenth-
century asylum, its founding moment occurring in 1844, when
thirteen physicians who ran small asylums met in Philadelphia to
THE ROOTS OF AN EPIDEMIC
43
form the Association of Medical Superintendents of American Insti¬
tutions for the Insane. At that time, the asylums provided a form of
environmental care known as moral therapy, which had been intro¬
duced into the United States by Quakers, and for a period, it pro¬
duced good results. At most asylums, more than 50 percent of
newly admitted patients would be discharged within a year, and
a significant percentage of those who left never came back. A
nineteenth-century long-term study of outcomes at Worcester State
Lunatic Asylum in Massachusetts found that 58 percent of the 984
patients discharged from the asylum remained well throughout the
rest of their lives. However, the asylums mushroomed in size in the
latter part of the 1800s, as communities dumped the senile elderly
and patients with syphilis and other neurological disorders into the
institutions, and since these patients had no chance of recovering,
moral therapy came to be seen as a failed form of care.
At their 1892 meeting, the asylum superintendents vowed to
leave moral therapy behind and instead utilize physical treatments.
This was the dawn of a new era in psychiatry, and in very short
order, they began announcing the benefits of numerous treatments
of this kind. Various water therapies, including high-pressure show¬
ers and prolonged baths, were said to be helpful. An injection of ex¬
tract of sheep thyroid was reported to produce a 50 percent cure
rate at one asylum; other physicians announced that injections of
metallic salts, horse serum, and even arsenic could restore lucidity
to a mad mind. Henry Cotton, superintendent at Trenton State
Hospital in New Jersey, reported in 1916 that he cured insanity by
removing his patients' teeth. Fever therapies were said to be benefi¬
cial, as were deep-sleep treatments, but while the initial reports of
all these somatic therapies told of great success, none of them stood
the test of time.
In the late 1930s and early 1940s, asylum psychiatrists embraced
a trio of therapies that acted directly on the brain, which the popu¬
lar media —at least initially— reported as "miracle” cures. First
came insulin coma therapy. Patients were injected with a high dose
of insulin, which caused them to lapse into hypoglycemic comas,
and when they were brought back to life with an injection of glu¬
cose, the New York Times explained, the "short circuits of the brain
44
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
vanish, and the normal circuits are once more restored and bring
back with them sanity and reality." 7 Next came the convulsive ther¬
apies. Either a poison known as Metrazol or electroshock was used
to induce a seizure in the patient, and when the patient awoke, he or
she would be free of psychotic thoughts and happier in spirit —or so
the asylum psychiatrists said. The final "breakthrough" treatment
was frontal lobotomy, the surgical destruction of the frontal lobes
apparently producing an instant cure. This "surgery of the soul,”
the New York Times explained, "transforms wild animals into
gentle creatures in the course of a few hours." 8
With such articles regularly appearing in major newspapers and
magazines like Harper's, Reader's Digest, and the Saturday Evening
Post, the public had reason to believe that psychiatry was making
great strides in treating mental illness, participating in medicine's
great leap forward, but then, in the wake of World War II, the pub¬
lic was forced to confront a very different reality, one that produced
a great sense of horror and disbelief. There were 425,000 people
locked up in the country's mental hospitals at that time, and first
Life magazine and then journalist Albert Deutsch, in his book The
Shame of the States, took Americans on a photographic tour of the
decrepit facilities. Naked men huddled in barren rooms, wallowing
in their own feces. Barefoot women clad in coarse tunics sat
strapped to wooden benches. Patients slept on threadbare cots in
sleeping wards so crowded that they had to climb over the foot
of their beds to get out. These images told of unimaginable neglect
and great suffering, and at last, Deutsch drew the inevitable com¬
parison:
As I passed through some of Byberry's wards, I was reminded
of the Nazi concentration camps at Belsen and Buchenwald. I
entered buildings swarming with naked humans herded like
cattle and treated with less concern, pervaded by a fetid odor
so heavy, so nauseating, that the stench seemed to have al¬
most a physical existence of its own. I saw hundreds of pa¬
tients living under leaking roofs, surrounded by moldy,
decaying walls, and sprawling on rotting floors for want of
seats or benches.’
THE ROOTS OF AN EPIDEMIC
45
The nation clearly needed to remake its care of the hospitalized
mentally ill, and even as it contemplated that need, it found reason
to worry about the mental health of the general population. During
the war, psychiatrists had been charged with screening draftees for
psychiatric problems, and they had deemed 1.75 million American
men mentally unfit for service. While many of the rejected draftees
may have been feigning illness in order to avoid conscription, the
numbers still told of a societal problem. Many veterans returning
from Europe were also struggling emotionally, and in September
1945, General Lewis Hershey, who was the director of the Selective
Service System, told Congress that the nation badly needed to ad¬
dress this problem, which had remained hidden for so long. "Men¬
tal illness was the greatest cause of noneffectiveness and loss of
manpower that we met” during the war, he said. 10
With mental illness now a primary concern for the nation —and
this awareness coming at the very time that antibiotics were taming
bacterial killers—it was easy for everyone to see where a long-term
solution might be found. The country could put its faith in the
transformative powers of science. The existing "medical" treatments
said to be so helpful—insulin coma, electroshock, and lobotomy —
would have to be provided to more patients, and then long-term so¬
lutions could arise from the same process that had produced such
astonishing progress in fighting infectious diseases. Research into
the biological causes of mental illnesses would lead to better treat¬
ments, both for those who were seriously ill and those who were
only moderately distressed. "I can envisage a time arriving when we
in the field of Psychiatry will entirely forsake our ancestry, forget¬
ting that we had our beginnings in the poorhouse, the workhouse
and the jail," said Charles Burlingame, director of the Institute of
the Living in Hartford, Connecticut. "I can envisage a time when
we will be doctors, think as doctors, and run our psychiatric institu¬
tions in much the same way and with much the same relationships
as obtain in the best medical and surgical institutions." 11
In 1946, Congress passed a National Mental Health Act that put
the federal government's economic might behind such reform. The
government would sponsor research into the prevention, diagnosis,
and treatment of mental disorders, and it would provide grants to
46
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
states and cities to help them establish clinics and treatment centers.
Three years later, Congress created the National Institute of Mental
Health (NIMH) to oversee this reform.
"We must realize that mental problems are just as real as physical
disease, and that anxiety and depression require active therapy as
much as appendicitis or pneumonia," wrote Dr. Howard Rusk, a
professor at New York University who penned a weekly column for
the New York Times. "They are all medical problems requiring
medical care." 12
The stage had now been set for a transformation of psychiatry and
its therapeutics. The public believed in the wonders of science, the
nation saw a pressing need to improve its care of the mentally ill, and
the NIMH had been created to make this happen. There was the
expectation of great things to come and, thanks to the sales of anti¬
biotics, a rapidly growing pharmaceutical industry ready to capital¬
ize on that expectation. And with all those forces lined up, perhaps
it is no surprise that wonder drugs for both severe and not-so-severe
mental illnesses —for schizophrenia, depression, and anxiety —soon
arrived.
4
Psychiatry’s Magic Bullets
"It was the first drug cure in all of
psychiatric history."
- NATHAN KLINE
DIRECTOR OF RESEARCH AT ROCKLAND
HOSPITAL IN NEW YORK (l974)‘
STATE
The "magic bullet” model of medicine that had led to the discovery
of the sulfa drugs and antibiotics was very simple in kind. First,
identify the cause or nature of the disorder. Second, develop a
treatment to counteract it. Antibiotics killed known bacterial in¬
vaders. Eli Lilly's insulin therapy was a variation on the same
theme. The company developed this treatment after researchers
came to understand that diabetes was due to an insulin deficiency.
In each instance, knowledge of the disease came first —that was the
magic formula for progress. However, if we look at how the first
generation of psychiatric drugs was discovered, and look too at
how they came to be called antipsychotics, anti-anxiety agents, and
antidepressants— words that indicate they were antidotes to specific
disorders —we see a very different process at work. The psychophar¬
macology revolution was born from one part science and two parts
wishful thinking.
48
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Neuroleptics, Minor Tranquilizers,
and Psychic Energizers
The story of the discovery of Thorazine, the drug that is remem¬
bered today as having kicked off the psychopharmacology "revolu¬
tion," begins in the 1940s, when researchers at Rhone-Poulenc, a
French pharmaceutical company, tested a class of compounds
known as phenothiazines for their magic-bullet properties. Phe-
nothiazines had first been synthesized in 1883 for use as chemical
dyes, and Rhone-Poulenc's scientists were trying to synthesize phe¬
nothiazines that were toxic to the microbes that caused malaria,
African sleeping sickness, and worm-borne illnesses. Although that
research didn't pan out, they did discover in 1946 that one of their
phenothiazines, promethazine, had antihistaminic properties, which
suggested it might have use in surgery. The body releases histamine
in response to wounds, allergies, and a range of other conditions,
and if this histaminic response is too strong, it can lead to a precipi¬
tous drop in blood pressure, which at the time occasionally proved
fatal to surgical patients. In 1949, a thirty-five-year-old surgeon in
the French Navy, Henri Laborit, gave promethazine to several of his
patients at the Maritime Hospital at Bizerte in Tunisia, and he dis¬
covered that in addition to its antihistaminic properties, it induced a
"euphoric quietude. . . . Patients are calm and somnolent, with a re¬
laxed and detached expression." 2
Promethazine, it seemed, might have use as an anesthetic. At that
time, barbiturates and morphine were regularly employed in medi¬
cine as general sedatives and painkillers, but those drugs suppressed
overall brain function, which made them quite dangerous. But
promethazine apparently acted only on selective regions of the
brain. The drug "made it possible to disconnect certain brain func¬
tions," Laborit explained. "The surgical patient felt no pain, no
anxiety, and often did not remember his operation." 3 If the drug
was used as part of a surgical cocktail, Laborit reasoned, it would
be possible to use much lower doses of the more dangerous anes¬
thetic agents. A cocktail that included promethazine —or an even
PSYCHIATRY'S MAGIC BULLETS • 49
more potent derivative of it, if such a compound could be synthe¬
sized—would make surgery much safer.
Chemists at Rhone-Poulenc immediately went to work. To assess
a compound, they would give it to caged rats that had learned, upon
hearing the sound of a bell, to climb a rope to a resting platform in
order to avoid being shocked (the floor of the cage was electrified).
They knew they had found a successor to promethazine when they
injected compound 4560 RP into the rats: Not only were the rats
physically unable to climb the rope, they weren't emotionally inter¬
ested in doing so either. This new drug, chlorpromazine, apparently
disconnected brain regions that controlled both motor movement
and the mounting of emotional responses, and yet it did so without
causing the rats to lose consciousness.
Laborit tested chlorpromazine as part of a drug cocktail in surgi¬
cal patients in June of 1951. As expected, it put them into a "twi¬
light state.” Other surgeons tested it as well, reporting that it served
to "potentiate” the effects of the other anesthetic agents, the cock¬
tail inducing an "artificial hibernation." In December of that year,
Laborit spoke of this new advance in surgery at an anesthesiology
conference in Brussels, and there he made an observation that
suggested chlorpromazine might also be of use in psychiatry. It
"produced a veritable medicinal lobotomy," he said.*
Although today we think of lobotomy as a mutilating surgery, at
that time it was regarded as a useful operation. Only two years
earlier, the Nobel Prize in Medicine had been awarded to the Por¬
tuguese neurologist, Egas Moniz, who had invented it. The press, in
its most breathless moments, had even touted lobotomy as an oper¬
ation that plucked madness neatly from the mind. But what the
surgery most reliably did, and this was well understood by those
who performed the operation, was change people in a profound
way. It made them lethargic, disinterested, and childlike. That was
seen by the promoters of lobotomy as an improvement over what
the patients had been before —anxious, agitated, and filled with psy¬
chotic thoughts —and now, if Laborit was to be believed, a pill had
been discovered that could transform patients in a similar way.
In the spring of 1952, two prominent French psychiatrists, Jean
50 •
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Delay and Pierre Deniker, began administering chlorpromazine to
psychotic patients at St. Anne's Hospital in Paris, and soon use of
the drug spread to asylums throughout Europe. Everywhere the re¬
ports were the same: Hospital wards were quieter, the patients
easier to manage. Delay and Deniker, in a series of articles they
published in 1952, described the "psychic syndrome” induced by
chlorpromazine:
Seated or lying down, the patient is motionless on his bed,
often pale and with lowered eyelids. He remains silent most
of the time. If questioned, he responds after a delay, slowly, in
an indifferent monotone, expressing himself with few words
and quickly becoming mute. Without exception, the response
is generally valid and pertinent, showing that the subject is
capable of attention and of reflection. But he rarely takes the
initiative of asking a question; he does not express his preoc¬
cupations, desires, or preference. He is usually conscious of
the amelioration brought on by the treatment, but he does not
express euphoria. The apparent indifference or the delay of
the response to external stimuli, the emotional and affective
neutrality, the decrease in both initiative and preoccupation
without alteration in conscious awareness or in intellectual
faculties constitute the psychic syndrome due to the treat¬
ment. 5
U.S. psychiatrists dubbed chlorpromazine, which was marketed
in the United States as Thorazine, as a "major tranquilizer.” Back in
France, Delay and Deniker coined a more precise scientific term:
This new drug was a "neuroleptic,” meaning it took hold of the ner¬
vous system. Chlorpromazine, they concluded, induced deficits sim¬
ilar to those seen in patients ill with encephalitis lethargica. "In
fact," Deniker wrote, "it would be possible to cause true encephali¬
tis epidemics with the new drugs. Symptoms progressed from re¬
versible somnolence to all types of dyskinesia and hyperkinesia, and
finally to parkinsonism." 6 Physicians in the United States similarly
understood that this new drug was not fixing any known pathology.
"We have to remember that we are not treating diseases with this
PSYCHIATRY'S MAGIC BULLETS
51
drug," said psychiatrist E. H. Parsons, at a 1955 meeting in
Philadelphia on chlorpromazine. "We are using a neuropharma-
cologic agent to produce a specific effect." 7
At the same time that Rhone-Poulenc was testing phenothiazines
for their possible magic-bullet properties against malaria, Frank
Berger, a Czech-born chemist, was doing research of a somewhat
similar kind in London, and his work led, in 1955, to the introduc¬
tion of "minor tranquilizers” to the market.
During the war, Berger had been one of the scientists in Britain
who had helped develop methods to produce medically useful quan¬
tities of penicillin. But penicillin was effective only against gram¬
positive bacteria (microbes that took up a stain developed by
Danish scientist Hans Christian Gram), and after the war ended,
Berger sought to find a magic bullet that could kill gram-negative
microbes, the ones that caused a host of troubling respiratory, uri¬
nary, and gastrointestinal illnesses. At that time, there was a com¬
mercial disinfectant sold in Britain, called Phenoxetol, that was
advertised as effective against gram-negative bacteria in the envi¬
ronment, and Berger, who worked for British Drug Houses, Ltd.,
tinkered with the active ingredient in that product, a phenylglycerol
ether, in an effort to produce a product with superior antibacterial
effects. When a compound called mephenesin proved promising, he
gave it to mice to test its toxicity. "The compound, much to my sur¬
prise, produced reversible flaccid paralysis of the voluntary skeletal
muscles unlike that I had ever seen before,” Berger wrote. 8
Berger had stumbled on a potent muscle-relaxing agent. That
was curious enough, but what was even more surprising, the drug-
paralyzed mice didn't show any signs of being stressed by their new
predicament. He would put the animals on their backs and they
would be unable to right themselves, and yet their "heart beat was
regular, and there were no signs suggesting an involvement of the
autonomic nervous system.” The mice remained quiet and tranquil,
and Berger found that even when he administered low doses of this
amazing new compound to mice —the doses too small to cause mus¬
cle paralysis —they displayed this odd tranquility.
52
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Berger realized that a drug of this sort might have commercial
possibilities as an agent that allayed anxiety in people. However,
mephenesin was a very short-acting drug, providing only a few min¬
utes of peace. In 1947, Berger moved to the United States and went
to work for Wallace Laboratories in New Jersey, where he synthe¬
sized a compound, meprobamate, that lasted eight times as long in
the body as mephenesin. When Berger gave it to animals, he discov¬
ered that it also had powerful "taming” effects. "Monkeys after
being given meprobamate lost their viciousness and could be more
easily handled," he wrote. 9
Wallace Laboratories brought meprobamate to market in 1955,
selling it as Miltown. Other pharmaceutical companies scrambled
to develop competitor drugs, and as they did so, they looked for
compounds that would make animals less aggressive and numb to
pain. At Hoffmann-La Roche, chemist Leo Sternbach identified
chlordiazepoxide as having a "powerful and unique" tranquilizing
effect after he gave it to mice that ordinarily could be prompted to
fight by the application of electric shocks to their feet. 1 " Even with a
low dose of the drug, the mice remained noncombative when
shocked. This compound also proved to have potent taming effects
in larger animals —it turned tigers and lions into pussycats. The
final proof of chlordiazepoxide's merits involved another electric-
shock exam. Hungry rats were trained to press a lever for food, and
then they were taught that if they did so while a light in the cage
blinked on, they would be shocked. Although the rats quickly
learned not to press the lever while the light was on, they neverthe¬
less exhibited signs of extreme stress —defecating, etc.— whenever it
lit up their cage. But if they were given a dose of chlordiazepoxide?
The light would flash and they wouldn't be the least bit bothered.
Their "anxiety” had vanished, and they would even press the lever
to get something to eat, unworried about the shock to come.
Hoffmann-La Roche brought chlordiazepoxide to market in 1960,
selling it as Librium.
For obvious reasons, the public heard little about the animal tests
that had given rise to the minor tranquilizers. However, an article
published in the Science News Letter was the exception to the rule,
as its reporter put the animal experiments into a human frame of
PSYCHIATRY'S MAGIC BULLETS
53
reference. If you took a minor tranquilizer, he explained, "this
would mean that you might still feel scared when you see a car
speeding toward you, but the fear would not make you run." 11
Psychiatry now had a new drug for quieting hospitalized patients and
a second one for easing anxiety, the latter a drug that could be mar¬
keted to the general population, and by the spring of 1957, it gained
a medicine for depressed patients, iproniazid, which was marketed as
Marsilid. This drug, which was dubbed a "psychic energizer," could
trace its roots back to a poetically apt source: rocket fuel.
Toward the end of World War II, when Germany ran low on the
liquid oxygen and ethanol it used to propel its V-2 rockets, its scien¬
tists developed a novel compound, hydrazine, to serve as a substitute
fuel. After the war ended, chemical companies from the Allied coun¬
tries swooped in to grab samples of it, their pharmaceutical divi¬
sions eager to see if its toxic properties could be harnessed for
magic-bullet purposes. In 1951, chemists at Hoffmann-La Roche
created two hydrazine compounds, isoniazid and iproniazid, that
proved effective against the bacillus that caused tuberculosis. The
novel medicines were rushed into use in several TB hospitals, and
soon there were reports that the drug seemed to "energize” patients.
At Staten Island's Sea View Hospital, Time magazine reported, "pa¬
tients who had taken the drugs danced in the wards, to the delight
of news photographers." 12
The sight of TB patients doing a jig suggested that these drugs
might have a use in psychiatry as a treatment for depression. For
various reasons, iproniazid was seen as having the greater potential,
but initial tests did not find it to be particularly effective in lifting
spirits, and there were reports that it could provoke mania. Tuber¬
culosis patients treated with iproniazid were also developing so
many nasty side effects —dizziness, constipation, difficulty urinat¬
ing, neuritis, perverse skin sensations, confusion, and psychosis —
that its use had to be curtailed in sanitariums. However, in the
spring of 1957, Nathan Kline, a psychiatrist at Rockland State Hos¬
pital in Orangeburg, New York, rescued iproniazid with a report
that if depressed patients were kept on the drug long enough, for at
54
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
least five weeks, it worked. Fourteen of the sixteen patients he'd
treated with iproniazid had improved, and some had a "complete
remission of all symptoms." 13
On April 7, 1957, the New York Times summed up iproniazid's
strange journey: "A side effect of an anti-tuberculosis drug may
have led the way to chemical therapy for the unreachable, severely
depressed mental patient. Its developers call it an energizer as
opposed to a tranquilizer." 14
Such were the drugs that launched the psychopharmacology revolu¬
tion. In the short span of three years (1954-1957), psychiatry
gained new medicines for quieting agitated and manic patients in
asylums, for anxiety, and for depression. But none of these drugs
had been developed after scientists had identified any disease
process or brain abnormality that might have been causing these
symptoms. They arrived out of the post-World War II search for
magic bullets against infectious diseases, with researchers, during
that process, stumbling on compounds that affected the central ner¬
vous system in novel ways. The animal tests of chlorpromazine,
meprobamate, and chlordiazepoxide revealed that these agents
sharply curbed normal physical and emotional responses, but did so
without causing a loss of consciousness. That was what was so
novel about the major and minor tranquilizers. They curbed brain
function in a selective manner. It was unclear how iproniazid
worked —it seemed to rev up the brain in some way —but, as the
New York Times had noted, its mood-lifting properties were prop¬
erly seen as a "side effect" of an anti-tuberculosis agent.
The drugs were best described as "tonics.” But in the media, a
story of a much different sort was being told.
An Unholy Alliance
The storytelling forces in American medicine underwent a profound
shift in the 1950s, and to see how that is so, we need to briefly
PSYCHIATRY'S MAGIC BULLETS
• 55
recount the history of the American Medical Association prior to
that time. At the turn of the century, the AMA set itself up as the or¬
ganization that would help the American public distinguish the
good from the bad. At that time, there were fifty thousand or so
medicinal products sold in the United States, and they were of two
basic types. There were thousands of small companies that sold
syrups, elixirs, and herbal remedies directly to the public (or as
packaged goods in stores), with these "patent" medicines typically
made from "secret" ingredients. Meanwhile, Merck and other
"drug houses" sold their chemical preparations, which were known
as "ethical" drugs, to pharmacists, who then acted as the retail ven¬
dors of these products. Neither group needed to prove to a govern¬
ment regulatory agency that its products were safe or effective, and
the AMA, eager to establish a place for doctors in this freewheeling
marketplace, set itself up as the organization that would do this as¬
sessment. It established a "propaganda department" to investigate
the patent medicines and thus protect Americans from "quackery,"
and it established a Council on Pharmacy and Chemistry to conduct
chemical tests of the ethical drugs. The AMA published the results
of these tests in its journals and provided the best ethical drugs with
its "seal of approval." The AMA also published each year a "useful
drugs” book, and its medical journals would not allow advertise¬
ments for any drug that had not passed its vetting process.
With this work, the AMA turned itself into a watchdog of the
pharmaceutical industry and its products. By doing so, the organi¬
zation was both providing a valuable service to the public and fur¬
thering its members' financial interests, for its drug evaluations
provided patients with a good reason to visit a doctor. A physician,
armed with his book of useful drugs, could prescribe an appropriate
one. And it was this knowledge, as opposed to any government-
authorized prescribing power, that provided physicians with their
value in the marketplace (in terms of providing access to medicines).
The selling of drugs in the United States began to change with the
passage of the 1938 Food and Drug Cosmetics Act. The law re¬
quired drug firms to prove to the Food and Drug Administration
that their products were safe (they still did not have to prove that
their drugs were helpful), and in its wake, the FDA began decreeing
5 6 •
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
that certain medicines could be purchased only with a doctor's
prescription.* In 1951, Congress passed the Durham-Humphrey
Amendment to the act, which decreed that most new drugs would
be available by prescription only, and that prescriptions would be
needed for refills, too.
Physicians now enjoyed a very privileged place in American soci¬
ety. They controlled the public's access to antibiotics and other new
medicines. In essence, they had become the retail vendors of these
products, with pharmacists simply fulfilling their orders, and as
vendors, they now had financial reason to tout the wonders of their
products. The better the new drugs were perceived to be, the more
inclined the public would be to come to their offices to obtain a pre¬
scription. "It would appear that a physician's own market position
is strongly influenced by his reputation for using the latest drug,”
explained Fortune magazine. 15
The financial interests of the drug industry and physicians were
lined up in a way they never had been before, and the AMA
quickly adapted to this new reality. In 1952, it stopped publishing
its yearly book on "useful drugs." Next, it began allowing adver¬
tisements in its journals for drugs that had not been approved by
its Council on Pharmacy and Chemistry. In 1955, the AMA aban¬
doned its famed "seal of acceptance” program. By 1957, it had cut
the budget for its Council on Drugs to a paltry $75,000, which
was understandable, given that the AMA was no longer in the
business of assessing the merits of these products. Three years later,
the AMA even lobbied against a proposal by Tennessee senator
Estes Kefauver that drug companies prove to the FDA that their
new drugs were effective. The AMA, in its relationship to the phar¬
maceutical industry, had "become what I would call sissy,” con¬
fessed Harvard Medical School professor Maxwell Finland, in
testimony to Congress. 16
But it wasn't just that the AMA had given up its watchdog role.
The AMA and physicians were also now working with the
* In 1914, the Harrison Narcotics Act required a doctor's prescription for opi¬
ates and cocaine. The 1938 Food and Drug Cosmetics Act extended that
prescription-only requirement to a larger number of drugs.
PSYCHIATRY'S MAGIC BULLETS
57
pharmaceutical industry to promote new drugs. In 1951, the year
that the Durham-Humphrey Act was passed, Smith Kline and
French and the American Medical Association, began jointly pro¬
ducing a television program called The March of Medicine, which,
among other things, helped introduce Americans to the "wonder”
drugs that were coming to market. Newspaper and magazine arti¬
cles about new medications inevitably included testimonials from
doctors touting their benefits, and as Pfizer physician Haskell Wein¬
stein later confessed to a congressional committee, "much of what
appears [in the popular press] has in essence been placed by the
public relations staffs of the pharmaceutical firms." 17 In 1952, an
industry trade publication, FDC Reports, noted that the pharma¬
ceutical industry was enjoying a "sensationally favorable press,”
and a few years later, it commented on why this was so. "Virtually
all important drugs," it wrote, receive "lavish praise by the medical
profession on introduction." 18
This new marketplace for drugs proved profitable for all in¬
volved. Drug industry revenues topped $1 billion in 1957, the phar¬
maceutical companies enjoying earnings that made them "the
darlings of Wall Street," one writer observed. 18 Now that physicians
controlled access to antibiotics and all other prescription drugs,
their incomes began to climb rapidly, doubling from 1950 to 1970
(after adjusting for inflation). The AMA's revenues from drug ad¬
vertisements in its journals rose from $2.5 million in 1950 to $10
million in 1960, and not surprisingly, these advertisements painted
a rosy picture. A 1959 review of drugs in six major medical journals
found that 89 percent of the ads provided no information about the
drugs' side effects. 20
Such was the environment in the 1950s when the first psychiatric
drugs were brought to market. The public was eager to hear of
wonder drugs, and this was just the story that the pharmaceutical
industry and the nation's physicians were eager to tell.
58
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Miracle Pills
Smith Kline and French, which obtained a license from Rhone-
Poulenc to sell chlorpromazine in the United States, secured FDA
approval for Thorazine on March 26, 1954. A few days later, the
company used its March of Medicine show to launch the product.
Although Smith Kline and French had spent only $350,000 devel¬
oping Thorazine, having administered it to fewer than 150 psychi¬
atric patients prior to submitting its application to the FDA, the
company's president, Francis Boyer, told viewers that this was a
product that had gone through the most rigorous testing imagin¬
able. "It was administered to well over five thousand animals and
proved active and safe for human administration," he said. "We
then placed the compound in the hands of physicians in our great
American medical centers to explore its clinical value and possible
limitations. In all, over two thousand doctors in this country and
Canada have used it. . . . The development of a new medicine is dif¬
ficult and costly, but it is a job our industry is privileged to per¬
form." 21
Boyer's was a story of rigorous science at work, and less than
three months later, Time, in an article titled "Wonder Drug of
1954?", pronounced Thorazine a "star performer." After a dose of
Thorazine, the magazine explained, patients "sit up and talk sense
with [the doctor], perhaps for the first time in months." 22 In a
follow-up article, Time reported that patients "willingly took [the]
pills” and that once they did, they "fed themselves, ate heartily and
slept well.” Thorazine, the magazine concluded, was as important
"as the germ-killing sulfas discovered in the 1930s." 23
This was a magic-bullet reference that was impossible to miss,
and other newspapers and magazines echoed that theme. Thanks to
chlorpromazine, U.S. News and World Report explained, "patients
who were formerly untreatable within a matter of weeks or months
become sane, rational human beings." 24 The New York Times, in a
series of articles in 1954 and 1955, called Thorazine a "miracle” pill
that brought psychiatric patients "peace of mind” and "freedom
PSYCHIATRY’S MAGIC BULLETS
5 9
from confusion.” Thorazine, newspapers and magazines agreed,
had ushered in a "new era of psychiatry." 25
With such stories being told about Thorazine, it was little won¬
der that the public went gaga when Miltown, in the spring of 1955,
was introduced into the market. This drug, Time reported, was for
"walk-in neurotics rather than locked-in psychotics," and accord¬
ing to what psychiatrists were telling newspaper and magazine re¬
porters, it had amazing properties. 26 Anxiety and worries fled so
quickly, Changing Times explained, that it could be considered a
"happy pill." Reader's Digest likened it to a "Turkish bath in a
tablet.” The drug, explained Consumer Reports, "does not deaden
or dull the senses, and it is not habit forming. It relaxes the muscles,
calms the mind, and gives people a renewed ability to enjoy life." 22
The public rush to obtain this new drug was such that Wallace
Laboratories and Carter Products, which were jointly selling
meprobamate, struggled to keep up with the demand. Drugstores
lucky enough to have a supply put out signs that screamed: YES, WE
have miltown! The comedian Milton Berle said that he liked the
drug so much that he might change his first name to Miltown. Wal¬
lace Laboratories hired Salvador Dali to help stoke Miltown fever,
paying the great artist $35,000 to create an exhibit at an AMA
convention that was meant to capture the magic of this new drug.
Attendees walked into a darkened claustrophobic tunnel that repre¬
sented the interior of a caterpillar —this was what it was like to be
anxious —and then, as they emerged back into the light, they came
upon a golden "butterfly of tranquility," this metamorphosis due to
meprobamate. "To Nirvana with Miltown" is how Time described
Dali's exhibit. 28
There was one slightly hesitant note that appeared in newspaper
and magazine articles during the introduction of Thorazine and
Miltown. In the 1950s, many of the psychiatrists at top American
medical schools were Freudians, who believed that mental disorders
were caused by psychological conflicts, and their influence led Smith
Kline and French, in its initial promotion of Thorazine, to caution
reporters that "there is no thought that chlorpromazine is a cure for
mental illness, but it can have great value if it relaxes patients and
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
6 o •
makes them accessible to treatment." 29 Both Thorazine and
Miltown, explained the New York Titties, should be considered as
"adjuncts to psychotherapy, not the cure." 30 Thorazine was called a
"major tranquilizer" and Miltown a "minor tranquilizer,” and
when Hoffmann-La Roche brought iproniazid to market, it was de¬
scribed as a "psychic energizer." These drugs, although they may
have been remarkable in kind, were not antibiotics for the mind. As
Life magazine noted in a 1956 article titled "The Search Has Only
Started,” psychiatry was still in the early stages of its revolution, for
the "bacteria” of mental disorders had yet to be discovered. 31
Yet, in very short order, even this note of caution went by the
wayside. In 1957, the New York Times reported that researchers
now believed that iproniazid might be a "potent regulator of unbal¬
anced cerebral metabolism." 32 This suggested that the drug, which
had been developed to fight tuberculosis, might be fixing something
that was wrong in the brains of depressed patients. A second drug
for depressed patients, imipramine, arrived on the market during
this time, and in 1959 the New York Times called them "anti¬
depressants" for the first time. Both appeared to "reverse psychic
states,” the paper said. 33 These drugs were gaining a new status, and
finally psychiatrist Harold Himwich, in a 1958 article in Science,
explained that they "may be compared with the advent of insulin,
which counteracts symptoms of diabetes." 34 The antidepressants
were fixing something wrong in the brain, and when Hoffmann-La
Roche brought Librium to market in 1960, it picked up on this cur¬
ative message. Its new drug was not just another tranquilizer, but
rather "the successor to this entire group. . . . Librium is the biggest
step yet toward 'pure' anxiety relief as distinct from central sedation
or hypnotic action." 33 Merck did the same, marketing its drug
Suavitil as "a mood normalizer. . . . Suavitil offers a new and spe¬
cific type of neurochemical treatment for the patient who is disabled
by anxiety, tension, depression, or obsessive-compulsive manifesta¬
tions." 36
The final step in this image makeover of the psychiatric drugs
came in 1963. The NIMH had conducted a six-week trial of Tho¬
razine and other neuroleptics, and after these drugs were shown to
be more effective than a placebo in knocking down psychotic
PSYCHIATRY'S MAGIC BULLETS
6 1
symptoms, the researchers concluded that that the drugs should be
regarded "as antischizophrenic in the broad sense. In fact, it is ques¬
tionable whether the term 'tranquilizer' should be retained." 3 '
With this pronouncement by the NIMH, the transformation of
the psychiatric drugs was basically complete. In the beginning, Tho¬
razine and other neuroleptics had been viewed as agents that made
patients quieter and emotionally indifferent. Now they were
"antipsychotic" medications. Muscle relaxants that had been devel¬
oped for use in psychiatry because of their "taming” properties
were now "mood normalizers." The psychic energizers were "anti¬
depressants." All of these drugs were apparently antidotes to spe¬
cific disorders, and in that sense, they deserved to be compared to
antibiotics. They were disease-fighting agents, rather than mere ton¬
ics. All that was missing from this story of magic-bullet medicine
was an understanding of the biology of mental disorders, but with
the drugs reconceived in this way, once researchers came to under¬
stand how the drugs affected the brain, they developed two
hypotheses that, at least in theory, filled in this gap.
Chemicals in the Brain
At the start of the 1950s, there was an ongoing debate among neuro¬
logists about how signals crossed the tiny synapses that separated
neurons in the brain. The prevailing view was that the signaling was
electrical in kind, but others argued for chemical transmission, a de¬
bate that historian Elliot Valenstein, in his book Blaming the Brain,
characterized as the "war between the sparks and the soups.” How¬
ever, by the mid-1950s, researchers had isolated a number of possi¬
ble chemical messengers in the brains of rats and other mammals,
including acetylcholine, serotonin, norepinephrine, and dopamine,
and soon the "soup” model had prevailed.
With that understanding in place, an investigator at the NIMH,
Bernard Brodie, planted the intellectual seed that grew into the
theory that depression was due to a chemical imbalance in the
brain. In 1955, in experiments with rabbits, Brodie reported that re-
62
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
serpine, an herbal drug used in India to quiet psychotic patients,
lowered brain levels of serotonin. It also made the animals "lethar¬
gic" and "apathetic.” Arvid Carlsson, a Swedish pharmacologist
who had worked for a time in Brodie’s lab, soon reported that re-
serpine also reduced brain levels of norepinephrine and dopamine
(which jointly are known as catecholamines). Thus, a drug that
depleted serotonin, norepinephrine, and dopamine in the brain
seemed to make animals "depressed.” However, investigators dis¬
covered that if animals were pretreated with iproniazid or imi-
pramine before they were given reserpine, they didn't become
lethargic and apathetic. The two "antidepressants," in one manner
or another, apparently blocked reserpine's usual depletion of sero¬
tonin and the catecholamines. 38
During the 1960s, scientists at the NIMH and elsewhere figured
out how iproniazid and imipramine worked. The transmission of
signals from the "presynaptic” neuron to the "postsynaptic" neu¬
ron needs to be lightning fast and sharp, and in order for the signal
to be terminated, the chemical messenger must be removed from the
synapse. This is done in one of two ways. Either the chemical is me¬
tabolized by an enzyme and shuttled off as waste, or else it flows
back into the presynaptic neuron. Researchers discovered that ipro¬
niazid thwarts the first process. It blocks an enzyme, known as
monoamine oxidase, that metabolizes norepinephrine and sero¬
tonin. As a result, the two chemical messengers remain in the
synapse longer than normal. Imipramine inhibits the second
process. It blocks the "reuptake” of norepinephrine and serotonin
by the presynaptic neuron, and thus, once again, the two chemicals
remain in the synapse longer than normal. Both drugs produce a
similar end result, although they do so by different means.
In 1965, the NIMH's Joseph Schildkraut, in a paper published in
the Archives of General Psychiatry, reviewed this body of research
and set forth a chemical imbalance theory of affective disorders:
Those drugs [like reserpine] which cause depletion and
inactivation of norepinephrine centrally produce sedation or
depression, while drugs which increase or potentiate norepi¬
nephrine are associated with behavioral stimulation or excite-
PSYCHIATRY'S MAGIC BULLETS
63
merit and generally exert an antidepressant effect in man.
From these findings a number of investigators have formu¬
lated a hypothesis about the pathophysiology of the affective
disorders. This hypothesis, which has been designated the
"catecholamine hypothesis of affective disorders," proposes
that some, if not all depressions are associated with an ab¬
solute or relative deficiency of catecholamines, particularly
norepinephrine. 39
Although this hypothesis had its obvious limitations —it was,
Schildkraut said, "at best a reductionistic oversimplification of a
very complex biological state" —the first pillar in the construction
of the doctrine known today as "biological psychiatry" had been
erected. Two years later, researchers erected the second pillar: the
dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia.
Evidence for this theory arose from investigations into Parkin¬
son's disease. In the late 1950s, Sweden's Arvid Carlsson and others
suggested that Parkinson's might be due to a deficiency in dopa¬
mine. To test this possibility, Viennese neuropharmacologist Oleh
Hornykiewicz applied iodine to the brain of a man who'd died from
the illness, as this chemical turns dopamine pink. The basal gan¬
glia, an area of the brain that controls motor movements, was
known to be rich in dopaminergic neurons, and yet in the basal
ganglia of the Parkinson's patient, there was "hardly a tinge of pink
discoloration," Hornykiewicz reported. 90
Psychiatric researchers immediately understood the possible rele¬
vance of this to schizophrenia. Thorazine and other neuroleptics reg¬
ularly induced Parkinsonian symptoms —the same tremors, tics, and
slowed gait. And if Parkinson's resulted from the death of dopamin¬
ergic neurons in the basal ganglia, then it stood to reason that anti¬
psychotic drugs, in some manner or another, thwarted dopamine
transmission in the brain. The death of dopaminergic neurons and
the blocking of dopamine transmission would both produce a
dopamine malfunction in the basal ganglia. Carlsson soon reported
that Thorazine and the other drugs for schizophrenia did just that.
This was a finding, however, that told of drugs that "discon¬
nected” certain brain regions. They weren't normalizing brain
64
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
function; they were creating a profound pathology. However, at
this same time, researchers reported that amphetamines —drugs
known to trigger hallucinations and paranoid delusions —elevated
dopamine activity in the brain. Thus, it appeared that psychosis
might be caused by too much dopamine activity, which the neu¬
roleptics then curbed (and thus brought back into balance). If so,
the drugs could be said to be antipsychotic in kind, and in 1967,
Dutch scientist Jacques Van Rossum explicitly set forth the
dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia. "When the hypothesis of
dopamine blockade by neuroleptic agents can be further substanti¬
ated, it may have fargoing consequences for the pathophysiology of
schizophrenia. Overstimulation of dopamine receptors could then
be part of the aetiology” of the disease. 41
Expectations Fulfilled
The revolution in mental health care that Congress had hoped for
when it created the NIMH twenty years earlier was now —or so it
seemed —complete. Psychiatric drugs had been developed that were
antidotes to biological disorders, and researchers believed that the
drugs worked by countering chemical imbalances in the brain. The
horrible mental hospitals that had so shamed the nation at the end
of World War II could now be shuttered, as schizophrenics —thanks
to the new drugs —could be treated in the community. Those suffer¬
ing from a milder disorder, like depression or anxiety, simply needed
to reach into their medicine cabinets for relief. In 1967, one in three
American adults filled a prescription for a "psychoactive” medica¬
tion, with total sales of such drugs reaching $692 million. 42
This was a narrative of a scientific triumph, and in the late 1960s
and early 1970s, the men who had been the pioneers in this new
field of "psychopharmacology" looked back with pride at their
handiwork. "It was a revolution and not just a transition period,”
said Frank Ayd Jr., editor of the International Drug Therapy
Newsletter. "There was an actual revolution in the history of
psychiatry and one of the most important and dramatic epics in the
PSYCHIATRY'S MAGIC BULLETS
65
history of medicine itself." 43 Roland Kuhn, who had "discovered"
imipramine, reasoned that the development of antidepressants
could properly be seen as "an achievement of the progressively
developing human intellect." 44 Anti-anxiety medicines, said Frank
Berger, the creator of Miltown, were "adding to happiness, human
achievement, and the dignity of man." 45 Such were the sentiments
of those who had led this revolution, and finally, at a 1970 sympo¬
sium on biological psychiatry in Baltimore, Nathan Kline summed
up what most of those in attendance understood to be true: They all
had earned a place in the pantheon of great medical men.
"Medicine and science will be just that much different because
we have lived," Kline told his colleagues. "Treatment and under¬
standing of [mental] illness will forever be altered . . . and in our
own way we will persist for all time in that small contribution we
have made toward the Human Venture." 46
A Scientific Revolution ... or a Societal Delusion?
Today, by retracing the discovery of the first generation of psychi¬
atric drugs and following their transformation into magic bullets,
we can see that by 1970 two possible histories were unfolding. One
possibility is that psychiatry, in a remarkably fortuitous turn of
events, had stumbled on several types of drugs that, although they
produced abnormal behaviors in animals, nevertheless fixed various
abnormalities in the brain chemistry of those who were mentally ill.
If so, then a true revolution was indeed under way, and we can ex¬
pect that when we review the long-term outcomes produced by
these drugs, we will find that they help people get well and stay
well. The other possibility is that psychiatry, eager to have its own
magic pills and eager to take its place in mainstream medicine,
turned the drugs into something they were not. These first-
generation drugs were simply agents that perturbed normal brain
function in some way, which is what the animal research had
shown, and if that is so, then it stands to reason that the long-term
outcomes produced by the drugs might be problematic in kind.
66
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Two possible histories were under way, and in the 1970s and
1980s, researchers investigated the critical question: Do people
diagnosed with depression and schizophrenia suffer from a chemi¬
cal imbalance that is then corrected by the medication? Were the
new drugs truly antidotes to something chemically amiss in the
brain?
5
The Hunt for Chemical Imbalances
The great tragedy of science—the slaying of a
beautiful hypothesis by an ugly fact."
-THOMAS HUXLEY (1870)'
The adult human brain weighs about three pounds, and when you
see it close up, removed from the skull, it is a bit larger than you
imagined it to be. I had thought a brain could rest fairly easily in the
palm of one's hand, but you really need both hands to lift it securely
into the air. If the brain is fresh, not yet pickled in formaldehyde, a
spiderweb of blood vessels pinkens the surface, and the tissue feels
soft, almost gelatinous. It is definitely "biological" in kind, and yet
somehow it gives rise to all of the mysterious and remarkable tal¬
ents of the human mind. At the invitation of a friend, Jang-Ho Cha,
who is a neuroscientist at Massachusetts General Hospital, I at¬
tended a brain-cutting seminar at the hospital, with the thought that
seeing a human brain would help me better visualize the neuro¬
transmitter pathways that are said to give rise to depression and
psychosis, but naturally my visit turned into something more than
that. The human brain up close takes your breath away.
The mechanics of its messaging system are fairly well under¬
stood. There are, Cha noted, 100 billion neurons in the human
brain. The cell body of a "typical" neuron receives input from a vast
web of dendrites, and it sends out a signal via a single axon that
may project to a distant area of the brain (or down the spinal cord).
At its end, an axon branches into numerous terminals, and it is from
68
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
these terminals that chemical messengers —dopamine, serotonin,
etc.—are released into the synaptic cleft, which is a gap about
twenty nanometers wide (a nanometer is one-billionth of a meter).
A single neuron has between one thousand and ten thousand synap¬
tic connections, with the adult brain as a whole having perhaps 150
trillion synapses.
The axons of neurons that use the same neurotransmitter are
regularly bundled together, almost like the strands of a telecommu¬
nications cable, and once scientists discovered that dopamine,
norepinephrine, and serotonin fluoresced different colors when ex¬
posed to formaldehyde vapors, it became possible to track those
neurotransmitter pathways in the brain. Although Joseph Schild-
kraut, when he formulated his theory of affective disorders, thought
that norepinephrine was the neurotransmitter most likely to be in
short supply in those who were depressed, researchers fairly quickly
turned much of their attention to serotonin, and so for our pur¬
poses, in regard to our investigation of the chemical imbalance
theory of mental disorders, we need to look at that pathway in the
Serotonergic Pathways in the Brain
Cerebral cortex
Frontal
lobe
of
serotonergic
nerves
Basal ganglia
Temporal lobe
Hypothalamus
Raphe nuclei
(clusters
of cell
bodies of
serotonergic
nerves)
Pituitary
gland
To spinal cord
THE HUNT FOR CHEMICAL IMBALANCES
6 9
brain for depression, and at the dopaminergic pathway for schizo¬
phrenia.
The serotonergic pathway is one with ancient evolutionary roots.
Serotonergic neurons are found in the nervous systems of all verte¬
brates and most invertebrates, and in humans their cell bodies are
located in the brain stem, in an area known as the raphe nuclei.
Some of these neurons send long axons down the spinal cord, a sys¬
tem that is involved in the control of respiratory, cardiac, and gas¬
trointestinal activities. Other serotonergic neurons have axons that
ascend into all areas of the brain —the cerebellum, the hypothala¬
mus, the basal ganglia, the temporal lobes, the limbic system, the
cerebral cortex, and the frontal lobes. This pathway is involved in
Frontal
cortex
Mesocortical
system
Mesolimbic
system
Ventral
tegmental area
Substantia
nigra
Nigrostriatal
system
To basal
ganglia
Dopaminergic Pathways in the Brain
To striatum
70
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
memory, learning, sleep, appetite, and the regulation of moods and
behaviors. As Efrain Azmitia, a professor of biology at NYU, has
noted, "the brain serotonin system is the single largest brain system
known and can be characterized as a 'giant' neuronal system." 2
There are three major dopaminergic pathways in the brain. The
cell bodies of all three systems are located atop the brain stem, in ei¬
ther the substantia nigra or the ventral tegmentum. Their axons
project to the basal ganglia (nigrostriatal system), the limbic region
(mesolimbic system), and the frontal lobes (mesocortical system).
The basal ganglia initiates and controls movement. The limbic
structures —the olfactory tubercle, the nucleus accumbens, and the
amygdala, among others —are located behind the frontal lobes and
help regulate our emotions. It is here that we feel the world, a
process that is vital to our sense of self and our conceptions of real¬
ity. The frontal lobes are the most distinguishing feature of the
human brain, and provide us with the godlike capacity to monitor
our own selves.
All of this physiology —the 100 billion neurons, the 150 trillion
synapses, the various neurotransmitter pathways —tell of a brain
that is almost infinitely complex. Yet the chemical imbalance theory
of mental disorders boiled this complexity down to a simple disease
mechanism, one easy to grasp. In depression, the problem was that
the serotonergic neurons released too little serotonin into the synap¬
tic gap, and thus the serotonergic pathways in the brain were "un¬
deractive." Antidepressants brought serotonin levels in the synaptic
gap up to normal, and that allowed these pathways to transmit mes¬
sages at a proper pace. Meanwhile, the hallucinations and voices
that characterized schizophrenia resulted from overactive dopamin¬
ergic pathways. Either the presynaptic neurons pumped out too
much dopamine into the synapse or the target neurons had an ab¬
normally high density of dopamine receptors. Antipsychotics put a
brake on this system, and this allowed the dopaminergic pathways
to function in a more normal manner.
That was the chemical imbalance theory put forth by Schildkraut
and Jacques Van Rossum, and the very research that had led
Schildkraut to his hypothesis also provided investigators with a
method for testing it. The studies of iproniazid and imipramine had
THE HUNT FOR CHEMICAL IMBALANCES
• 71
shown that neurotransmitters were removed from the synapse in
one of two ways. Either the chemical was taken back up into the
presynaptic neuron and restored for later use, or it was metabolized
by an enzyme and carted off as waste. Serotonin is metabolized
into 5-hydroxyindole acetic acid (5-HIAA); dopamine is turned
into homovanillic acid (HVA). Researchers could comb the cere¬
brospinal fluid for these metabolites, and the amounts found would
serve as an indirect gauge of the synaptic levels of the neurotrans¬
mitters. Since low serotonin was theorized to cause depression,
anyone in that emotional state should have lower-than-normal
levels of 5-HIAA in his or her cerebrospinal fluid. Similarly, since an
overactive dopamine system was theorized to cause schizophrenia,
people who heard voices or were paranoid should have abnormally
high cerebrospinal levels of HVA.
This line of research kept scientists busy for nearly fifteen years.
The Serotonin Hypothesis Is Put to the Test
In 1969, Malcolm Bowers at Yale University became the first to re¬
port on whether depressed patients had low levels of serotonin
metabolites in their cerebrospinal fluid. In a study of eight depressed
patients (all of whom had been previously exposed to antidepres¬
sants), he announced that their 5-HIAA levels were lower than nor¬
mal, but not "significantly" so. 3 Two years later, investigators at
McGill University said that they, too, had failed to find a "statisti¬
cally significant" difference in the 5-HIAA levels of depressed pa¬
tients and normal controls, and that they also had failed to find any
correlation between 5-HIAA levels and the severity of depressive
symptoms. 4 In 1974, Bowers was back with a more finely tuned
follow-up study: Depressed patients who had not been exposed to
antidepressants had perfectly normal 5-HIAA levels. 5
The serotonin theory of depression did not seem to be panning
out, and in 1974, two researchers at the University of Pennsylvania,
Joseph Mendels and Alan Frazer, revisited the evidence that had led
Schildkraut to advance his theory in the first place. Schildkraut had
72
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
noted that reserpine, which depleted monoamines in the brain
(norepinephrine, serotonin, and dopamine), regularly made people
depressed. But when Mendels and Frazer looked closely at the scien¬
tific literature, they found that when hypertensive patients were
given reserpine, only 6 percent in fact got the blues. Furthermore, in
1955, a group of physicians in England had given the herbal drug to
their depressed patients, and it had lifted the spirits of many. Reser¬
pine, Mendels and Frazer concluded, didn't reliably induce depres¬
sion at all. 6 They also noted that when researchers had given other
monoamine-depleting drugs to people, those agents hadn't induced
depression either. "The literature reviewed here strongly suggests
that the depletion of brain norepinephrine, dopamine or serotonin
is in itself not sufficient to account for the development of the clini¬
cal syndrome of depression," they wrote.’
It seemed that the theory was about to be declared dead and
buried, but then, in 1975, Marie Asberg and her colleagues at the
Karolinska Institute in Stockholm breathed new life into it. Twenty
of the sixty-eight depressed patients they had tested suffered from
low 5-F1IAA levels, and these low-serotonin patients were some¬
what more suicidal than the rest, with two of the twenty eventually
committing suicide. This was evidence, the Swedish researchers
said, that there might be "a biochemical subgroup of depressive dis¬
order characterized by a disturbance of serotonin turnover." 8
Soon prominent psychiatrists in the United States were writing
that "nearly 30 percent" of depressed patients had been found to
have low serotonin levels. The serotonin theory of depression
seemed at least partly vindicated. But today, if we revisit Asberg's
study and examine her data, we can see that her finding of a "bio¬
logical subgroup” of depressed patients was mostly a story of wish¬
ful thinking.
In her study, Asberg reported that 25 percent of her "normal"
group had cerebrospinal 5-HIAA levels below fifteen nanograms
per milliliter. Fifty percent had fifteen to twenty-five nanograms of
5-HIAA per milliliter, and the remaining 25 percent had levels
above twenty-five nanograms. The bell curve for her "normals"
showed that 5-HIAA levels were quite variable. But what she failed
THE HUNT FOR CHEMICAL
MBALANCES
73
to note in her discussion was that the bell curve for the sixty-eight
depressed patients in her study was almost exactly the same.
Twenty-nine percent (twenty of the sixty-eight) had 5-HIAA counts
below fifteen nanograms, 47 percent had levels between fifteen and
twenty-five nanograms, and 24 percent had levels above twenty-five
nanograms. Twenty-nine percent of depressed patients may have
had "low” levels of serotonin metabolites in their cerebrospinal
fluid (this was her "biological subgroup"), but then so did 25
percent of "normal" people. The median level for normals was
twenty nanograms, and, it so turned out, more than half of the de¬
pressed patients —thirty-seven of sixty-eight —had levels above that
amount.
Viewed in this way, her study had not provided any new reason
to believe in the serotonin theory of depression. Japanese investiga¬
tors soon revealed, in an unwitting way, the faulty logic at work.
They reported that some antidepressants (used in Japan) blocked
serotonin receptors, inhibiting the firing of those pathways, and
thus they reasoned that depression might be caused by an "excess of
free serotonin in the synaptic cleft." 9 They had applied the same
backwards reasoning that had given rise to the low-serotonin the¬
ory of depression, and if the Japanese scientists had wanted to, they
could have pointed to Asberg's study for support of their theory, as
the Swedes had found that 24 percent of depressed patients had
"high" levels of serotonin.
In 1984, NIMH investigators studied the low-serotonin theory
of depression one more time. They wanted to see whether the "bio¬
logical subgroup” of depressed patients with "low” levels of sero¬
tonin were the best responders to an antidepressant, amitriptyline,
that selectively blocked its reuptake. If an antidepressant was an anti¬
dote to a chemical imbalance in the brain, then amitriptyline should
be most effective in that subgroup. But, lead investigator James
Maas wrote, "contrary to expectations, no relationships between
cerebrospinal 5-HIAA and response to amitriptyline were found." 10
Moreover, he and the other NIMH researchers discovered—just as
Asberg had —that 5-HIAA levels varied widely in depressed pa¬
tients. Some had high levels of serotonin metabolites in their cere-
74 • ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
brospinal fluid, while others had low levels. The NIMH scientists
drew the only possible conclusion: "Elevations or decrements in the
functioning of serotonergic systems per se are not likely to be
associated with depression."*
Even after this report, the serotonin theory of depression did not
completely go away. The commercial success of Prozac, a "selective
serotonin reuptake inhibitor” brought to market in 1988 by Eli
Lilly, fueled a new round of public claims that depression was due
to low levels of this neurotransmitter, and once again any number of
investigators conducted experiments to see if that were so. But this
second round of studies produced the same results as the first. "I
spent the first several years of my career doing full-time research on
brain serotonin metabolism, but I never saw any convincing evi¬
dence that any psychiatric disorder, including depression, results
from a deficiency of brain serotonin," said Stanford psychiatrist
David Burns in 2 003.“ Numerous others made this same point.
"There is no scientific evidence whatsoever that clinical depression
is due to any kind of biological deficit state,” wrote Colin Ross, an
associate professor of psychiatry at Southwest Medical Center in
Dallas, in his 1995 book, Pseudoscience in Biological Psychiatry. 11
In 2 000, the authors of Essential Psychopharmacology told medical
students "there is no clear and convincing evidence that monoamine
deficiency accounts for depression; that is, there is no 'real'
monoamine deficit." 13 Yet, fueled by pharmaceutical advertise¬
ments, the belief lived on, and it caused Irish psychiatrist David
Healy, who has written a number of books on the history of psychi-
* The NIMH researchers also looked at a number of other possible associa¬
tions between variable neurotransmitter levels and response to an antidepres¬
sant. They measured norepinephrine metabolites and dopamine metabolites;
they divided their depressed patients into bipolar and unipolar groups; and
they evaluated their response to two antidepressants, imipramine and
amitriptyline. They found mild associations between several of these sub¬
groups and their response to one or other of the drugs; I have focused here on
their findings regarding whether (a) depression is due to low levels of sero¬
tonin, and (b) if the subgroup of patients with low levels of serotonin responds
better to a drug that selectively blocks the reuptake of this neurotransmitter.
THE HUNT FOR CHEMICAL
MBALANCES
75
atry, to quip in 2005 that this theory needed to be put into the med¬
ical dustbin, where other such discredited theories can be found.
"The serotonin theory of depression," he wrote, with evident exas¬
peration, "is comparable to the masturbatory theory of insanity." 14
Dopamine Deja Vu
When Van Rossum set forth his dopamine hypothesis of schizo¬
phrenia, he noted that the first thing that investigators needed to do
was "further substantiate" that antipsychotic drugs did indeed
thwart dopamine transmission in the brain. This took some time,
but by 1975, Solomon Snyder at Johns Hopkins Medical School
and Philip Seeman at the University of Toronto had fleshed out how
the drugs achieved that effect. First, Snyder identified two distinct
types of dopamine receptors, known as D, and D., Next, both in¬
vestigators found that antipsychotics blocked 70 to 90 percent of
the D 2 receptors. 15 Newspapers now told of how these drugs might
be correcting a chemical imbalance in the brain.
"Too much dopamine function in the brain could account for the
overwhelming flood of sensations that plagues the schizophrenic,”
the New York Times explained. "By blocking the brain's receptor
sites for dopamine, neuroleptics put an end to sights and sounds
that are not really there." 16
However, even as Snyder and Seeman were reporting their re¬
sults, Malcolm Bowers was announcing findings that cast a cloud
over the dopamine hypothesis. He had measured the level of
dopamine metabolites in the cerebrospinal fluid of unmedicated
schizophrenics and found them to be quite normal. "Our findings,"
he wrote, "do not furnish neurochemical evidence for an over¬
arousal in these patients emanating from a midbrain dopamine sys¬
tem.” 17 Others soon reported similar results. In 1975, Robert Post
at the NIMH determined that HVA levels in the cerebrospinal fluid
of twenty unmedicated schizophrenics "were not significantly dif¬
ferent from controls." 16 Autopsy studies also revealed that the brain
tissue of drug-free schizophrenics did not have abnormal levels of
76
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
dopamine. In 1982, UCLA's John Haracz reviewed this body of
research and drew the obvious bottom-line conclusion: "These
findings do not support the presence of elevated dopamine turnover
in the brains of [unmedicated] schizophrenics." 19
Having discovered that dopamine levels in never-medicated
schizophrenics were normal, researchers turned their attention to a
second possibility. Perhaps people with schizophrenia had an over¬
abundance of dopamine receptors. If so, the postsynaptic neurons
would be "hypersensitive” to dopamine, and this would cause the
dopaminergic pathways to be overstimulated. In 1978, Philip See-
man at the University of Toronto announced in Nature that this was
indeed the case. At autopsy, the brains of twenty schizophrenics had
70 percent more Di receptors than normal. At first glance, it seemed
that the cause of schizophrenia had been found, but Seeman cau¬
tioned that all of the patients had been on neuroleptics prior to their
deaths. "Although these results are apparently compatible with the
dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia in general," he wrote, the in¬
crease in D 2 receptors might "have resulted from the long-term ad¬
ministration of neuroleptics." 20
A variety of studies quickly proved that the drugs were indeed the
culprit. When rats were fed neuroleptics, their Dj receptors quickly
increased in number. 21 If rats were given a drug that blocked D, re¬
ceptors, that receptor subtype increased in density. 22 In each in¬
stance, the increase was evidence of the brain trying to compensate
for the drug's blocking of its signals. Then, in 1982, Angus MacKay
and his British colleagues reported that when they examined brain
tissue from forty-eight deceased schizophrenics, "the increases in
[Di] receptors were seen only in patients in whom neuroleptic medi¬
cation had been maintained until the time of death, indicating that
they were entirely iatrogenic [drug-caused]." 23 A few years later,
German investigators reported the same results from their autopsy
studies. 24 Finally, investigators in France, Sweden, and Finland used
positron emission topography to study D,-receptor densities in liv¬
ing patients who had never been exposed to neuroleptics, and all re¬
ported "no significant differences" between the schizophrenics and
"normal controls." 25
THE HUNT FOR CHEMICAL
MBALANCES
77
Since that time, researchers have continued to study whether
there might be something amiss with the dopaminergic pathways in
people diagnosed with schizophrenia, and now and then someone
reports having found an abnormality of some type in a subset of pa¬
tients. But by the end of the 1980s, it was clear that the chemical-
imbalance hypothesis of schizophrenia —that this was a disease
characterized by a hyperactive dopamine system that was then put
somewhat back into balance by the drugs —had come to a crashing
end. "The dopaminergic theory of schizophrenia retains little credi¬
bility for psychiatrists," observed Pierre Deniker in 1990. 2 ' Four
years later, John Kane, a well-known psychiatrist at Long Island
Jewish Medical Center, echoed the sentiment, noting that there
was "no good evidence for any perturbation of the dopamine func¬
tion in schizophrenia." 27 Still, the public continued to be told that
people diagnosed with schizophrenia had overactive dopamine sys¬
tems, with the drugs likened to "insulin for diabetes,” and thus for¬
mer NIMH director Steve Hyman, in his 2002 book, Molecular
Neuropharmacology, was moved to once again remind readers of
the truth. "There is no compelling evidence that a lesion in the
dopamine system is a primary cause of schizophrenia,” he wrote. 2 '
Requiem for a Theory
The low-serotonin hypothesis of depression and the high-dopamine
hypothesis of schizophrenia had always been the twin pillars of the
chemical-imbalance theory of mental disorders, and by the late
1980s, both had been found wanting. Other mental disorders have
also been touted to the public as diseases caused by chemical imbal¬
ances, but there was never any evidence to support those claims.
Parents were told that children diagnosed with attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder suffered from low dopamine levels, but the
only reason they were told that was because Ritalin stirred neurons
to release extra dopamine. This became the storytelling formula
that was relied upon by pharmaceutical companies again and again:
78
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Researchers would identify the mechanism of action for a class of
drugs, how the drugs either lowered or raised levels of a brain neuro¬
transmitter, and soon the public would be told that people treated
with those medications suffered from the opposite problem.
From a scientific point of view, it is apparent today that the
chemical imbalance hypothesis was always wobbly in kind, and
many of the scientists who watched its rise and fall have looked
back on it with a bit of embarrassment. As early as 1975, Joseph
Mendels and Alan Frazer had concluded that Schildkraut's hypoth¬
esis of depression had arisen out of "tunnel thinking” that relied on
an "inadequate evaluation of certain findings not consistent with
the initial assumption." 2 ’ In 1990, Deniker said that the same was
true of the dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia. When psychiatric
researchers recast the drugs as "antischizophrenic” agents, he
noted, they had gone "a bit far . . . one can say that neuroleptics di¬
minish certain phenomena of schizophrenia, but [the drugs] do not
pretend to be an etiological treatment of these psychoses." 30 The
chemical-imbalance theory of mental disorders, wrote David Flealy,
in his book The Creation of Psychopharmacology, was embraced by
psychiatrists because it "set the stage” for them "to become real
doctors." 31 Doctors in internal medicine had their antibiotics, and
now psychiatrists could have their "anti-disease" pills too.
Yet a societal belief in chemical imbalances has remained (for rea¬
sons that will be explored later), and it has led those who have in¬
vestigated and written about this history to emphasize, time and
again, the same bottom-line conclusion. "The evidence does not sup¬
port any of the biochemical theories of mental illness,” concluded
Elliot Valenstein, a professor of neuroscience at the University of
Michigan, in his 1998 book Blaming the Brain. 32 Even U.S. surgeon
general David Satcher, in his 1999 report Mental Health, confessed
that "the precise causes [etiologies] of mental disorders are not
known." 33 In Prozac Backlash, Joseph Glenmullen, an instructor of
psychiatry at Harvard Medical School, noted that "in every instance
where such an imbalance was thought to be found, it was later
proved to be false." 34 Finally, in 2005, Kenneth Kendler, coeditor in
chief of Psychological Medicine, penned an admirably succinct
epitaph for this whole story: "We have hunted for big simple
THE HUNT FOR CHEMICAL
MBALANCES
79
neurochemical explanations for psychiatric disorders and have not
found them." 35
This brings us to our next big question: If psychiatric drugs don't
fix abnormal brain chemistry, what do they do?
Prozac on My Mind
During the 1970s and 1980s, investigators put together detailed ac¬
counts of how the various classes of psychiatric drugs act on the
brain, and how the brain in turn reacts to the drugs. We could relate
the history of antidepressants, neuroleptics, benzodiazepines, or
stimulants, and all of those histories would tell of a somewhat com¬
mon process at work. But since the story of chemical imbalances in
the public mind really took off after Eli Lilly brought Prozac (fluox¬
etine) to market, it seems appropriate to review what Eli Lilly
scientists and other investigators, in reports published in scientific
journals, had to say about how this "selective serotonin reuptake
inhibitor" actually worked.
As was noted earlier, once a presynaptic neuron has released
serotonin into the synaptic gap, it must be quickly removed so that
the signal can be crisply terminated. An enzyme metabolizes a small
amount; the rest is pumped back into the presynaptic neuron, enter¬
ing via a channel known as SERT (serotonin reuptake transport).
Fluoxetine blocks this reuptake channel, and as a result, Eli Lilly
scientist James Clemens wrote in 1975, it causes a "pile-up of sero¬
tonin at the synapse." 36
However, as the Eli Lilly investigators discovered, a feedback
mechanism then kicks in. The presynaptic neuron has "auto-
receptors" on its terminal membrane that monitor the level of
serotonin in the synapse. If serotonin levels are too low, one scientist
quipped, these autoreceptors scream "turn on the serotonin ma¬
chine.” If serotonin levels are too high, they scream "turn it off."
This is a feedback loop designed by evolution to keep the serotoner¬
gic system in balance, and fluoxetine triggers the latter message.
With serotonin no longer being whisked away from the synapse, the
So
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
autoreceptors tell the presynaptic neurons to fire at a dramatically
lower rate. They begin to release lower-than-normal amounts of
serotonin into the synapse.
Feedback mechanisms also change the postsynaptic neurons.
Within four weeks, the density of their serotonin receptors drops 25
percent below normal, Eli Lilly scientists reported in 1981. 33 Other
investigators subsequently reported that "chronic fluoxetine treat¬
ment" may lead to a 50 percent reduction in serotonin receptors in
certain areas of the brain. 38 As a result, the postsynaptic neurons be¬
come "desensitized” to the chemical messenger.
At this point, it may seem that the brain has successfully adapted
to the drug. Fluoxetine blocks the normal reuptake of serotonin
from the synapse, but the presynaptic neurons then begin releasing
less serotonin and the postsynaptic neurons become less sensitive to
serotonin and thus don't fire so readily. The drug was designed to
accelerate the serotonergic pathway; the brain responded by putting
on the brake. It has kept its serotonergic pathway more or less in
balance, an adaptive response that researchers have dubbed "synap¬
tic resilience." 35 However, there is one other change that occurs dur¬
ing this initial two-week period, and it ultimately short-circuits the
brain's compensatory response. The autoreceptors for serotonin on
the presynaptic neurons decline in number. As a result, this feed¬
back mechanism becomes partially disabled, and the "turn off the
serotonin machine” message dims. The presynaptic neurons begin
to fire at a normal rate again, at least for a while, and to release
more serotonin than normal each time.* 40
As the Eli Lilly scientists and others put together this picture of
fluoxetine's effects on the brain, they speculated as to what part of
this process was responsible for the drug’s antidepressant proper¬
ties. Psychiatrists had long observed that antidepressants took two
or three weeks to "work,” and thus the Eli Lilly researchers rea¬
soned in 1981 that it was the decline in serotonin receptors, which
took several weeks to occur, that was "the underlying mechanism
associated with the therapeutic response." 41 If so, the drug could be
* Over the long term, it appears that serotonin release falls to an abnormally
low level, at least in certain regions of the brain.
THE HUNT FOR CHEMICAL IMBALANCES
81
said to work because it drove the serotonergic system into a less
responsive state. But once researchers discovered that fluoxetine
partially disabled the feedback mechanism, Claude de Montigny at
McGill University argued that this was what allowed the drug to
begin working. This disabling process also took two or three weeks
to occur, and it allowed the presynaptic neurons to begin pumping
higher amounts of serotonin than normal into the synapse. At that
point, with fluoxetine continuing to block serotonin's removal, the
neurotransmitter could now indeed "pile up” in the synapse, and
that would lead "to an enhancement of central serotonergic neuro¬
transmission," de Montigny wrote. 42
That is the scientific story of how fluoxetine alters the brain, and
it may be that this process helps depressed people get well and stay
well. Only the outcomes literature can reveal whether that is so. But
the medicine clearly doesn't fix a chemical imbalance in the brain.
Instead, it does precisely the opposite. Prior to being medicated, a
depressed person has no known chemical imbalance. Fluoxetine
then gums up the normal removal of serotonin from the synapse,
and that triggers a cascade of changes, and several weeks later the
serotonergic pathway is operating in a decidedly abnormal manner.
The presynaptic neuron is putting out more serotonin than usual.
Its serotonin reuptake channels are blocked by the drug. The sys¬
tem's feedback loop is partially disabled. The postsynaptic neurons
are "desensitized” to serotonin. Mechanically speaking, the sero¬
tonergic system is now rather mucked up.
Eli Lilly's scientists were well aware that this was so. In 1977,
Ray Fuller and David Wong observed that fluoxetine, since it dis¬
rupted serotonergic pathways, could be used to study "the role of
serotonin neurons in various brain functions —behavior, sleep, reg¬
ulation of pituitary hormone release, thermoregulation, pain re¬
sponsiveness and so on." To conduct such experiments, researchers
could administer fluoxetine to animals and observe which functions
became compromised. They would look for pathologies to appear.
This type of research in fact was already being done: Fuller and
Wong reported in 1977 that the drug stirred "stereotyped hyper¬
activity" in rats and "suppressed REM sleep” in both rats and
cats. 43
82
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
In 1991, in a paper published in the Journal of Clinical Psychia¬
try, Princeton neuroscientist Barry Jacobs made this very point
about SSRIs. He wrote:
These drugs "alter the level of synaptic transmission beyond
the physiologic range achieved under [normal] environmen¬
tal/biological conditions. Thus, any behavioral or physiologic
change produced under these conditions might more appro¬
priately be considered pathologic, rather than reflective of the
normal biological role of 5-HT [serotonin.]” 44
During the 1970s and 1980s, researchers studying the effects of
neuroleptics fleshed out a similar story. Thorazine and other
standard antipsychotics block 70 to 90 percent of all D, receptors in
the brain. In response, the presynaptic neurons begin pumping out
more dopamine and the postsynaptic neurons increase the density
of their D= receptors by 30 percent or more. In this manner, the
brain is trying to "compensate” for the drug's effects so that it can
maintain the transmission of messages along its dopaminergic path¬
ways. However, after about three weeks, the pathway's feedback
mechanism begins to fail, and the presynaptic neurons begin to fire
in irregular patterns or turn quiescent. It is this "inactivation" of
dopaminergic pathways that "may be the basis for the antipsychotic
action," explains the American Psychiatric Association's Textbook
of Psychopharmacology. 15
Once again, this is a story of neurotransmitter pathways that
have been transformed by the medication. After several weeks, their
feedback loops are partially disabled, the presynaptic neurons are
releasing less dopamine than normal, the drug is thwarting
dopamine's effects by blocking D: receptors, and the postsynaptic
neurons have an abnormally high density of these receptors. The
drugs do not normalize brain chemistry, but disturb it, and if
Jacob's reasoning is followed, to a degree that could be considered
"pathological."
THE HUNT FOR CHEMICAL IMBALANCES • 83
A Paradigm for Understanding Psychotropic Drugs
Today, as provost of Harvard University, Steve Hyman is mostly en¬
gaged in the many political and administrative tasks that come with
leading a large institution. But he is a neuroscientist by training, and
in 1996 to 2001, when he was the director of the NIMH, he wrote
a paper, one both memorable and provocative in kind, that summed
up all that had been learned about psychiatric drugs. Titled "Initia¬
tion and Adaptation: A Paradigm for Understanding Psychotropic
Drug Action," it was published in the American Journal of Psychia¬
try, and it told of how all psychotropic drugs could be understood
to act on the brain in a common way. 46
Antipsychotics, antidepressants, and other psychotropic drugs,
he wrote, "create perturbations in neurotansmitter functions.” In
response, the brain goes through a series of compensatory adapta¬
tions. If a drug blocks a neurotransmitter (as an antipsychotic does),
the presynaptic neurons spring into hyper gear and release more of
it, and the postsynaptic neurons increase the density of their recep¬
tors for that chemical messenger. Conversely, if a drug increases the
synaptic levels of a neurotransmitter (as an antidepressant does), it
provokes the opposite response: The presynaptic neurons decrease
their firing rates and the postsynaptic neurons decrease the density
of their receptors for the neurotransmitter. In each instance, the
brain is trying to nullify the drug's effects. "These adaptations,”
Hyman explained, "are rooted in homeostatic mechanisms that
exist, presumably, to permit cells to maintain their equilibrium in
the face of alterations in the environment or changes in the internal
milieu.”
However, after a period of time, these compensatory mechanisms
break down. The "chronic administration" of the drug then causes
"substantial and long-lasting alterations in neural function,"
Hyman wrote. As part of this long-term adaptation process, there
are changes in intracellular signaling pathways and gene expression.
After a few weeks, he concluded, the person's brain is functioning in
a manner that is "qualitatively as well as quantitatively different
from the normal state.”
84. ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
His was an elegant paper, and it summed up what had been
learned from decades of impressive scientific work. Forty years ear¬
lier, when Thorazine and the other first-generation psychiatric drugs
were discovered, scientists had little understanding of how neurons
communicated with one another. Now they had a remarkably de¬
tailed understanding of neurotransmitter systems in the brain and
of how drugs acted on them. And what science had revealed was
this: Prior to treatment, patients diagnosed with schizophrenia, de¬
pression, and other psychiatric disorders do not suffer from any
known "chemical imbalance.” However, once a person is put on a
psychiatric medication, which, in one manner or another, throws a
wrench into the usual mechanics of a neuronal pathway, his or her
brain begins to function, as Hyman observed, abnormally.
Back to the Beginning
While Dr. Hyman's paper may seem startling, it serves as a coda to
a scientific narrative that is, in fact, consistent from beginning to
end. His was not a conclusion that should be seen as unexpected,
but rather one that was predicted by psychopharmacology's open¬
ing chapter.
As we saw, Thorazine, Miltown, and Marsilid were all derived
from compounds that had been developed for other purposes —for
use in surgery or as possible "magic bullets" against infectious dis¬
eases. Those compounds were then found to cause alterations in
mood, behavior, and thinking that were seen as helpful to psychi¬
atric patients. The drugs, in essence, were perceived as having bene¬
ficial side effects. They perturbed normal function, and that
understanding was reflected in the initial names given to them.
Chlorpromazine was a "major tranquilizer,” and it was said to pro¬
duce a change in being similar to frontal lobotomy. Meprobamate
was a "minor tranquilizer," and in animal studies, it had been
shown to be a powerful muscle relaxant that blocked normal emo¬
tional response to environmental stressors. Iproniazid was a "psy¬
chic stimulator,” and if the report of TB patients dancing in the
THE HUNT FOR CHEMICAL
MBALANCES
85
wards was truthful, it was a drug that could provoke something
akin to mania. However, psychiatry then reconceived the drugs as
"magic bullets" for mental disorders, the drugs hypothesized to be
antidotes to chemical imbalances in the brain. But that theory,
which arose as much from wishful thinking as from science, was
investigated and it did not pan out. Instead, as Hyman wrote, psy-
chotropics are drugs that perturb the normal functioning of
neuronal pathways in the brain. Psychiatry's first impression of its
new drugs turned out to be the scientifically accurate one.
With this understanding of psychiatric medications now in mind,
it is possible to pose the scientific question at the heart of this book:
Do these drugs help or harm patients over the long term? What do
fifty years of outcomes research show?
Part Three
Outcomes
6
A Paradox Revealed
"If we wish to base psychiatry on evidence-based
medicine, we run a genuine risk in taking a closer
look at what has long been considered fact."
-EMMANUEL STIP, EUROPEAN PSYCHIATRY (2002)'
The basement in Harvard Medical School's Countway Library is
one of my favorite places in Boston. After stepping off the elevator,
you enter a huge, somewhat dingy room, filled with the musty smell
of old books. I often stop a few feet inside the doorway and take in
the grand sight: row after row of bound copies of medical journals
from the early 1800s to 1986. The place is almost always empty,
and yet there are rich histories to be discovered here, and soon, as
you begin to piece together a particular narrative of medicine, you
are hopping from one journal to the next, the pile of books on your
desk growing ever higher. There is the thrill of the chase, and it
seems too that this part of the library never disappoints. All of the
journals are organized in alphabetical order, and whenever in one
article you find a citation that interests you, all you have to do is
walk a few feet and inevitably you find the journal you need. At
least up until recently, the Countway Library seems to have pur¬
chased nearly every medical journal that was published.
This is where we can begin our quest to find out how psychiatric
drugs affect long-term outcomes. The research method we'll need to
follow is straightforward. First, to the best we can, we'll have to
flesh out the natural spectrum of outcomes for each particular dis¬
order. In the absence of antipsychotic medications, how would peo-
90
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
pie diagnosed with schizophrenia likely fare over time? What
chance —if any —would they have of recovering? How well might
they fare in society? The same questions can be asked in regard to
anxiety, depression, and bipolar illness. What would outcomes look
like in the absence of anti-anxiety drugs, antidepressants, and mood
stabilizers? Once we have a sense of a baseline for a disorder, we
can trace the outcomes literature for that illness, and we can hope
that it will tell a consistent, coherent story. Do the drug treatments
alter the long-term course of a mental disorder —in the patient pop¬
ulation as a whole —for the better? Or for the worse?
Since chlorpromazine (Thorazine) was the drug that launched the
psychopharmacology revolution, it seems appropriate to investigate
schizophrenia outcomes first.
The Natural History of Schizophrenia
Schizophrenia today is regularly thought of as a lifelong, chronic ill¬
ness, and that is an understanding that originated with the work of
German psychiatrist Emil Kraepelin. In the late 1800s, he systemat¬
ically tracked the outcomes of patients at an asylum in Estonia, and
he observed that there was an identifiable group that reliably deteri¬
orated into dementia. These were patients who, upon entry to the
asylum, showed a lack of emotion. Many were catatonic, or lost
hopelessly in their own worlds, and they often had gross physical
problems. They walked oddly, suffered from facial tics and muscle
spasms, and were unable to complete willed physical acts. In his
1899 textbook Lehrbuch der Psychiatrie, Kraepelin wrote that
these patients suffered from dementia praecox, and in 1908, Swiss
psychiatrist Eugen Bleuler coined the term "schizophrenia” as a
substitute diagnostic term for patients in this dilapidated condition.
However, as British historian Mary Boyle convincingly argued in
a 1990 article, "Is Schizophrenia What It Was? A Re-analysis of
Kraepelin's and Bleuler's Population,” many of Kraepelin's demen¬
tia praecox patients were undoubtedly suffering from a viral dis¬
ease, encephalitis lethargica, which in the late 1800s had yet to be
A PARADOX REVEALED
91
identified. This disease caused people to turn delirious, or to drop
into a stupor, or to start walking in a jerky manner, and once Aus¬
trian neurologist Constantin von Economo described the illness in
1917, the encephalitis lethargica patients were no longer part of the
"schizophrenia” pool, and after that happened, the patient group
that remained was quite different from Kraepelin's dementia prae-
cox group. "The inaccessible, the stuporous catatonic, the intellec¬
tually deteriorated” —those types of schizophrenia patients, Boyle
noted, largely disappeared. As a result, the descriptions of schizo¬
phrenia in psychiatric textbooks during the 1920s and 1930s
changed. All of the old physical symptoms —the greasy skin, the
odd gait, the muscle spasms, the facial tics —disappeared from the
diagnostic manuals. What remained were the mental symptoms —
the hallucinations, the delusions, and the bizarre thoughts. "The
referents of schizophrenia," Boyle wrote, "gradually changed until
the diagnosis came to be applied to a population who bore only a
slight, and possibly superficial, resemblance to Kraepelin's." 2
So now we have to ask: What is the natural spectrum of out¬
comes for that group of psychotic patients? Here, unfortunately, we
run into a second problem. From 1900 until the end of World War
II, eugenic attitudes toward the mentally ill were quite popular in
the United States, and that social philosophy dramatically affected
their outcomes. Eugenicists argued that the mentally ill needed to be
sequestered in hospitals to keep them from having children and
spreading their "bad genes.” The goal was to keep them confined in
asylums, and in 1923, an editorial in the Journal of Heredity con¬
cluded, with an air of satisfaction, that "segregation of the insane is
fairly complete." 3 As a result, many people diagnosed with schizo¬
phrenia in the first half of the century were hospitalized and never
discharged, but that social policy was then misperceived as out¬
comes data. The fact that schizophrenics never left the hospital was
seen as proof that the disease was a chronic, hopeless illness.
However, after World War II, eugenics fell into disrepute. This
was the very "science” that Hitler and Nazi Germany had em¬
braced, and after Albert Deutsch's expose of the abysmal conditions
in U.S. mental hospitals, in which he likened them to concentration
camps, many states began talking about treating the mentally ill in
92
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
the community. Social policy changed and discharge rates soared.
As a result, there is a brief window of time, from 1946 to 1954,
when we can look at how newly diagnosed schizophrenia patients
fared and thereby get a sense of the "natural outcomes" of schizo¬
phrenia prior to the arrival of Thorazine.*
Here's the data. In a study conducted by the NIMH, 62 percent
of first-episode psychotic patients admitted to Warren State Hospi¬
tal in Pennsylvania from 1946 to 1950 were discharged within
twelve months. At the end of three years, 73 percent were out of the
hospital. 4 A study of 216 schizophrenia patients admitted to Dela¬
ware State Hospital from 1948 to 1950 produced similar results.
Eighty-five percent were discharged within five years, and on January
1, 1956 —six years or more after initial hospitalization —70 percent
were successfully living in the community. 5 Meanwhile, Hillside
Hospital in Queens, New York, tracked 87 schizophrenia patients
discharged in 1950 and determined that slightly more than half
never relapsed in the next four years. 6 During this period, outcomes
studies in England, where schizophrenia was more narrowly defined,
painted a similarly encouraging picture: Thirty-three percent of the
patients enjoyed a "complete recovery," and another 20 percent a
"social recovery," which meant they could support themselves and
live independently. 7
These studies provide a rather startling view of schizophrenia
outcomes during this time. According to the conventional wisdom,
it was Thorazine that made it possible for people with schizophre¬
nia to live in the community. But what we find is that the majority
of people admitted for a first episode of schizophrenia during the
late 1940s and early 1950s recovered to the point that within the
first twelve months, they could return to the community. By the end
of three years, that was true for 75 percent of the patients. Only a
small percentage —20 percent or so —needed to be continuously
hospitalized. Moreover, those returning to the community weren't
* During this period, schizophrenia was a diagnosis being broadly applied to
those being hospitalized. Many of these patients would be diagnosed as bipolar
or schizoaffective today. Still, this was the diagnosis for the most "seriously
disturbed" people in American society at that time.
A PARADOX REVEALED
93
living in shelters and group homes, as facilities of that sort didn't yet
exist. They were not receiving federal disability payments, as the SSI
and SSDI programs had yet to be established. Those discharged
from hospitals were mostly returning to their families, and judging
by the social recovery data, many were working. All in all, there
was reason for people diagnosed with schizophrenia during that
postwar period to be optimistic that they could get better and func¬
tion fairly well in the community.
It is also important to note that the arrival of Thorazine did not
improve discharge rates in the 1950s for people newly diagnosed
with schizophrenia, nor did its arrival trigger the release of chronic
patients. In 1961, the California Department of Mental Hygiene re¬
ported on discharge rates for all 1,413 first-episode schizophrenia
patients hospitalized in 1956, and it found that 88 percent of those
who weren't prescribed a neuroleptic were discharged within eigh¬
teen months. Those treated with a neuroleptic —about half of the
1,413 patients —had a lower discharge rate; only 74 percent were
discharged within eighteen months. This is the only large-scale study
from the 1950s that compared discharge rates for first-episode pa¬
tients treated with and without drugs, and the investigators con¬
cluded that "drug-treated patients tend to have longer periods of
hospitalization. . . . The untreated patients consistently show a
somewhat lower retention rate." 8
The discharge of chronic schizophrenia patients from state men¬
tal hospitals —and thus the beginning of deinstitutionalization —got
under way in 1965 with the enactment of Medicare and Medicaid.
In 1955, there were 267,000 schizophrenia patients in state and
county mental hospitals, and eight years later, this number had
barely budged. There were still 253,000 schizophrenics residing in
the hospitals. 9 But then the economics of caring for the mentally ill
changed. The 1965 Medicare and Medicaid legislation provided
federal subsidies for nursing home care but no such subsidy for care
in state mental hospitals, and so the states, seeking to save money,
naturally began shipping their chronic patients to nursing homes.
That was when the census in state mental hospitals began to notice¬
ably drop, rather than in 1955, when Thorazine was introduced.
Unfortunately, our societal belief that it was this medication that
94
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
emptied the asylums, which is so central to the "psychopharma-
cology revolution" narrative, is belied by the hospital census data.
Through a Lens Darkly
In 1955, pharmaceutical companies were not required to prove to
the FDA that their new drugs were effective (that requirement was
added in 1962), and thus it fell to the NIMH to assess the merits of
Thorazine and the other new "wonder drugs" coming to market.
Much to its credit, the NIMH organized a conference in September
1956 to "consider carefully the entire psychotropic question," and
ultimately the conversation at the conference focused on a very
particular question: How could psychiatry adapt, for its own use,
a scientific tool that had recently proven its worth in infectious
medicine: the placebo-controlled, double-blind, randomized clinical
trial? 10
As many speakers noted, this tool wasn't particularly well suited
for assessing outcomes of a psychiatric drug. How could a study of
a neuroleptic possibly be "double-blind"? The psychiatrist would
quickly see who was on the drug and who was not, and any patient
given Thorazine would know he was on a medication as well. Then
there was the problem of diagnosis: How would a researcher know
if the patients randomized into a trial really had "schizophrenia”?
The diagnostic boundaries of mental disorders were forever chang¬
ing. Equally problematic, what defined a "good outcome"? Psychi¬
atrists and hospital staff might want to see drug-induced behavioral
changes that made the patient "more socially acceptable” but
weren't to the "ultimate benefit of the patient," said one conference
speaker." And how could outcomes be measured? In a study of a
drug for a known disease, mortality rates or laboratory results
could serve as objective measures of whether a treatment worked.
For instance, to test whether a drug for tuberculosis was effective,
an X-ray of the lung could show whether the bacillus that caused
the disease was gone. What would be the measurable endpoint in a
A PARADOX REVEALED
95
trial of a drug for schizophrenia? The problem, said NIMH
physician Edward Evarts at the conference, was that "the goals of
therapy in schizophrenia, short of getting the patient 'well,' have
not been clearly defined ." 12
All of these questions bedeviled psychiatry, and yet the NIMH, in
the wake of that conference, made plans to mount a trial of the
neuroleptics. The push of history was simply too great. This was the
scientific method now used in internal medicine to assess the merits
of a therapy, and Congress had created the NIMH with the thought
that it would transform psychiatry into a more modern, scientific
discipline. Psychiatry's adoption of this tool would prove that it was
moving toward that goal. The NIMH established a Psychopharma¬
cology Service Center to head up this effort, and Jonathan Cole, a
psychiatrist from the National Research Council, was named its
director.
Over the next couple of years, Cole and the rest of psychiatry set¬
tled on a trial design for testing psychotropic drugs. Psychiatrists
and nurses would use "rating scales" to measure numerically the
characteristic symptoms of the disease that was to be studied. Did a
drug for schizophrenia reduce the patient's "anxiety"? His or her
"grandiosity”? "Hostility”? "Suspiciousness"? "Unusual thought
content"? "Uncooperativeness"? The severity of all of those symp¬
toms would be measured on a numerical scale and a total
"symptom” score tabulated, and a drug would be deemed effective
if it reduced the total score significantly more than a placebo did
within a six-week period.
At least in theory, psychiatry now had a way to conduct trials of
psychiatric drugs that would produce an "objective” result. Yet the
adoption of this assessment put psychiatry on a very particular
path: The field would now see short-term reduction of symptoms as
evidence of a drug's efficacy. Much as a physician in internal medi¬
cine would prescribe an antibiotic for a bacterial infection, a psychi¬
atrist would prescribe a pill that knocked down a "target symptom”
of a "discrete disease.” The six-week "clinical trial” would prove
that this was the right thing to do. However, this tool wouldn't pro¬
vide any insight into how patients were faring over the long term.
96
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Were they able to work? Were they enjoying life? Did they have
friends? Were they getting married? None of those questions would
be answered.
This was the moment that magic-bullet medicine shaped psychia¬
try's future. The use of the clinical trial would cause psychiatrists to
see their therapies through a very particular prism, and even at the
1956 conference, New York State Psychiatric Institute researcher
Joseph Zubin warned that when it came to evaluating a therapy for
a psychiatric disorder, a six-week study induced a kind of scientific
myopia. "It would be foolhardy to claim a definite advantage for a
specified therapy without a two- to five-year follow-up,” he said.
"A two-year follow-up would seem to be the very minimum for the
long-term effects." 13
The Case for Neuroleptics
The Psychopharmacology Service Center launched its nine-hospital
trial of neuroleptics in 1961, and this is the study that marks the be¬
ginning of the scientific record that serves today as the "evidence
base" for these drugs. In the six-week trial, 2 70 patients were given
Thorazine or another neuroleptic (which were also known as "phe-
nothiazines,”) while the remaining 74 were put on a placebo. The
neuroleptics did help reduce some target symptoms —unrealistic
thinking, anxiety, suspiciousness, auditory hallucinations, etc. —bet¬
ter than the placebo, and thus, according to the rating's scales cu¬
mulative score, they were effective. Furthermore, the psychiatrists in
the study judged 75 percent of the drug-treated patients to be
"much improved” or "very much .improved," versus 23 percent of
the placebo patients.
After that, hundreds of smaller trials produced similar results, and
thus the evidence that these drugs reduce symptoms over the short
term better than placebo is fairly robust.* In 1977, Ross Baldessarini
* In 2007, the Cochrane Collaboration, an international group of scientists
that doesn't take funding from pharmaceutical companies, raised questions
A PARADOX REVEALED
97
at Harvard Medical School reviewed 149 such trials and found that
the antipsychotic drug proved superior to a placebo in 83 percent of
them. 14 The "Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale” (BPRS) was regularly
employed in such trials, and the American Psychiatric Association
eventually decided that a 20 percent reduction in total BPRS score
represented a clinically significant response to a drug. 15 Based on this
measurement, an estimated 70 percent of all schizophrenia patients
suffering from an acute episode of psychosis "respond,” over a six-
week period, to an antipsychotic medication.
Once the NIMH investigators determined that the antipsychotics
were efficacious over the short term, they naturally wanted to know
how long schizophrenia patients should stay on the medication. To
investigate this question, they ran studies that, for the most part, had
this design: Patients who were good responders to the medication
were either maintained on the drug or abruptly withdrawn from it.
In 1995, Patricia Gilbert at the University of California at San Diego
reviewed sixty-six relapse studies, involving 4,365 patients, and she
found that 53 percent of the drug-withdrawn patients relapsed
within ten months versus 16 percent of those maintained on the
medications. "The efficacy of these medications in reducing the risk
of psychotic relapse has been well documented," she concluded.* 16
This is the scientific evidence that supports the use of anti¬
psychotic medications for schizophrenia, both in the hospital and
long-term. As John Geddes, a prominent British researcher, wrote in
about this short-term efficacy record. They conducted a meta-analysis of all
chlorpromazine-versus-placebo studies in the scientific literature, and after
identifying fifty of decent quality, they concluded that the advantage of drug
over placebo was smaller than commonly thought. They calculated that seven
patients had to be treated with chlorpromazine to produce a net gain of one
"global improvement," and that "even this finding may be an overestimate of
the positive and an understimate of the negative effects of giving chlorpro¬
mazine." The Cochrane investigators, somewhat startled by their results,
wrote that "reliable evidence about [chlorpromazine's] short-term efficacy is
surprisingly weak."
* There is an evident flaw with Gilbert's meta-analysis. She didn't determine
whether the speed with which drugs were withdrawn affected the relapse rate.
98
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
a 2002 article in the New England Journal of Medicine, "Anti¬
psychotic drugs are effective in treating acute psychotic symptoms
and preventing relapse.” 17 Still, as many investigators have noted,
there is a hole in this evidence base, and it's the very hole that Zubin
predicted would arise. "Little can be said about the efficacy and ef¬
fectiveness of conventional antipsychotics on nonclinical outcomes,"
confessed Lisa Dixon and other psychiatrists at the University of
Maryland School of Medicine in 1995. "Well-designed long-term
studies are virtually nonexistent, so the longitudinal impact of treat¬
ment with conventional antipsychotics is unclear." 18
This doubt prompted an extraordinary 2002 editorial in Euro¬
pean Psychiatry, penned by Emmanuel Stip, a professor of psychia¬
try at the Universite de Montreal. "After fifty years of neuroleptics,
are we able to answer the following simple question: Are neuro¬
leptics effective in treating schizophrenia?" There was, he said, "no
compelling evidence on the matter, when 'long-term' is consid¬
ered." 19
A Conundrum Appears
Although Dixon's and Stip's comments suggest that there is no long¬
term data to be reviewed, it is in fact possible to piece together a
story of how antipsychotics alter the course of schizophrenia, and
this story begins, quite appropriately, with the NIMH's follow-up
study of the 344 patients in its initial nine-hospital trial. In some
ways, the patients —regardless of what treatment they had received
in the hospital —were not faring so badly. At the end of one year,
After her study appeared, Adele Viguera at Harvard Medical School reanalyzed
the same sixty-six studies and determined that when the drugs were gradually
withdrawn, the relapse rate was only one-third as high as in the abrupt-
withdrawal studies. The abrupt-withdrawal design in the majority of the re¬
lapse studies dramatically increased the risk that the schizophrenia patients
would become sick again. Indeed, the relapse rate for gradually withdrawn pa¬
tients was similar to what it was for the drug-maintained patients.
A PARADOX REVEALED
99
254 were living in the community, and 58 percent of those who —
according to their age and gender —could be expected to work were
in fact employed. Two-thirds of the "housewives" were functioning
OK in that domestic role. Although the researchers didn't report on
the medication use of patients during the one-year follow-up, they
were startled to discover that "patients who received placebo treat¬
ment [in the six-week trial] were less likely to be rehospitalized than
those who received any of the three active phenothiazines." 20
Here, at this very first moment in the scientific literature, there
is the hint of a paradox: While the drugs were effective over the short
term, perhaps they made people more vulnerable to psychosis over
the long term, and thus the higher rehospitalization rates for drug-
treated patients at the end of one year. Soon, NIMH investigators
were back with another surprising result. In two drug withdrawal
trials, both of which included patients who weren't on any drug at
the start of the study, relapse rates rose in correlation with drug
dosage. Only 7 percent of those who had been on a placebo at the
start of the study replapsed, compared to 65 percent of those taking
more than five hundred milligrams of chlorpromazine before the
drug was withdrawn. "Relapse was found to be significantly related
to the dose of the tranquilizing medication the patient was receiving
before he was put on placebo —the higher the dose, the greater the
probability of relapse,” the researchers wrote. 21
Something was amiss, and clinical observations deepened the sus¬
picion. Schizophrenia patients discharged on medications were re¬
turning to psychiatric emergency rooms in such droves that hospital
staff dubbed it the "revolving door syndrome.” Even when patients
reliably took their medications, relapse was common, and re¬
searchers observed that "relapse is greater in severity during drug
administration than when no drugs are given." 22 At the same time,
if patients relapsed after quitting the medications, Cole noted, their
psychotic symptoms tended to "persist and intensify," and, at least
for a time, they suffered from a host of new symptoms as well: nau¬
sea, vomiting, diarrhea, agitation, insomnia, headaches, and weird
motor tics. 23 Initial exposure to a neuroleptic seemed to be setting
patients up for a future of severe psychotic episodes, and that was
true regardless of whether they stayed on the medications.
loo
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
These poor results prompted two psychiatrists at Boston Psycho¬
pathic Hospital, J. Sanbourne Bockoven and Harry Solomon, to
revisit the past. They had been at the hospital for decades, and in
the period after World War II ended, when they treated psychotic
patients with a progressive form of psychological care, they had
seen the majority regularly improve. That led them to believe that
"the majority of mental illnesses, especially the most severe, are
largely self-limiting in nature if the patient is not subjected to a
demeaning experience or loss of rights and liberties." The anti-
psychotics, they reasoned, should speed up this natural healing
process. But were the drugs improving long-term outcomes? In a
retrospective study, they found that 45 percent of the patients
treated in 1947 at their hospital hadn't relapsed in the next five
years and that 76 percent were successfully living in the community
at the end of that follow-up period. In contrast, only 31 percent of
the patients treated at the hospital in 1967 with neuroleptics
remained relapse-free for five years, and as a group they were much
more "socially dependent" —on welfare and needing other forms of
support. "Rather unexpectedly, these data suggest that psy¬
chotropic drugs may not be indispensable," Bockoven and Solomon
wrote. "Their extended use in aftercare may prolong the social
dependency of many discharged patients." 24
With debate over the merits of neuroleptics rising, the NIMH
funded three studies during the 1970s that reexamined whether
schizophrenia patients —and in particular those suffering a first
episode of schizophrenia —could be successfully treated without
medications. In the first study, which was conducted by William
Carpenter and Thomas McGlashan at the NIMH's clinical research
facility in Bethesda, Maryland, those treated without drugs were
discharged sooner than the drug-treated patients, and only 35 per¬
cent of the nonmedicated group relapsed within a year after dis¬
charge, compared to 45 percent of the medicated group. The
off-drug patients also suffered less from depression, blunted emo¬
tions, and retarded movements. Indeed, they told Carpenter and
McGlashan that they had found it "gratifying and informative” to
have gone through their psychotic episodes without having their
feelings numbed by the drugs. Medicated patients didn't have that
A PARADOX REVEALED
IOI
same learning experience, and as a result, Carpenter and Mc-
Glashan concluded, over the long term they "are less able to cope
with subsequent life stresses." 25
A year later, Maurice Rappaport at the University of California
in San Francisco announced results that told the same story, only
more strongly so. He had randomized eighty young newly diag¬
nosed male schizophrenics admitted to Agnews State Hospital into
drug and non-drug groups, and although symptoms abated more
quickly in those treated with antipsychotics, both groups, on aver¬
age, stayed only six weeks in the hospital. Rappaport followed the
patients for three years, and it was those who weren't treated with
antipsychotics in the hospital and who stayed off the drugs after
discharge that had —by far —the best outcomes. Only two of the
twenty-four patients in this never-exposed-to-antipsychotics group
relapsed during the three-year follow-up. Meanwhile, the patients
that arguably fared the worst were those on drugs throughout the
study. The very standard of care that, according to psychiatry's "ev¬
idence base,” was supposed to produce the best outcomes had
instead produced the worst.
"Our findings suggest that antipsychotic medication is not the
treatment of choice, at least for certain patients, if one is interested
in long-term clinical improvement,” Rappaport wrote. "Many
unmedicated-while-in-hospital patients showed greater long-term
Rappaport's Study: Three-Year Schizophrenia Outcomes
Medication Use
(In hospital/
after discharge)
Number of Patients
Severity Illness Scale
(1 = best outcome;
7 = worst outcome)
Rehospitalization
Placebo/off
24
1.70
8%
Antipsychotic/off
1 7
2.79
4 7%
Placebo/on
1 7
3.54
5 3 %
Antipsychotic/on
22
3.51
7 3 %
In this study, patients were grouped according to both their in-hospital care (placebo or drug)
and whether they used antipsychotics after they were discharged. Thus, 24 ofthe 41 patients
treated with placebo in the hospital remained offthe drugs during the follow-up period.This
never-exposed group had the best outcomes by far. Rappaport, M."Are there schizophrenics for
whom drugs may be unnecessary or contraindicated." International Pharmacopsychiatry 13
(1 978): 1 00-1 1._
102
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
improvement, less pathology at follow-up, fewer rehospitalizations,
and better overall functioning in the community than patients who
were given chlorpromazine while in the hospital." 26
The third study was led by Loren Mosher, head of schizophrenia
studies at the NIMH. Although he may have been the nation's top
schizophrenia doctor at the time, his vision of the illness was at
odds with many of his peers, who had come to think that schizo¬
phrenics suffered from a "broken brain." He believed that psy¬
chosis could arise in response to emotional and inner trauma and, in
its own way, could be a coping mechanism. As such, he believed
there was the possibility that people could grapple with their hallu¬
cinations and delusions, struggle through a schizophrenic break,
and regain their sanity. And if that was so, he reasoned that if he
provided newly psychotic patients with a safe house, one staffed by
people who had an evident empathy for others and who wouldn't
be frightened by strange behavior, many would get well, even
though they weren't treated with antipsychotics. "I thought that
sincere human involvement and understanding were critical to heal¬
ing interactions," he said. "The idea was to treat people as people,
as human beings, with dignity and respect.”
The twelve-room Victorian house he opened in Santa Clara,
California, in 1971 could shelter six patients at a time. He called it
Soteria House, and eventually he started a second home as well,
Emanon. All told, the Soteria Project ran for twelve years, with
eighty-two patients treated at the two homes. As early as 1974,
Mosher began reporting that his Soteria patients were faring better
than a matched cohort of patients being treated conventionally
with drugs in a hospital, and in 1979, he announced his two-year
results. At the end of six weeks, psychotic symptoms had abated as
much in his Soteria patients as in the hospitalized patients, and at
the end of two years, the Soteria patients had "lower psycho¬
pathology scores, fewer [hospital] readmissions, and better global
adjustment." 22 Later, he and John Bola, an assistant professor at the
University of Southern California, reported on their medication use:
Forty-two percent of the Soteria patients had never been exposed
to drugs, 39 percent had used them on a temporary basis, and only
19 percent had needed them throughout the two-year follow-up.
A PARADOX REVEALED
103
"Contrary to popular views, minimal use of antipsychotic med¬
ications combined with specially designed psychosocial intervention
for patients newly identified with schizophrenia spectrum disorder
is not harmful but appears to be advantageous,” Mosher and Bola
wrote. "We think that the balance of risks and benefits associated
with the common practice of medicating nearly all early episodes of
psychosis should be re-examined." 28
Three NIMH-funded studies, and all pointed to the same conclu¬
sion.* Perhaps 50 percent of newly diagnosed schizophrenia pa¬
tients, if treated without antipsychotics, would recover and stay
well through lengthy follow-up periods. Only a minority of patients
seemed to need to take the drugs continuously. The "revolving
door" syndrome that had become so familiar was due in large part
to the drugs, even though, in clinical trials, the drugs had proven to
be effective in knocking down psychotic symptoms. Carpenter and
McGlashan neatly summarized the scientific conundrum that psy¬
chiatry now faced:
There is no question that, once patients are placed on medica¬
tion, they are less vulnerable to relapse if maintained on
neuroleptics. But what if these patients had never been treated
with drugs to begin with? . . . We raise the possibility that
* In the early 1960s, Philip May conducted a study that compared five forms of
in-hospital treatment: drug, electroconvulsive therapy (ECT), psychotherapy,
psychotherapy plus drug, and milieu therapy (a supportive environment). Over
the short term, the drug-treated patients did much better. As a result, the study
came to be cited as proof that schizophrenia patients could not be treated with¬
out drugs. However, the two-year results told a more nuanced story. Fifty-nine
percent of patients initially treated with milieu therapy but no drugs were suc¬
cessfully discharged in the initial study period, and this group "functioned over
the follow-up at least as well, if not better, than the successes from the other
treatments." Thus, the May study, which is usually cited as proving that all
psychotic patients should be medicated, in fact suggested that a majority of
first-episode patients would fare best over the long term if initially treated with
milieu therapy rather than drugs. Source: P. May, "Schizophrenia: a follow-up
study of the results of five forms of treatment," Archives of General Psychiatry
38 (1981): 776-84.
104
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
antipsychotic medication may make some schizophrenic pa¬
tients more vulnerable to future relapse than would be the
case in the natural course of the illness. 29
And if that was so, these drugs were increasing the likelihood
that a person who suffered a psychotic break would become chron¬
ically ill.
A Cure Worse Than the Disease?
All drugs have a risk-benefit profile, and the usual thought within
medicine is that a drug should provide a benefit that outweighs the
risks. A drug that curbs psychotic symptoms clearly provides a
marked benefit, and that was why antipsychotics could be viewed as
helpful even though the list of negatives with these drugs was a long
one. Thorazine and other first-generation neuroleptics caused
Parkinsonian symptoms and extraordinarily painful muscle spasms.
Patients regularly complained that the drugs turned them into emo¬
tional "zombies.” In 1972, researchers concluded that neuroleptics
"impaired learning." 30 Others reported that even if medicated pa¬
tients stayed out of the hospital, they seemed totally unmotivated
and socially disengaged. Many lived in "virtual solitude” in group
homes, spending most of the time "staring vacantly at television,"
wrote one investigator. 31 None of this told of medicated schizophre¬
nia patients faring well, and here was the quandary that psychiatry
now faced: If the drugs increased relapse rates over the long term,
then where was the benefit? This question was made all the more
pressing by the fact that many patients maintained on the drugs
were developing tardive dyskinesia (TD), a gross motor dysfunction
that remained even after the drugs were withdrawn, evidence of per¬
manent brain damage.
All of this required psychiatry to recalculate the risks and benefits
of antipsychotics, and in 1977 Jonathan Cole did so in an article
provocatively titled "Is the Cure Worse Than the Disease?” He
reviewed all of the long-term harm the drugs could cause and
A PARADOX REVEALED
• 105
observed that studies had shown that at least 50 percent of all schiz¬
ophrenia patients could fare well without the drugs. There was only
one moral thing for psychiatry to do: "Every schizophrenic outpa¬
tient maintained on antipsychotic medication should have the bene¬
fit of an adequate trial without drugs.” This, he explained, would
save many "from the dangers of tardive dyskinesia as well as the
financial and social burdens of prolonged drug therapy." 32
The evidence base for maintaining schizophrenia patients on
antipsychotics had collapsed. "Are the antipsychotics to be with¬
drawn?” asked Pierre Deniker, the French psychiatrist who, in the
early 1950s, had first promoted their use. 33
Supersensitivity Psychosis
In the late 1970s, two physicians at McGill University, Guy
Chouinard and Barry Jones, stepped forward with a biological ex¬
planation for why the drugs made schizophrenia patients more
biologically vulnerable to psychosis. Their understanding arose, in
large part, from the investigations into the dopamine hypothesis of
schizophrenia, which had detailed how the drugs perturbed this
neurotransmitter system.
Thorazine and other standard antipsychotics block 70 to 90 per¬
cent of all D, receptors in the brain. In an effort to compensate for
this blockade, the postsynaptic neurons increase the density of their
D= receptors by 30 percent or more. The brain is now "supersensi¬
tive" to dopamine, Chouinard and Jones explained, and this neuro¬
transmitter is thought to be a mediator of psychosis. "Neuroleptics
can produce a dopamine supersensitivity that leads to both dyski-
netic and psychotic symptoms," they wrote. "An implication is that
the tendency toward psychotic relapse in a patient who has devel¬
oped such a supersensitivity is determined by more than just the
normal course of the illness." 31
A simple metaphor can help us better understand this drug-induced
biological vulnerability to psychosis and why it flares up when the
drug is withdrawn. Neuroleptics put a brake on dopamine trans-
io6
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
mission, and in response the brain puts down the dopamine acceler¬
ator (the extra Dj receptors). If the drug is abruptly withdrawn, the
brake on dopamine is suddenly released while the accelerator is still
pressed to the floor. The system is now wildly out of balance, and
just as a car might careen out of control, so too the dopaminergic
pathways in the brain. The dopaminergic neurons in the basal
ganglia may fire so rapidly that the patient withdrawing from the
drugs suffers weird tics, agitation, and other motor abnormalities.
The same out-of-control firing is happening with the dopaminergic
pathway to the limbic region, and that may lead to "psychotic
relapse or deterioration,” Chouinard and Jones wrote. 35
This was an extraordinary piece of scientific detective work by
the two Canadian investigators. They had —at least in theory —
identified the reason that relapse rates were so high in the medication-
withdrawal trials, which psychiatry had mistakenly interpreted as
proving that the drugs prevented relapse. The severe relapse suffered
by many patients withdrawn from antipsychotics was not necessar¬
ily the result of the "disease" returning, but rather was drug-related.
Chouinard and Jones's work also revealed that both psychiatrists
and their patients would regularly suffer from a clinical delusion:
They would see the return of psychotic symptoms upon drug with¬
drawal as proof that the antipsychotic was necessary and that it
"worked.” The relapsed patient would then go back on the drug and
often the psychosis would abate, which would be further proof that
it worked. Both doctor and patient would experience this to be
"true,” and yet, in fact, the reason that the psychosis abated with the
return of the drug was that the brake on dopamine transmission was
being reapplied, which countered the stuck dopamine accelerator. As
Chouinard and Jones explained: "The need for continued neurolep¬
tic treatment may itself be drug-induced.”
In short, initial exposure to neuroleptics put patients onto a path
where they would likely need the drugs for life. Yet—and this was
the second haunting aspect to this story of medicine —staying on the
drugs regularly led to a bad end. Over time, Chouinard and Jones
noted, the dopaminergic pathways tended to become permanently
dysfunctional. They became irreversibly stuck in a hyperactive state,
A PARADOX REVEALED
• 107
and soon the patient's tongue was slipping rhythmically in and out
of his mouth (tardive dyskinesia) and psychotic symptoms were
worsening (tardive psychosis). Doctors would then need to pre¬
scribe higher doses of antipsychotics to tamp down those tardive
symptoms. "The most efficacious treatment is the causative agent it¬
self, the neuroleptic,” Chouinard and Jones said.
Over the next few years, Chouinard and Jones continued to flesh
out and test their hypothesis. In 1982, they reported that 30 percent
of 216 schizophrenia outpatients they studied showed signs of tar¬
dive psychosis. 36 They also observed that it tended to afflict those
patients who, at initial diagnosis, had a "good prognosis," and thus
would have had a chance to fare well over the long term if they had
never been exposed to neuroleptics. These were the "placebo re¬
sponders" who had fared best in the studies conducted by Rappa-
port and Mosher, and now Chouinard and Jones were reporting
that they were becoming chronically psychotic after years of taking
antipsychotics. Finally, Chouinard quantified the risk, reporting that
tardive psychosis seemed to develop at a slightly slower rate than
tardive dyskinesia. It afflicted 3 percent of patients a year, with the
result that after fifteen years on the drugs, perhaps 45 percent suf¬
fered from it. When tardive psychosis sets in, Chouinard added,
"the illness appears worse” than ever before. "New schizophrenic
or original symptoms of greater severity will appear." 37
Animal studies confirmed this picture too. Philip Seeman re¬
ported that antipsychotics caused an increase in Dz receptors in rats,
and while the density of these receptors could revert to normal if the
drug was withdrawn (he reported that for every month of exposure,
it took two months for renormalization to occur), at some point the
increase in receptors became irreversible. 38
In 1984, Swedish physician Lars Martensson, in a presentation at
the World Federation of Mental Health Conference in Copenhagen,
summed up the devastating bottom line. "The use of neuroleptics is
a trap," he said. "It is like having a psychosis-inducing agent built
into the brain." 39
io8
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
A Crazy Idea ... Or Not?
This was the view of neuroleptics that came together in the early
1980s, and it was a story of science at its best. Psychiatrists saw that
the drugs "worked.” They saw that antipsychotics knocked down
psychotic symptoms, and they observed that patients who stopped
taking their medications regularly became psychotic again. Scien¬
tific tests reinforced their clinical perceptions. Six-week trials proved
the drugs were effective. Relapse studies proved that patients should
be maintained on the drugs. Yet once researchers came to under¬
stand how the drugs acted on the brain, and once they began inves¬
tigating why patients were developing tardive dyskinesia and why
they were becoming so chronically ill, then this counterintuitive pic¬
ture of the drugs —that they were increasing the likelihood that pa¬
tients would become chronically ill —emerged. It was Chouinard
and Jones who explicitly connected all the dots, and for a time, their
work did stir up a hornet's nest within psychiatry. One physician, at
a meeting where the two McGill University doctors spoke, asked in
astonishment: "I put my patients on neuroleptics because they're
psychotic. Now you're saying that the same drug that controls their
schizophrenia also causes a psychosis?" 40
But what was psychiatry supposed to do with this information? It
clearly imperiled the field's very foundation. Could it really now
confess to the public, or even admit to itself, that the very class of
drugs said to have "revolutionized” the care of the mentally ill was
in fact making patients chronically ill? That antipsychotics made
patients —at least in the aggregate —more psychotic over time? Psy¬
chiatry desperately needed this discussion to go away. Soon the arti¬
cles by Chouinard and Jones on "supersensitivity psychosis” were
filed away in the "interesting hypothesis" category, and everyone in
the field breathed a sigh of relief when Solomon Snyder, who knew
as much about dopamine receptors as any scientist in the world, as¬
sured everyone in his 1986 book Drugs and the Brain that it had all
turned out to be a false alarm. "If dopamine receptor sensitivity is
greater in patients with tardive dyskinesia, one might wonder
whether they would also suffer a corresponding increase in schizo-
A PARADOX REVEALED
109
phrenia symptoms. Interestingly, though researchers have looked
carefully for any possible exacerbation of schizophrenic symptoms
in patients who begin to develop tardive dyskinesia, none has ever
been found." 41
That moment of crisis within psychiatry, when it briefly worried
about supersensitivity psychosis, occurred nearly thirty years ago,
and today the notion that antipsychotics increase the likelihood that
a person diagnosed with schizophrenia will become chronically ill
seems, on the face of it, absurd. Ask psychiatrists at top medical
schools, staff at a mental hospital, NIMH officials, leaders of the
National Alliance for the Mentally 111, science writers at major
newspapers, or the ordinary person in the street, and everyone will
attest that antipsychotics are essential for treating schizophrenia,
the very cornerstone of care, and that anyone who touts a different
idea is, well, a bit loony. Still, we started down this path of research,
I've invited readers into this loony bin, and so now we need to move
up one floor in the Countway Library. The volumes in the basement
end in 1986, and now we need to comb the scientific literature since
that date, and see what story it has to tell. Was it all a false
alarm ... or not?
The most efficient way to answer that question is to summarize,
one by one, the relevant studies and avenues of research.
The Vermont longitudinal study
In the late 1950s and early 1960s, Vermont State Hospital dis¬
charged 269 chronic schizophrenics, most of whom were middle-
aged, into the community. Twenty years later, Courtenay Harding
interviewed 168 patients from this cohort (those who were still
alive), and found that 34 percent were recovered, which meant they
were "asymptomatic and living independently, had close relation¬
ships, were employed or otherwise productive citizens, were able to
care for themselves, and led full lives in general." 42 This was a star¬
tling good long-term outcome for patients who had been seen as
hopeless in the 1950s, and those who had recovered, Harding told
the APA Monitor, had one thing in common: They all "had long
since stopped taking medications." 43 She concluded that it was a
no
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
"myth" that schizophrenia patients "must be on medication all
their lives," and that, in fact, "it may be a small percentage who
need medication indefinitely." 44
The World Health Organization cross-cultural studies
In 1969, the World Health Organization launched an effort to track
schizophrenia outcomes in nine countries. At the end of five years,
the patients in the three "developing” countries —India, Nigeria,
and Colombia —had a "considerably better course and outcome”
than patients in the United States and five other "developed coun¬
tries.” They were much more likely to be asymptomatic during the
follow-up period, and even more important, they enjoyed "an ex¬
ceptionally good social outcome.”
These findings stung the psychiatric community in the United
States and Europe, which protested that there must have been a de¬
sign flaw in the study. Perhaps the patients in India, Nigeria, and
Colombia had not really been schizophrenic. In response, WHO
launched a ten-country study in 1978, and this time they primarily
enrolled patients suffering from a first episode of schizophrenia, all
of whom were diagnosed by Western criteria. Once again, the re¬
sults were much the same. At the end of two years, nearly two-
thirds of the patients in the "developing countries" had had good
outcomes, and slightly more than one-third had become chronically
ill. In the rich countries, only 37 percent of the patients had good
outcomes, and 59 percent became chronically ill. "The findings of a
better outcome of patients in developing countries was confirmed,”
the WHO scientists wrote. "Being in a developed country was a
strong predictor of not attaining a complete remission." 45
Although the WHO investigators didn't identify a reason for the
stark disparity in outcomes, they had tracked antipsychotic usage in
the second study, having hypothesized that perhaps patients in the
poor countries fared better because they more reliably took their
medication. However, they found the opposite to be true. Only 16
percent of the patients in the poor countries were regularly main¬
tained on antipsychotics, versus 61 percent of the patients in the
rich countries. Moreover, in Agra, India, where patients arguably
A PARADOX REVEALED • III
fared the best, only 3 percent of the patients were kept on an
antipsychotic. Medication usage was highest in Moscow, and that
city had the highest percentage of patients who were constantly ill. 46
In this cross-cultural study, the best outcomes were clearly associ¬
ated with low medication use. Later, in 1997, WHO researchers in¬
terviewed the patients from the first two studies once again (fifteen
to twenty-five years after the initial studies), and they found that
those in the poor countries continued to do much better. The "out¬
come differential” held up for "general clinical state, symptomatol¬
ogy, disability, and social functioning.” In the developing countries,
53 percent of the schizophrenia patients were simply "never psy¬
chotic” anymore, and 73 percent were employed. 47 Although the
WHO investigators didn't report on medication usage in their
follow-up study, the bottom line is clear: In countries where patients
hadn't been regularly maintained on antipsychotics earlier in their
illness, the majority had recovered and were doing well fifteen years
later.
Tardive dyskinesia and global decline
Tardive dyskinesia and tardive psychosis occur because the
dopaminergic pathways to the basal ganglia and limbic system be¬
come dysfunctional. But there are three dopaminergic pathways,
and so it stands to reason that the third one, which transmits mes¬
sages to the frontal lobes, also becomes dysfunctional over time. If
so, researchers could expect to find a global decline in brain func¬
tion in patients diagnosed with tardive dyskinesia, and from 1979
to 2000, more than two dozen studies found that to be the case.
"The relationship appears to be linear,” reported Medical College
of Virginia psychiatrist James Wade in 1987. "Individuals with se¬
vere forms of the disorder are most impaired cognitively." 48 Re¬
searchers determined that tardive dyskinesia was associated with a
worsening of the negative symptoms of schizophrenia (emotional
disengagement); psychosocial impairment; and a decline in memory,
visual retention, and the capacity to learn. People with TD lose their
"road map of consciousness," concluded one investigator. 49 Investi¬
gators have dubbed this long-term cognitive deterioration tardive
112
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
dementia; in 1994, researchers found that three-fourths of med¬
icated schizophrenia patients seventy years and older suffer from a
brain pathology associated with Alzheimer's disease. 50
MRI studies
The invention of magnetic resonance imaging technology provided
researchers with the opportunity to measure volumes of brain struc¬
tures in people diagnosed with schizophrenia, and while they hoped
to identify abnormalities that might characterize the illness, they
ended up documenting instead the effect of antipsychotics on brain
volumes. In a series of studies from 1994 to 1998, investigators re¬
ported that the drugs caused basal ganglion structures and the thal¬
amus to swell, and the frontal lobes to shrink, with these changes in
volumes "dose related." 51 Then, in 1998, Raquel Gur at the Univer¬
sity of Pennsylvania Medical Center reported that the swelling of
the basal ganglia and thalamus was "associated with greater sever¬
ity of both negative and positive symptoms." 52
This last study provided a very clear picture of an iatrogenic
process. The antipsychotic causes a change in brain volumes, and as
this occurs, the patient becomes more psychotic (known as the
"positive symptoms" of schizophrenia) and more emotionally dis¬
engaged ("negative symptoms"). The MRI studies showed that
antipsychotics worsen the very symptoms they are supposed to
treat, and that this worsening begins to occur during the first three
years that patients are on the drugs.
Modeling psychosis
As part of their investigations of schizophrenia, researchers have
sought to develop biological "models" of psychosis, and one way
they have done that is to study the brain changes induced by various
drugs —amphetamines, angel dust, etc. —that can trigger delusions
and hallucinations. They also have developed ways to induce
psychotic-like behaviors in rats and other animals. Lesions to the
hippocampus can cause such disturbed behaviors; certain genes can
be "knocked out" to produce such symptoms. In 2005, Philip
A PARADOX REVEALED
113
Seeman reported that all of these psychotic triggers cause an in¬
crease in D 2 receptors in the brain that have a "HIGH affinity” for
dopamine, and by that, he meant that the receptors bound quite
easily with the neurotransmitter. These "results imply that there
may be many pathways to psychosis, including multiple gene muta¬
tions, drug abuse, or brain injury, all of which may converge via D,
HIGH to elicit psychotic symptoms," he wrote. 53
Seeman reasoned that this is why antipsychotics work: They
block D 2 receptors. But in his research, he also found that these
drugs, including the newer ones like Zyprexa and Risperdal, double
the density of "high affinity” Di receptors. They induce the same
abnormality that angel dust does, and thus this research confirms
what Lars Martensson observed in 1984: Taking a neuroleptic is
like having a "psychosis inducing agent built into the brain."
Nancy Andreasen’s longitudinal MRI study
In 1989, Nancy Andreasen, a psychiatry professor at the University
of Iowa who was editor in chief of the American Journal of Psychi¬
atry from 1993 to 2005, began a long-term study of more than five
hundred schizophrenia patients. In 2003, she reported that at the
time of initial diagnosis, the patients had slightly smaller frontal
lobes than normal, and that over the next three years, their frontal
lobes continued to shrink. Furthermore, this "progressive reduction
in frontal lobe white matter volume” was associated with a worsen¬
ing of negative symptoms and functional impairment, and thus An¬
dreasen concluded that this shrinkage is evidence that schizophrenia
is a "progressive neurodevelopmental disorder," one which antipsy¬
chotics unfortunately fail to arrest. "The medications currently used
cannot modify an injurious process occurring in the brain, which is
the underlying basis of symptoms." 54
Hers was a picture of antipsychotics as therapeutically ineffec¬
tive, rather than harmful, and two years later, she fleshed out this
picture. Her patients' cognitive abilities began to "worsen signifi¬
cantly” five years after initial diagnosis, a decline tied to the "pro¬
gressive brain volume reductions after illness onset." 55 In other
words, as her patients' frontal lobes shrank in size, their ability
114
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
to think declined. But other researchers conducting MRI studies
had found that the shrinkage of the frontal lobes was drug-related,
and in a 2008 interview with the New York Titties, Andreasen con¬
ceded that the "more drugs you've been given, the more brain tissue
you lose.” The shrinkage of the frontal lobes may be part of a
disease process, which the drugs then exacerbate. "What exactly do
these drugs do?” Andreasen said. "They block basal ganglia activ¬
ity. The prefrontal cortex doesn't get the input it needs and is being
shut down by drugs. That reduces the psychotic symptoms. It also
causes the prefrontal cortex to slowly atrophy.” 56
Once again, Andreasen's investigations revealed an iatrogenic
process at work. The drugs block dopamine activity in the brain
and this leads to brain shrinkage, which in turn correlates with a
worsening of negative symptoms and cognitive impairment. This
was yet another disturbing finding, and it prompted Yale psychia¬
trist Thomas McGlashan, who three decades earlier had wondered
whether antipsychotics were making patients "more biologically
vulnerable to psychosis," to once again question this entire para¬
digm of care. He put his troubled thoughts into a scientific context:
In the short term, acute D 2 [receptor] blockade detaches
salience and the patient's investment in positive symptoms. In
the long term, chronic D 2 blockade dampens salience for all
events in everyday life, inducing a chemical anhedonia that is
sometimes labeled postpsychotic depression or neuroleptic
dysphoria. . . . Do we free patients from the asylum with D 2
blocking agents only to block incentive, engagement with the
world, and the joie de vivre of everyday life? Medication can
be lifesaving in a crisis, but it may render the patient more
psychosis-prone should it be stopped and more deficit-ridden
should it be maintained. 57
His comments appeared in a 2006 issue of the Schizophrenia Bul¬
letin, and at that moment it seemed like the late 1970s all over
again. The "cure,” it seemed, had once again been proven to be
"worse than the disease."
A PARADOX REVEALED
11$
The Clinician's Illusion
I attended the 2008 meeting of the American Psychiatric Associa¬
tion for a number of reasons, but the person I most wanted to hear
speak was Martin Harrow, who is a psychologist at the University
of Illinois College of Medicine. From 1975 to 1983, he enrolled
sixty-four young schizophrenics in a long-term study funded by the
NIMH, recruiting the patients from two Chicago hospitals. One
was private and the other public, as this ensured that the group
would be economically diverse. Ever since then, he has been period¬
ically assessing how well they are doing. Are they symptomatic? In
recovery? Employed? Do they take antipsychotic medications? His
results provide an up-to-date look at how schizophrenia patients in
the United States are faring, and thus his study can bring our in¬
vestigation of the scientific literature to a fitting climax. If the con¬
ventional wisdom is to be believed, then those who stayed on
antipsychotics should have had better outcomes. If the scientific lit¬
erature we have just reviewed is to be believed, then it should be the
reverse.
Here is Harrow's data. In 2007, he published a report on the pa¬
tients' fifteen-year outcomes in the Journal of Nervous and Mental
Disease, and he further updated that review in his presentation at
the APA's 2008 meeting. 58 At the end of two years, the group not on
antipsychotics were doing slightly better on a "global assessment
scale” than the group on the drugs. Then, over the next thirty
months, the collective fates of the two groups began to dramatically
diverge. The off-med group began to improve significantly, and by
the end of 4.5 years, 39 percent were "in recovery" and more than
60 percent were working. In contrast, outcomes for the medication
group worsened during this thirty-month period. As a group, their
global functioning declined slightly, and at the 4.5-year mark, only
6 percent were in recovery and few were working. That stark diver¬
gence in outcomes remained for the next ten years. At the fifteen-
year follow-up, 40 percent of those off drugs were in recovery, more
than half were working, and only 28 percent suffered from psy¬
chotic symptoms. In contrast, only 5 percent of those taking anti-
I 1 6
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Long-term Recovery Rates for Schizophrenia Patients
50%
2 4.5 7.5 10 15
Year Year Year Year Year
Follow-up Follow-up Follow-up Follow-up Follow-up
Source: Harrow, M. "Factors involved in outcome and recovery in schizophrenia patients not on
antipsychotic medications." The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1 95 (2007): 406-1 4.
psychotics were in recovery, and 64 percent were actively psychotic.
"I conclude that patients with schizophrenia not on antipsychotic
medication for a long period of time have significantly better global
functioning than those on antipsychotics," Harrow told the APA
audience.
Indeed, it wasn't just that there were more recoveries in the un¬
medicated group. There were also fewer terrible outcomes in this
group. There was a shift in the entire spectrum of outcomes. Ten of
the twenty-five patients who stopped taking antipsychotics recov¬
ered, eleven had so-so outcomes, and only four (16 percent) had a
"uniformly poor outcome.” In contrast, only two of the thirty-nine
patients who stayed on antipsychotics recovered, eighteen had so-so
outcomes, and nineteen (49 percent) fell into the "uniformly poor”
camp. Medicated patients had one-eighth the recovery rate of un¬
medicated patients, and a threefold higher rate of faring miserably
over the long term.
This is the outcomes picture revealed in an NIMH-funded study,
A PARADOX REVEALED
• 117
Spectrum ofOutcomes in Schizophrenia Patients
On Antipsychotics
Off Antipsychotics
The spectrum ofoutcomes for medicated versus unmedicated patients. Those on antipsychotics
had a much lower recovery rate, and were much more likely to have a "uniformly poor" outcome.
Source: Harrow, M. "Factors involved in outcome and recovery in schizophrenia patients not on
antipsychotic medications." The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1 95 (2007): 406-14.
the most up-to-date one we have today. It also provides us with in¬
sight into how long it takes for the better outcomes for nonmedi-
cated patients, as a group, to become apparent. Although this
difference began to show up at the end of two years, it wasn't until
the 4.5-year mark that it became evident that the nonmedicated
group, as a whole, was doing much better. Furthermore, through his
rigorous tracking of patients, Harrow discovered why psychiatrists
remain blind to this fact. Those who got off their antipsychotic
medications left the system, he said. They stopped going to day pro¬
grams, they stopped seeing therapists, they stopped telling people
they had ever been diagnosed with schizophrenia, and they disap¬
peared into society. A few of the nonmedicated people in Harrow's
study even got "high-level jobs” —one became a college professor
and another a lawyer —and several had "mid-level jobs.” Explained
Harrow: "We [clinicians] get our experience from seeing those who
leave us, and then come back because they relapse. We don't see the
ones who don't relapse. They don't come back. They are quite
happy.”
Afterward, I asked Dr. Harrow why he thought the nonmedi¬
cated patients did so much better. He did not attribute it to their
being off antipsychotics, but rather said it was because this group
"had a stronger internal sense of self,” and once they initially stabi¬
lized on the medications, this "better personhood” gave them the
118 •
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
confidence to go off the drugs. "It's not that those who went off
medications did better, but rather it was those who did better [ini¬
tially] who then went off the medications.” When I pressed on with
a question about whether his findings supported a different inter¬
pretation, which was that the drugs worsened long-term outcomes,
he grew a bit testy. "That's a possibility, but I'm not advocating it,”
he said. "People recognize there may be side effects. . . . I'm not just
trying to avoid the question. I'm one of the few people in the field
without drug money."
I asked one last question. At the very least, shouldn't his findings
be worked into the paradigm of care used in our society to treat
those diagnosed with schizophrenia? "There is no question about
that," he replied. "Our data is overwhelming that not all schizo¬
phrenic patients need to be on antipsychotics all their lives."
Reviewing the Evidence
We have followed a trail of documents to a surprising end, and thus
I think we need to ask one final question: Does the evidence refuting
the common wisdom all hang together? In other words, does the
outcomes literature tell a coherent and consistent story? We need to
double-check to make sure we are not missing something, for it is
always discomforting to arrive at a conclusion so at odds with what
society "knows" to be true.
First, as researchers Lisa Dixon and Emmanuel Stip acknowl¬
edged, there is no good evidence that antipsychotics improve long¬
term schizophrenia outcomes. As such, we can be confident that we
haven't missed any such studies in our survey. Second, evidence that
the drugs might worsen long-term outcomes showed up in the very
first follow-up study conducted by the NIMH, and then it appears
again and again over the next fifty years. We can link the authors
of this research into a lengthy chain: Cole, Bockoven, Rappaport,
Carpenter, Mosher, Harding, the World Health Organization, and
Harrow. Third, once researchers came to understand how anti-
A PARADOX REVEALED
119
psychotics affected the brain, Chouinard and Jones stepped forward
with a biological explanation for why the drugs made patients more
vulnerable to psychosis over the long term. They were also able to
explain why the drug-induced brain changes made it so risky for
people to go off the medications, and thus they revealed why the
drug-withdrawal studies misled psychiatrists into believing that the
drugs prevented relapse. Fourth, evidence that long-term recovery
rates are higher for nonmedicated patients appears in studies and
investigations of many different types. It shows up in the random¬
ized studies conducted by Rappaport, Carpenter, and Mosher; in
the cross-cultural studies conducted by the World Health Organiza¬
tion; and in the naturalistic studies conducted by Harding and Har¬
row. Fifth, we see in the tardive dyskinesia studies evidence that
the drugs induce global brain dysfunction in a high percentage of
patients over the long term. Sixth, once a new tool for studying
brain structures came along (MRIs), investigators discovered that
antipsychotics cause morphological changes in the brain and that
these changes are associated with a worsening of both positive and
negative symptoms, and with cognitive impairment as well. Finally,
for the most part, the psychiatric researchers who conducted these
studies hoped and expected to find the reverse. They wanted to tell
a story of drugs that help schizophrenia patients fare well over the
long term —their bias was in that direction.
We are trying to solve a puzzle in this book —why have the num¬
ber of disabled mentally ill soared over the past fifty years —and I
think we now have our first puzzle piece in hand. We saw that in the
decade before the introduction of Thorazine, 65 percent or so of
first-episode schizophrenics would be discharged within twelve
months, and the majority of those discharged would not be rehospi¬
talized in follow-up periods of four and five years. This was what
we saw in Bockoven's study, too: Seventy-six percent of the psy¬
chotic patients treated with a progressive form of psychosocial care
in 1947 were living successfully in the community five years later.
But, as we saw in Harrow's study, only 5 percent of schizophrenia
patients who stayed on their drugs long-term ended up recovered.
That is a dramatic decline in recovery rates in the modern era, and
120
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
older psychiatrists, who can still remember what it was like to work
with unmedicated patients, can personally attest to this difference in
outcomes.
"In the nonmedication era, my schizophrenic patients did far bet¬
ter than do those in the more modern era," said Maryland psychia¬
trist Ann Silver, in an interview. "They chose careers, pursued them,
and married. One patient, who had been called the sickest admitted
to the adolescent division [of her hospital], is raising three children
and works as a registered nurse. In the later [medicated] era, none
chose a career, although many held various jobs, and none married
or even had lasting relationships."
We can also see how this drug-induced chronicity has con¬
tributed to the rise in the number of disabled mentally ill. In 1955,
there were 267,000 people with schizophrenia in state and county
mental hospitals, or one in every 617 Americans. Today, there are
an estimated 2.4 million people receiving SSI or SSDI because they
are ill with schizophrenia (or some other psychotic disorder), a dis¬
ability rate of one in every 125 Americans. 59 Since the arrival of
Thorazine, the disability rate due to psychotic illness has increased
fourfold in our society.
Cathy, George, and Kate
In the second chapter, we met two people —Cathy Levin and George
Badillo —who had been diagnosed with schizoaffective disorder
(Cathy) or schizophrenia (George). We can now see how their
stories fit into the outcomes literature.
As I said, Cathy Levin is one of the best responders to atypical
antipsychotics that I've ever met. She could be Janssen's poster girl
for promoting Risperdal. Still, she remains on SSDI and she per¬
ceives the medications as a barrier to her working full-time. Now
let's go back to that moment when she had her first psychotic
episode at Earlham College. What might her life have been like if
she had not been immediately placed on neuroleptics, but instead
A PARADOX REVEALED
121
had been treated with some form of psychosocial care? Or if, at
some point early on, she had been encouraged to withdraw gradu¬
ally from the antipsychotic medication? Would she have cycled in
and out of hospitals for the next twelve years? Would she have
ended up on SSDI? Although we can't really answer those ques¬
tions, we can say that the drug treatment increased the likelihood
that she would suffer that long period of constant hospitalizations,
and decreased the likelihood that she would fully recover from her
initial crackup. As Cathy said: "The thing I remember, looking
back, is that I was not really that sick early on. I was really just con¬
fused.
Meanwhile, George Badillo's story illustrates how getting off
meds can be the key to recovery, at least for some people diagnosed
with schizophrenia. His journey out of the back wards of a state
hospital began when he started tonguing his antipsychotic medica¬
tion. He is healthy today, he has an evident zest for life, and he rev¬
els in being a good father to his son and having his daughter
Madelyne back in his life. He is an example of the many recovered
people who showed up in the long-term studies by Harding and
Harrow —former patients who have quit taking antipsychotics and
are doing well.
Here is a third story of a young woman I'll call Kate, as she did
not want her real name used. Diagnosed with schizophrenia at age
nineteen, she did well on antipsychotics. In Harrow’s study, she
would have been among the 5 percent on meds who recovered. But
she also knows what it is like to be off meds and doing well, and
from her perspective, the latter type of recovery is totally unlike the
first.
Before I met Kate in person, I knew from a phone conversation
the bare outlines of her story, of how she had spent ten years on
antipsychotics, and given that those drugs can take such a physical
toll, I was a bit startled by her appearance when she showed up at
my office. To be blunt, the words "drop dead gorgeous" popped
into my head. A dark-haired woman, she wore jeans, a roseate top,
and light makeup, and she introduced herself in a confident, warm
way. Soon, she was showing me a "before” picture taken three
122
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
years earlier. "I was well over two-hundred pounds," she says. "I
was very slow, my face was droopy. I smoked a lot of cigarettes. . . .
It was very inhibiting to any sort of professional look."
Kate's story about her childhood is a familiar one. Her parents
divorced when she was eight, and she remembers herself as socially
awkward and horribly shy. "I only had social skills enough to inter¬
act with my family members," she says, and that awkwardness fol¬
lowed her to college. During her freshman year at the University of
Massachusetts at Dartmouth, she found it difficult to make friends,
and she felt so isolated that she cried constantly. Early in her sopho¬
more year she dropped out and went to live with her mother in
Boston, hoping to find a "purpose in life.” Instead, "my sense of re¬
ality started to disintegrate,” she recalls. "I started worrying about
God versus the devil, and I started becoming afraid of everything.
I'd say to my mom's friend, 'Is the food poisoned?' I was acting
quite bizarre, and I couldn't make sense of the conversations around
me. I would say these very odd things, and I would speak very
slowly, very deliberately, and weird.”
When she began talking about seeing wolves in her bedroom, her
mother put her in the hospital. Although she stabilized pretty well
on the antipsychotic medication, she hated how it made her feel,
and not long after she was discharged, she abruptly went off it,
which triggered a florid psychotic break. During her second hospi¬
talization, in February 1997, she was diagnosed with schizophrenia,
and this time she accepted the fact that she would have to take anti-
psychotics for life. Eventually, she found a two-drug combination
that worked well for her, and she began rebuilding a life. In 2001,
she graduated from UMass Boston, and a year later she married a
man she had met in a day treatment program. "We both had a psy¬
chiatric disability, and we both smoked heavily," she says. "We both
saw therapists daily. This is what we had in common."
Kate took a job in a group home for the mentally handicapped,
and although at times she had trouble staying awake, a side effect of
her medications, she earned enough to get off SSDI. For a person
with schizophrenia, she was doing extremely well. Yet she wasn't
happy. She had gained nearly one hundred pounds, and her hus¬
band often cruelly taunted her, telling her that she was "ugly” and
A PARADOX REVEALED
123
had a "fat ass.” She chafed too over how everybody in the system
treated her. "Recovery on the med model requires you to be obedi¬
ent, like a child," she explains. "You are obedient to your doctors,
you are compliant with your therapist, and you take your meds.
There's no striving toward greater intellectual concerns."
In 2005, she grew closer to a longtime friend, who was twenty
years older and belonged to a fundamentalist religious community.
She began attending their meetings, and they in turn began advising
her to dress, speak, and present herself to the world in a more for¬
mal way. "They told me, 'You are representing God, and you don't
want to bring shame to God,” she says. Kate's older friend also
urged her to stop thinking of herself as schizophrenic. "He's making
me think outside the box, and to think in ways that before I never
would have accepted. I would always defend my therapist, defend
my psychiatrist, defend the drugs, and defend my illness. He was
asking me to give up my identity as a mentally impaired person."
Soon, her old life fell completely apart. She discovered that her
husband had been sleeping with one of her friends, and after she
moved out of their apartment, she had to sleep for a time in her car.
Although at first, during that desperate time, she clung to her meds,
the nonschizophrenic vision of herself also beckoned, and in Febru¬
ary of 2006, she decided to take the leap: She would stop smoking,
she would stop drinking coffee, and she would wean herself from
her psychiatric medications. "Now I have no drugs, no nicotine,
and no coffee, and my body is going into shock. I am coming down
from all of this, and I am almost vibrating because I need my
cigarettes, my drugs."
This decision also put her at odds with most everyone in her life.
"I stopped talking to my family, because I didn't want to go back
into that identity [of a disabled person]. My mind was very delicate.
So I had to disengage from what I knew, and disengage from my
therapist." Soon, she was losing so much weight that her friends
thought she must be sick. As she struggled to stay sane, she clung to
the advice from her religious group, speaking to others in a very for¬
mal manner, and this behavior convinced her mother that she was
relapsing. "Strange ain't the word, honey" is how her mother puts
it, and even Kate privately feared that she was becoming psychotic
124
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
again. "But I had this hope, this faith, and so I said to myself, 'I am
going to walk this tightrope across this horrible canyon, and hope¬
fully when I get to the other side, there will be a mountain ridge I
can stand on.' I had to focus on going forward regardless of where
it took me, because if I fell off the tightrope, I was back in the hos¬
pital."
It was at that perilous moment, when it seemed that she was
about to crash, that Kate agreed to meet her mother for dinner. "I
think she is having a breakdown," her mother says. "She sat very
proper, and looked scattered and disorganized. Her body was stiff. I
was seeing a lot of the same symptoms as before. Her eyes were di¬
lated and she seemed paranoid." As they drove away from the
restaurant, Kate's mother started to turn toward the hospital, but at
the last second she changed her mind. Kate "wasn't so crazy" that
she needed to be locked up. "I went home and cried,” her mother
remembers. "I didn't know what was happening."
By her mother's reckoning, it took Kate six months to get
through this withdrawal process. But she emerged on the other side
transformed. "I see that her face is so alive now and she is more
connected to her body,” her mother says. "She feels comfortable in
her own skin and more at peace with herself than ever. She is physi¬
cally healthy. I didn't know that this kind of recovery was possible."
In 2007, Kate married the older man who had encouraged her to go
this route; she also has thrived in her job as the manager of a home
for people with psychiatric problems, the company recognizing her
for her "outstanding” performance in 2008, an award that came
with a cash prize.
Kate does still struggle at times. The home she manages provides
shelter to several men who are sexual deviants— 'T've had people
say they are going to set me on fire, or they are going to pee in my
mouth," she says —and she no longer is having her emotional re¬
sponses to such stress numbed by medication. 'T've been off the
drugs for two years, and sometimes I find it very, very difficult to
deal with my emotions. I tend to have these rages of anger. Did the
drugs bring such a cloud over my mind, make me so comatose, that
I never gained skills on how to deal with my emotions? Now I'm
finding myself getting angrier than ever and getting happier than
A PARADOX REVEALED
•25
ever too. The circle with my emotions is getting wider. And yes, it's
easy to deal with when you're happy, but how do you deal with it
when you are mad? I'm working on not getting overly defensive,
and trying to take things in stride.”
Kate's story, of course, is idiosyncratic in kind. Her success at get¬
ting off meds does not mean that everyone can successfully with¬
draw from them. Kate is an amazing person —incredibly willful and
incredibly brave. Indeed, what the scientific literature reveals is that
once a person is on an antipsychotic, it can be very difficult and
risky to withdraw from the medication, and that many people suffer
severe relapses. But the literature also reveals that there are people
who can successfully withdraw from the medications and that it is
this group that fares best in the long term. Kate made it into that
group.
"That day in 2005 when I decided to get better, that's the divid¬
ing line in my life," she says. "I was a completely different person
then. I was very heavy, I smoked all the time, I had flat affect. Today
I run into people who knew me then, and they don't even recognize
me. Even my mother says, 'You are not the same person.' "
7
The Benzo Trap
"What seemed so good about the benzodiazepines
when I was playing with them was that it seemed like
we really did have a drug that didn't have many
problems. But in retrospect it's difficult to put a
spanner into a wristwatch and expect that it won't
do any harm."
— ALEC JENNER, BRITISH PHYSICIAN WHO
CONDUCTED FIRST TRIALS OF A BENZODIAZEPINE IN
THE UK (2003)
Fans of the cable television series Mad Men, which tells of the lives
of Don Draper and other Madison Avenue advertising men in the
early 1960s, may recall a scene from the last episode of season two,
when a friend of Draper's wife, Betty, says to her: "Do you want a
Miltown? It's the only thing keeping me from chewing my nails
off.” That was a nice, historically accurate touch, and if the creators
of Mad Men retain this period accuracy in season three and beyond,
which will tell the story of the ad men and their families during the
turbulent years of the mid-1960s, viewers can expect Betty Draper
and her friends to reach into their purses and make sly references to
"mother's little helper.” Hoffmann-La Roche brought Valium to
market in 1963, advertising it in particular to women, and from
1968 to 1981, it was the bestselling drug in the Western world. Yet,
as Americans gobbled up this pill designed to keep them tranquil,
something very odd happened: The number of people admitted to
mental hospitals, psychiatric emergency rooms, and mental health
outpatient clinics soared.
The scientific literature can explain why the two were linked.
THE BENZO TRAP
127
Anxiety Before Miltown
Although anxiety is a regular part of the human psyche, our minds
fashioned by evolution to worry and fret, there are some people who
are more anxious than others, and the notion that such emotional
distress is a diagnosable condition can be traced back to a New York
nerve doctor, George Beard. In 1869, he announced that dread,
worry, fatigue, and insomnia resulted from "tired nerves,” a physi¬
cal illness he dubbed "neurasthenia." The diagnosis proved to be a
popular one, this illness thought to be a by-product of the industrial
revolution that was sweeping America in the wake of the Civil War,
and naturally the market created a variety of therapies that could
restore a person's "tired” nerves. Makers of patent medicines sold
"nerve revitalizers" laced with opiates, cocaine, and alcohol. Neuro¬
logists touted the restorative powers of electricity, and this led those
diagnosed with neurasthenia to buy electric belts, suspenders, and
handheld massagers. Those who were wealthier could head to spas
that offered "rest cures," the patients' nerves restored through the
healing touch of soothing baths, massages, and various electric
gadgets.
Sigmund Freud provided psychiatry with a rationale for treating
this group of patients and, in so doing, enabled psychiatry to move
out of the asylum and into the office. Born in 1856, Freud set out
his shingle as a nerve doctor in Vienna in 1886, which meant that
many of his patients were women suffering from neurasthenia
(Beard's disease had become popular in Europe, too). After hours of
conversation with his clients, Freud became convinced that their
feelings of dread and worry were psychological in origin, rather
than the result of tired nerves. In 1895, he wrote about "anxiety
neurosis” in women, which he theorized arose in large part from
their unconscious repression of sexual desires and fantasies. Those
suffering from such psychological conflicts could find relief through
psychoanalysis, the patient on the couch led by the doctor into an
exploration of her unconscious mind.
At this time, psychiatry was a profession for those who treated
mad patients in the asylum. People with tired nerves went to see a
128
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
nerve doctor or a general practitioner for help. But if anxiety arose
from a psychological disorder in the brain, rather than from a fraz¬
zling of the nerves, then it made sense that psychiatrists could tend
to these patients, and after Freud visited America in 1909, psycho¬
analytic societies began to form, with New York City the hub of this
new therapy. Nationwide, only 3 percent of psychiatrists were in pri¬
vate practice in 1909; thirty years later, 38 percent were seeing pa¬
tients in private settings. 2 Moreover, Freudian theory made nearly
everyone a candidate for the psychiatrist’s couch. "Neurotics," Freud
explained during his 1909 tour, "fall ill of the same complexes with
which we sound people struggle." 3
Thanks to Freudian theories, psychiatric disorders were now di¬
vided into two basic categories: psychotic and neurotic. In 1952, the
American Psychiatric Association published the first edition of its
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, and it described the neurotic pa¬
tient in this way:
The chief characteristic of [neurotic] disorders is "anxiety,”
which may be directly felt and expressed or which may be un¬
consciously and automatically controlled by the utilization of
various psychological defense mechanisms. ... In contrast to
those with psychoses, patients with psychoneurotic disorder
do not exhibit gross distortion or falsification of external re¬
ality (delusions, hallucinations, illusions) and they do not
present gross disorganization of the personality. 4
Such was the understanding of anxiety when Miltown came
to market. Anxious people had their feet firmly planted in reality,
and rarely was anxiety a condition that required hospitalization.
In 1955, there were only 5,415 "psychoneurotic" patients in state
mental hospitals. 5 As Stanford psychiatrist Leo Flollister confessed
after the benzodiazepines were introduced, these drugs were "de¬
signed to treat what many would regard as a 'minor disorder.' " 6
The drugs were a balm for the "walking wounded," and thus, as we
review the outcomes literature for the benzodiazepines, we should
expect this patient group to function well. After all, that was the fu¬
ture promised by Miltown inventor Frank Berger: "Tranquilizers,
THE BENZO TRAP
I 2 9
by attenuating the disruptive influence of anxiety on the mind, open
the way to a better and more coordinated use of the existing gifts,”
he said. 7
The Minor Tranquilizers Fall from Grace
When Miltown first appeared, there were a number of studies pub¬
lished in medical journals that told —as two Harvard Medical
School researchers, David Greenblatt and Richard Shader, later re¬
called—of how it "was almost magically effective in reducing anxi¬
ety.” But as has often been the case in psychiatry, once a successor
pill appeared on the market (Librium, in 1960), the efficacy of the
old drug suddenly began to fade. In their review of the Miltown lit¬
erature in 1974, Greenblatt and Shader found that in twenty-six
well-controlled trials, there were only five in which Miltown "was
more effective than placebo" as a treatment for anxiety. Nor was
there any evidence that Miltown was better than a barbiturate in
calming the nerves. The initial popularity of this drug, they wrote,
"illustrates how factors other than scientific evidence may deter¬
mine physicians' patterns of drug use." 8
However, Miltown's fall from favor with the public arose from a
different problem than lack of scientific efficacy. Many who tried
the drug found that they became sick when they stopped taking it,
and in 1964, Carl Essig, a scientist at the Addiction Research Cen¬
ter in Lexington, Kentucky, reported that it "could induce physical
dependence in man." 5 Science News quickly announced that the
happy pill could be "addictive,” and on April 30, 1965, Time all
but buried Miltown. There is "a growing disillusionment with Mil-
town on the part of many doctors," the magazine wrote. "Some
doubt that it has any more tranquilizing effect than a dummy sugar
pill. ... A few physicians have reported that in some patients, Mil-
town may cause a true addiction, followed by withdrawal symp¬
toms like those of narcotics users 'kicking the habit.' " I0
Publicly, the benzodiazepines mostly escaped this opprobrium
during the 1960s. When Hoffmann-La Roche brought Librium to
130
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
market in 1960, it claimed that its drug provided "pure anxiety re¬
lief,” and unlike Miltown and the barbiturates, was "safe, harmless
and non-addicting." That belief took hold and the FDA did little to
counter it, even though very early on it started receiving letters from
people who were experiencing odd and quite distressing symptoms
when they tried to quit a benzodiazepine. They told of awful insom¬
nia, anxiety more severe than they had known before, and a rash of
physical symptoms —tremors, headaches, and nerves that "jangled
like crazy." As one man wrote the FDA, "I was not sleeping and in
general felt horrible. Sometimes I thought I would die and other
times wished I had.” 11 Although the FDA held a hearing on the mat¬
ter, it did not impose any legal control on benzodiazepines similar to
what had been placed on amphetamines and barbiturates, and so
the public's belief that the drugs were relatively nonaddictive and
harmless survived until 1975, when the U.S. Justice Department de¬
manded that they be classified as schedule IV drugs under the Con¬
trolled Substances Act. This designation limited the number of
refills a patient could obtain without a new prescription, and re¬
vealed to the public that the government had concluded that benzo¬
diazepines were, in fact, addictive.
"Danger ahead! Valium —The Pill You Love Can Turn on You,”
a Vogue headline screamed. A benzodiazepine, the magazine ex¬
plained, could lead to a "far worse addiction than heroin." 12 The
Valium backlash had begun, particularly in the pages of women’s
magazines, and soon Ms. magazine provided readers with first-
person accounts of the horrors of withdrawing from it. "My with¬
drawal symptoms are a double-dose of the anxiety, irritableness,
and insomnia I used to feel," one user said. Confessed another: "I
can't begin to describe the physical and mental anguish that accom¬
panied my withdrawal." 19 The happiness pill of the 1950s was turn¬
ing into the misery pill of the 1970s, with the New York Times
reporting in 1976 that "some critics go so far to say that [Valium] is
doing more harm than good, or even deny that it is doing any good
at all for the great majority of patients. Some cry with alarm that it
is far from being as safe as it is proclaimed, that it can be hideously
and dangerously addictive, and may be the direct cause of addicts'
deaths." 14 Two million Americans were said to be addicted to
THE BENZO TRAP
• 131
benzodiazepines, four times the number of heroin addicts in the
country, and one of the pill takers turned out to be former first lady
Betty Ford, who checked herself into an alcohol and drug rehab
center in 1978. Abuse of tranquilizers, said her physician Joseph
Pursch, was "the nation's number one health problem." 15
Over the next few years, the benzodiazepines officially fell from
grace. In 1979, Senator Edward Kennedy held a Senate Health Sub¬
committee hearing on the dangers of benzodiazepines, which he said
had "produced a nightmare of dependence and addiction, both very
difficult to treat and recover from." 15 After reviewing the scientific
literature, the White House Office of Drug Policy and the National
Institute of Drug Abuse concluded that the drugs' sleep-promoting
effects didn't last more than two weeks, and this finding was soon
seconded by the Committee on the Review of Medicines in the
United Kingdom, which found that the drugs' anti-anxiety effects
didn't last beyond four months. As such, the committee recom¬
mended that "patients receiving benzodiazepine therapy be carefully
selected and monitored and that prescriptions be limited to short¬
term use."” As an editorial in the British Medical Journal put it:
"Now that benzodiazepines have been shown to cause drug
dependence should their use be more closely controlled —or even
banned?" 18
The ABCs of Benzodiazepines
This story of the benzodiazepines' fall from grace might seem like
ancient history, a footnote in our quest to understand why there has
been such a rise in the number of disabled mentally ill in the United
States over the past fifty years, except for the fact that the benzodi¬
azepines never really went away. Although the number of prescrip¬
tions for benzodiazepines dropped after they were classified as
schedule IV drugs, from 103 million in 1975 to 71 million in 1980,
the following year Upjohn brought Xanax to market, and this
helped stabilize sales of benzodiazepines.” Psychiatrists continued
to prescribe benzodiazepines to many of their nervous patients, and
in 2002, Stephen Stahl, a well-known psychopharmacologist at the
13 2
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
University of California in San Diego, confessed to psychiatry's
dirty little secret in an article titled "Don't Ask, Don't Tell, But
Benzodiazepines Are Still the Leading Treatments for Anxiety Dis¬
orders." 20 Since that time, the prescribing of benzodiazepines in the
United States has increased, from 69 million prescriptions in 2002
to 83 million in 2007, which isn't all that far below the number
written at the height of the Valium craze in 1973. 21
So, given that benzodiazepines have been widely used for fifty
years, we need to look at what science has to tell about these drugs,
and whether their use may be contributing in some way to the in¬
crease in the number of disabled mentally ill in the United States.
Short-term efficacy
As anyone who has taken a benzodiazepine can attest, it acts rap¬
idly, and if a person hasn't become habituated to the drug, it will
numb his or her emotional distress. As such, a benzodiazepine has
an obvious utility in helping people through a situational crisis. The
writer Andrea Tone, in her book The Age of Anxiety, relates how a
benzodiazepine enabled her to get on an airplane after she some¬
what mysteriously developed a fear of flying. But as clinical trials re¬
vealed, that immediate efficacy quickly begins to fade and pretty
much disappears by the end of four to six weeks.
In 1978, Kenneth Solomon at Albany Medical College in New
York reviewed seventy-eight double-blind trials of benzodiazepines
and determined that the drugs had proved to be significantly better
than a placebo in only forty-four of them. At best, the collective re¬
sults could be said to "hint at therapeutic efficacy," he wrote. 22 Five
years later, Arthur Shapiro at Mt. Sinai School of Medicine in New
York City fleshed out this efficacy picture a bit more, reporting that
in a trial of 224 anxious patients, Valium proved superior to a
placebo for the first week, but then this advantage began to lessen.
Based on the patients' self-assessment of their symptoms, by the end
of the second week there was no difference between the drug and a
placebo, and by the end of six weeks, the placebo group was faring
slightly better. "It is unlikely in our opinion that carefully controlled
THE BENZO TRAP
133
studies would consistently show significant benzodiazepine thera¬
peutic antianxiety effects," Shapiro wrote. 23
That picture of the short-term efficacy of benzodiazepines has
not markedly changed since then. The drugs show clear efficacy for
the first week, and then their advantage over a placebo abates. But,
as British investigators noted in 1991, this brief period of efficacy
comes at a fairly high cost. "Both psychomotor and cognitive func¬
tioning may be impaired, and amnesia is a common effect of all ben¬
zodiazepines," they said. 24 In 2007, researchers in Spain looked at
whether these adverse events negated the small "efficacy benefit"
provided by the drugs, and found that the drop-out rates in clinical
trials, a measure often used to assess the overall "effectiveness" of a
drug, were the same for benzodiazepine and placebo patients. "This
systematic review did not find convincing evidence of the short-term
effectiveness of the benzodiazepines in the treatment of generalized
anxiety disorder," they reported. 25
Malcolm Lader, a psychiatrist at the Institute of Psychiatry in
London who is one of the world's leading experts on benzodi¬
azepines, explained the importance of this finding in an interview:
"Effectiveness is a measure of what it's like in real practice." 26
Withdrawal syndromes
Although the first report of benzodiazepine dependence appeared in
the scientific literature in 1961, when Leo Hollister at Stanford Uni¬
versity reported that patients withdrawing from Librium were expe¬
riencing odd symptoms, it wasn't until the Justice Department
classified benzodiazepines as schedule IV drugs that researchers
began investigating the problem with any vigor. In 1976, physicians
Barry Maletzky and James Kotter jump-started this inquiry, re¬
porting that when their patients stopped taking Valium, many
complained of "extreme anxiety." 27 Two years later, physicians at
Pennsylvania State University announced that patients withdrawing
from benzodiazepines often experienced "an increase in anxiety
above baseline levels ... a condition that we term 'rebound anxi¬
ety.' " 2S In Britain, Lader reported similar findings. "Anxiety rose
1 3 4
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
In this 1 985 study by British investigators, the patients treated with Valium did not fare better
than the placebo patients during the first six weeks. The Valium patients were then withdrawn
from the drug and their anxiety symptoms soared, to a much higher level than the symptoms
in placebo patients. Source: Power, K. "Controlled study of withdrawal symptoms and rebound
anxiety after six week course of diazepam for generalised anxiety." British Medical Journal 290
(1 985): 1 246-48.
sharply during withdrawal, and to a point of panic in several pa¬
tients. Patients commonly experienced bodily symptoms of anxiety,
such as a choking feeling, dry mouth, hot and cold, legs like jelly,
etc.
Patients withdrawing from benzodiazepines, it seemed, were be¬
coming more anxious than they had ever been. Over the course of
the next decade, Lader and other British physicians (most notably
Heather Ashton, a doctor at the University of Newcastle upon Tyne
who ran a withdrawal clinic) continued to investigate this problem,
and they compiled a long list of symptoms that could bedevil those
quitting a benzodiazepine. In addition to rebound anxiety, patients
could experience insomnia, seizures, tremors, headaches, blurred vi¬
sion, a ringing in the ears, extreme sensitivity to noise, a feeling that
insects were crawling over them, nightmares, hallucinations, ex¬
treme depression, depersonalization, and derealization (a sense that
the external world is unreal). Withdrawal, one patient told Heather
Ashton, was like "living death ... I thought I had gone mad.”
"These findings show very clearly that benzodiazepine withdrawal
THE BENZO TRAP
135
is a severe illness,” Ashton wrote. "The patients were usually fright¬
ened, often in intense pain, and genuinely prostrated. . . . Through
no fault of their own, the patients suffered considerable physical as
well as mental distress." 30
Not all people withdrawn from benzodiazepines suffer in this
way. The risk of suffering withdrawal symptoms varies according to
how long a person has been on the drug, the potency of the benzodi¬
azepine, and the speed of the drug-tapering process. A majority of
patients who've taken a benzodiazepine for a relatively short time,
such as a month or two, may be able to withdraw from it with little
difficulty. However, some people experience withdrawal symptoms
after taking a benzodiazepine for only a few weeks, and it can take a
longtime user a year or longer to taper from the drug. Moreover, a
small percentage of people suffer a "protracted withdrawal syn¬
drome," their anxiety remaining at elevated levels "for many months
after benzodiazepine withdrawal," Ashton observed. 31 Depression
may deepen, and the odd perceptual symptoms —the depersonaliza¬
tion, the derealization, the sensation of insects crawling on the
skin —can haunt a person for an extended period. Most alarming, a
small percentage of long-term users never fully recover. "It is very
worrying," Lader said, in an interview. "Somehow there has been a
change [in the brain], I cannot say that everybody is going to recover
back to normality when they come off long-term usage.”
The biology of benzodiazepine withdrawal
In 1977, researchers discovered that benzodiazepines affect a neuro¬
transmitter in the brain known as GABA. Unlike dopamine and
serotonin, which transmit an "excitatory" message telling a neuron
to fire, GABA (gamma-aminobutyric acid) inhibits neuronal activ¬
ity. A neuron receiving the GABA message either fires at a slower
rate or stops firing for a period of time. A majority of neurons in the
brain have GABA receptors, which means that this neurotransmit¬
ter acts as the brain's brake on neuronal activity. A benzodiazepine
binds to the GABA receptor and, in so doing, amplifies GABA's in¬
hibitory effects. It pushes down on the GABA brake, so to speak,
and as a result, it suppresses central nervous system activity.
1 3 6
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
In response, the brain decreases its output of GABA and de¬
creases the density of its GABA receptors. It is trying to "restore
normal GABA transmission," British scientists explained in 1982. 32
However, as a result of these adaptive changes, the brain's braking
system is now in a physiologically impaired state. Its braking fluid is
low (GABA output), and its brake pads are worn (GABA receptors).
As a result, when the benzodiazepine is withdrawn, the brain is no
longer able to properly inhibit neuronal activity, and its neurons
may begin firing at a helter-skelter pace. This overactivity, Heather
Ashton concluded, may "account for many of the effects of with¬
drawal."" The anxiety, the insomnia, the sensation of insects crawl¬
ing across the skin, the paranoia, the derealization, the seizures —all
of these vexing symptoms may arise from neuronal hyperactivity.
If a person gradually tapers off from a benzodiazepine, the
GABA system may slowly revert to normal, and thus withdrawal
symptoms may be mild. However, the fact that some long-term
users suffer "protracted symptoms" is probably "due to the failure
of the [GABA] receptors to revert to their normal state," Ashton
said. 34 Long-term benzodiazepine use, she explained, may "give rise
not only to slowly reversible functional changes in the central ner¬
vous system, but may also occasionally cause structural neuronal
damage.” 35 In such cases, the GABA brake never again functions
like it should.
Long-term effects
Once researchers in the United States and the United Kingdom de¬
termined that benzodiazepines did not provide any durable relief
from anxiety, an obvious question arose: Do these drugs, when
taken on a continual basis, worsen the very symptom they are sup¬
posed to treat? In 1991, Karl Rickels at the University of Pennsyl¬
vania School of Medicine reported on a group of anxious patients
who had tried to quit benzodiazepines three years earlier, and he
found that those who had successfully gotten off the drugs were
doing "significantly" better than those who had failed to do so. 35 A
few years later, he was back with a new study: When long-term
THE BENZO TRAP
137
users withdrew from benzodiazepines, they "became more alert,
more relaxed, and less anxious, and this change was accompanied
by improved psychomotor functions." 97 Those who stayed on the
benzodiazepines were more emotionally distressed than those who
got off.
Others told of similar long-term results. Canadian investigators
found that benzodiazepine usage led to a fourfold increase in de¬
pressive symptoms. 3 ' In England, Ashton observed that those who
stay on the drugs tend to became more ill: "Many patients find that
anxiety symptoms gradually increase over the years despite continu¬
ous benzodiazepine use, and panic attacks and agoraphobia may
appear for the first time." 33 These studies and observations told of a
very problematic long-term course, and in 2007, French researchers
surveyed 4,425 long-term benzodiazepine users and found that 75
percent were "markedly ill to extremely ill ... a great majority of
the patients had significant symptomatology, in particular major de¬
pressive episodes and generalized anxiety disorder, often with
marked severity and disability." 40
In addition to causing emotional distress, long-term benzodi¬
azepine usage also leads to cognitive impairment. Early on, re¬
searchers recognized that memory problems were associated with
short-term use, and this led David Knott, a physician at the Univer¬
sity of Tennessee, to warn in 1976 that "I am very convinced that
Valium, Librium and other drugs of that class cause damage to the
brain. I have seen damage to the cerebral cortex that I believe is due
to the use of these drugs, and I am beginning to wonder if the dam¬
age is permanent." 41 Over the next twenty-five years, reports of cog¬
nitive impairment in long-term benzodiazepine users regularly
appeared in scientific journals. These studies told of people who
were having trouble focusing, remembering things, learning new
material, and solving problems. However, the patients "are not
aware of their reduced ability," Lader wrote, evidence that their
self-insight was impaired as well. 43 In 2004, a group of Australian
scientists, after reviewing the relevant literature, concluded that
"long-term benzodiazepine users were consistently more impaired
than controls across all cognitive categories," with these deficits
1 3 8
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
"moderate to large” in magnitude. The studies showed the "higher
the intake, dose and period of use [of a benzodiazepine], the greater
the risk of impairment." 43
Increased anxiety, increased depression, and cognitive impair¬
ment-all of these factors contribute to a decline in a person's abil¬
ity to function in society. In 1983, the World Health Organization
noted a "striking deterioration in personal care and social inter¬
actions" in long-term benzodiazepine users. 44 Another investigator
reported that they end up with poor coping skills. 45 In a study
funded by Hoffmann-La Roche, the manufacturer of Valium, Uni¬
versity of Michigan investigators determined that taking this drug
was "associated with poor quality of life, poor performance in
work and personal life, low social support, perceived lack of inter¬
nal control, poor perceived health and high levels of stress." 46 Ashton
determined that long-term use led to "malaise, ill-health, and ele¬
vated scores for neuroticism." 47 Benzodiazepines, she said, contribute
to "job loss, unemployment, and loss of work through illness." 48
Such is the history told about benzodiazepines in the scientific litera¬
ture. Moreover, it is a story easily traced, as Dr. Stevan Gressitt, who
today is the medical director for Adult Mental Health Services in
Maine, can attest. In 2002, he helped form the Maine Benzo Study
Group, which was comprised of physicians and other health-care
professionals, and it concluded that "there is no evidence support¬
ing the long-term use of benzodiazepines for any mental health con¬
dition." Benzodiazepines, Gressitt and his colleagues wrote, may
"aggravate” both "medical and mental health problems.” In an in¬
terview, I asked Dr. Gressitt whether those "problems" included
increased anxiety, cognitive impairment, and functional decline. Was
his understanding of the scientific literature, I wondered, the same as
mine?
"Your words I don't contradict or argue with," he replied. 49
THE BENZO TRAP
139
Geraldine, Hal, and Jill
The scientific literature reveals that benzodiazepines —much as the
neuroleptics do —act like a trap. The drugs ameliorate anxiety for a
short period of time, and thus they can provide a distressed person
much needed relief. However, they work by perturbing a neuro¬
transmitter system, and in response, the brain undergoes compensa¬
tory adaptations, and as a result of this change, the person becomes
vulnerable to relapse upon drug withdrawal. That difficulty in turn
may lead some to take the drugs indefinitely, and these patients are
likely to become more anxious, more depressed, and cognitively
impaired.
Here are the stories of three people who fell into the trap.
Geraldine Burns, a thin woman with dark red hair, still lives in the
house she grew up in. She tells me her story while we sit in her
kitchen, her elderly mother darting in and out.
Born in 1955, Geraldine was one of six children, and theirs was a
happy family. Her father was Irish, her mother Lebanese, and their
Boston neighborhood was known as "Little Lebanon,” a place
where everybody definitely knew your name. Aunts, uncles, and
other relatives lived nearby. At age eighteen, Geraldine started dat¬
ing a boy who lived down the block, Joe Burns. "I've been with him
ever since," she says, and for a time their life unfolded just as Geral¬
dine had hoped. She had a job that she enjoyed in human resources
at a rehabilitation center, she and Joe had a healthy son (Garrett) in
1984, and they basked in their close-knit neighborhood. Geraldine —
outgoing and energetic —was the constant hostess for gatherings of
family and friends. "I loved my life,” she says. "I loved working, I
loved my family, and I loved this neighborhood. I was the one who
organized the reunion of my grammar school. I still had friends
from kindergarten. I couldn't have been more normal."
However, in March 1988, Geraldine gave birth to a daughter,
Liana, and she felt physically unwell afterward. "I kept telling the
doctors and nurses that I felt like I weighed a thousand pounds,”
140
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
she says, and after a doctor ruled out an infection, he figured she
must be anxious and prescribed Ativan. Geraldine came home from
the hospital with a prescription for that benzodiazepine, and al¬
though it helped for a short while, months later she still felt some¬
thing wasn't right and so she went to see a psychiatrist. "She
immediately tells me I have a chemical imbalance," Geraldine re¬
calls. "She says that I should keep taking the Ativan and assures me
that it is harmless and nonaddictive. She tells me that I will have to
take this drug for the rest of my life. Later, when I questioned her
about this, she explained it this way: 'If you were a diabetic you
would have to take insulin for the rest of your life, wouldn't you?' "
Soon her psychiatrist added an antidepressant to the Ativan, and
as Geraldine struggled to take care of her daughter that first year,
her emotions seemed numbed, her mind fogged. "I was in a daze
half the time. My mother would call and I would tell her something,
and she would say, 'You told me that last night.' And I'd say, 'I
did?' " Worse, as the months wore on, she found herself becoming
ever more anxious, so much so that she started staying inside her
house. Going back to her job in human resources at the rehabilita¬
tion center was now out of the question. At one point, after she
stopped taking Ativan for a day or two, she had a "massive panic
attack." The federal government agreed that she was disabled by
"anxiety" and thus eligible for a monthly SSDI payment. "Me, who
was the most social person on the planet, is not able to go out,"
Geraldine says, shaking her head in disbelief. "I wouldn't go out un¬
less my husband would take me."
Over the next eight years, Geraldine cycled through an endless
combination of anti-anxiety and antidepressant medications. None
worked. The anxiety and panic remained, and she suffered from a
medley of side effects —rashes, sexual dysfunction, weight gain,
tachycardia (from the panic attacks), and excessive menstrual bleed¬
ing, the last leading to a hysterectomy. "All of the women I've
known who were on Ativan long-term ended up having a hysterec¬
tomy, every single one of us," she says, with evident bitterness. At
last, in October 1996, she went to a new physician, who, after re¬
viewing her medical history, identified a likely culprit. "He told me,
'You are on one of the most addictive drugs known,' and I thought,
THE BENZO TRAP
141
'Thank God.' I was in tears. It was the drugs all along. I had been
made iatrogenically ill."
Geraldine spent two nightmarish years withdrawing from Ativan
and the other psychiatric drugs she had been taking. Horrible smells
came from her body, her muscles twitched, she lost weight, and at
one point, she couldn't sleep for weeks. "It was like hell opened up
and swallowed me in," she says. Although she did kick the habit, it
took several more years for her to feel better physically, and she still
suffers from a great deal of anxiety. The gregarious, socially-at-ease
person she had always been before that fateful day in March 1988
when she was prescribed Ativan has never returned. "Am I back to
my old self? No," she whispers. "I mourn who I used to be. We all
mourn. I am still so afraid of so many things."
Three days before I was to meet with Hal Flugman, who lives in
South Florida, he called to say that his anxiety had flared up again,
and the thought of leaving his house to talk to me was too stressful.
"I am not feeling right," he said. "I'm over-breathing, I have these
terrible gastrointestinal problems. I think I have to get my Klonopin
dose upped. . . . This is what is happening to me.”
Hal, whom I'd interviewed by phone a few months earlier, first
became anxious when he was thirteen years old. Overweight and
small, he didn't get along well with his classmates in middle school.
"I had panic attacks, and a slight fear of being around people," he
recalls. For the next five years, he went to counseling, but he was
not prescribed a medication. "I was living with it, dealing with it,”
he says, but then one night at a rock concert, the panic hit so hard
that he had to call his family and beg that they come get him. The
following day a doctor gave him a prescription for Klonopin.
"I remember saying to the doctor, 'Am I going to become ad¬
dicted and have a really hard time coming off?' I was worried about
the side effects, too. But the doctor said that the side effects would
go away in a couple of weeks, and didn't that beat living with these
unbearable panic attacks? I said, 'Well, of course.' And I knew from
the first pill that this was going to solve my anxiety problem. It
absolutely worked for me. I felt great.”
142
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Hal's life since then is a story of addiction. Shortly after going on
the drug, he moved to San Francisco to pursue a career as a musi¬
cian, and for a time it went well —he even got to hang out with Car¬
los Santana, the great guitarist. But his music career failed to take
off, and today he thinks that the Klonopin was partly to blame, for
it stifled his ambition and didn't help his finger dexterity, either.
Eventually, he fell into a deep depression — "I felt like a zombie," he
says —and at age twenty-nine he returned to Florida to live with his
parents. At that point, he was diagnosed with bipolar illness, the
government agreeing that he was so disabled by mental illness that
he was eligible to receive SSI. The years slid by, his mother passed
away, and then, in 2001, he began taking higher doses of Klonopin,
as otherwise his depression would become unbearable. His doctor
told him he was abusing the drug and sent him to a detox facility,
where, over a period of ten days, he was withdrawn from the ben¬
zodiazepine he had been taking for sixteen years.
"What happened next was absolutely the worst thing in my life,"
he says. "I could give you a list of symptoms, but that wouldn't do
justice to what I was going through mentally. Month after month I
got worse and worse. I couldn't sleep, and the symptoms —the most
debilitating one was this feeling that I was dead. I felt that my brain
was ripped out of my head, like I wasn't even a living thing. I had
depersonalization, my skin felt weird, my body felt weird. I didn't
even want to get into the shower. Even room-temperature water felt
strange on my skin. If I put on mildly hot water, it felt like it was
burning right through me. I couldn't digest food right, I couldn't go
to the bathroom for weeks at a time, I couldn't urinate right ... I
was in a constant state of panic attacks, and this doctor is telling me
it's all in my mind, that he won't write me a script, and that with¬
drawal symptoms can last a maximum of thirty days. I was cracking
up, going insane.”
This went on for ten months. He found Geraldine Burns on the
Internet, as she had started a benzodiazepine support group, and
she would console him for hours at a time. Ten, twenty times a
night he would call his sister Susan, screaming that he was going to
kill himself. He desperately sought to get a new prescription for
Klonopin, but the doctors he saw didn't believe that his torment
THE BENZO TRAP
143
was related to benzodiazepine withdrawal. Instead, they figured
that he had abused the drug in the past and so they refused to put
him back on it. "They don't understand that the drug changes the
whole biology of your brain, and that your brain doesn't work right
anymore,” Hal says. Finally, his sister found a physician who agreed
to write him a script, and "within hours, the nightmare was over.
Every single side effect, every single withdrawal problem I had been
going through was gone. Completely. Like magic. I was jumping up
and down I was so excited.”
Hal has never tried going off Klonopin again. His brain adapted
to the drug, he says, and now it can't adapt back. "Klonopin ruined
my life. It takes away your drive, and in the morning, you don't
want to get out of bed, because you feel so groggy. I don’t even
know what it's like to feel normal. This is my world. Things don't
get me as excited as most people because I'm in a constant state of
sedation. It should never have been prescribed for long-term use.”
Susan sees it much the same way. "My sister and I have talked at
length about how our brother is very good-looking, and how when
he is acting normal, you would not know there is anything wrong,"
she says. "He is adorable, charming; he carries on conversations.
He could have been with a nice woman and had a family. But now?
He has no friends. None whatsoever. He stays at home most of the
time, except when he has to go to the store. He is trapped. He can't
get off Klonopin. I feel terrible for him, and I feel terrible for my
dad, who when he dies will never have seen his son do well. It kills
us that he could have had a life."
If a picture is worth a thousand words, the photos that Jill, an Ohio
woman in her mid-thirties, sends me tell her story in a very succinct
fashion. There is the "before” photo in which she is smiling and
looking confidently into the camera, posed like a model in a fashion¬
able black dress. One hand is posed gracefully on her hip, a pearl
necklace adds a touch of elegance, and she is a bit dolled up —the
makeup and styled black hair tell of a woman who presents herself
carefully to the world. And then there is the "after” photo, her eyes
hollowed out and bloodshot, her face taut and drawn, her hair
144
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
thinned—she looks like a somewhat crazed methamphetamine
addict who is now getting her photo taken following an arrest.
We first spoke on the phone in July of 2008, three months after
she had taken her last dose of a benzodiazepine, a drug she had
been on for thirteen years. Here's how she starts her story: "My
head is feeling crushed. It's like horses are kicking my skull."
Jill, who asked that I not use her last name, grew up in an afflu¬
ent suburb of Columbus, Ohio, where she attended private schools
and excelled in multiple ways. She sang competitively, won school
awards for her art, and was a top student. Petite and pretty, she was
asked by a representative of the Miss Ohio pageant to enter that
competition. "I was a vibrant, creative, fun person,” she says. How¬
ever, she did occasionally struggle with anxiety and depression, and
during her sophomore year at Ohio State University a psychiatrist
put her on an antidepressant. Unfortunately, that drug seemed to
increase her anxiety, and so eventually the psychiatrist added
Klonopin to the mix. "He said it was a gentle little pill used to help
old ladies sleep. He said that it wasn't addictive and that if I wanted
to stop, at most I'd experience a few nights of bad sleep. But he said
I would probably need to take it for life, just like a diabetic needs
insulin."
For the next ten years, Jill functioned okay. She graduated
summa cum laude from Ohio State University in 1996, earned a
master's degree in counseling, and after various adventures, in 2002
she began teaching fourth grade in a public school. However,
throughout this period, her anxiety returned again and again, and
each time it did, her psychiatrist upped her dose of Klonopin. And
as the dose increased, her ability to function declined. "I would
wonder, What is wrong with me? Why am I becoming so with¬
drawn? Why am I losing interest in everything? I was getting sicker
and sicker.” Then, in late 2004, the anxiety, panic, and depression
returned worse than ever, and new symptoms —obsessions and sui¬
cidal ideation —appeared too. She was told this meant she was
"bipolar" and she was prescribed an antipsychotic, Abilify. "That's
when I flipped out. My anxiety went through the roof, it was like
being injected with stimulants, and I was teaching one day and I
THE BENZO TRAP
145
started crying in class. I couldn't take it anymore, and I was hospi¬
talized in a psychiatric ward.”
Now came the drug merry-go-round. During the next two years,
Jill was put on Lamictal, Lexapro, Seroquel, Neurontin, lithium,
Wellbutrin, and other drugs she can't remember, with Klonopin al¬
ways part of the cocktail. This treatment caused her eyes to swell,
her skin to break into rashes, and her eyebrows and hair to fall out.
"My poor brain was being treated like a mixing bowl," she says.
Only when she asked doctors whether the cocktail might be making
her sick, "they would say, 'We have tried the drugs and they are not
helping, and so the problem is you.' " Indeed, since the drugs
weren't working, her psychiatrists gave her electroshock, which
took its toll on her memory.
Growing ever more desperate, toward the end of 2006 Jill con¬
cluded that "it was the drugs that were making me sick.” She began
withdrawing from the medications one by one, and although she
was able to get off the antidepressants and antipsychotics, every
time she tried tapering off Klonopin she suffered a long list of tor¬
ments: hallucinations, horrible anxiety, vertigo, painful muscle
spasms, perceptual distortions, and derealization, just to name a
few. Finally, in the spring of 2008, she adopted a new strategy: She
would get off by progressively switching to less potent benzodi¬
azepines. Klonopin was replaced by Valium, the Valium by Librium,
and then, in April 2008, she withdrew from Librium. She was now
drug free, yet three months later, when I spoke to her on the phone,
she was still in withdrawal torment. "What I've been through . . .
the trauma," she says, breaking into tears. "I feel dizzy all the time.
It is like the floor is tilting one way and I am spinning the other
way. It is horrific. I have had hallucinations, I have to wear sun¬
glasses in the house, sometimes I scream from the pain."
At the end of our interview, I asked her to think back to what her
life had been like before she was put on a benzodiazepine, and once
more she began to cry.
"My anxiety then was like a mild case of asthma, and today it's
like I have end-stage lung disease. I'm terrified that I'm not going to
make it. I'm so, so scared.”
146
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Those interviews provide a snapshot of three lives, and several
months later I spoke to each of the subjects again to see if anything
had changed. Geraldine was doing much the same. Hal had become
much more distraught. The Klonopin no longer seemed to be work¬
ing, his anxiety had returned with a vengeance, and he felt physi¬
cally sick. "I've come to accept this is my life," he said, his voice
filled with what seemed like bottomless despair. There was, how¬
ever, an encouraging postscript to Jill's story. Not long after our
phone interview, her withdrawal symptoms began to abate, and in
early 2009, she had this to report: The hallucinations, the vertigo,
the seizures, the hair loss, and the blurry vision had all disappeared.
The muscle spasms, the tinnitus, and the hypersensitivity to light
and noise had become less severe. The feeling that her head was
"packed in cement" had lessened.
"I have a few good days now, and my bad days are not all that
bad anymore,” she says. "I think I can see the light at the end of the
tunnel. There is no doubt I am going to be better. I am going to
move to Boston, and although I'll have to start from scratch, I know
it will be okay. I now value life like nobody else I know. I enjoy
being able to walk in a straight line again, and being able to see
again, and even having a normal heartbeat. My hair is beginning to
come back. I am getting better; I am just waiting for the cement to
completely leave my brain."
The Disability Numbers
At least to a degree, we can track the toll that the anti-anxiety drugs
have taken over the past fifty years. As was noted at the beginning
of this chapter, once the Miltown craze erupted, the number of peo¬
ple turning up at mental hospitals, outpatient centers, and residen¬
tial facilities for the mentally ill began to sharply rise. The U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services dubs this number "pa¬
tient care episodes,” and it soared from 1.66 million in 1955 to 6.86
THE BENZO TRAP
147
million in 1975, when Valiumania was near its peak. 50 On a per-
capita basis, that was an increase from 1,028 patient-care episodes
per 100,000 people to 3,182 per 100,000, a threefold jump in
twenty years. While many factors may have contributed to that in¬
crease (the emotional struggles that some Vietnam veterans experi¬
enced is one possibility that comes to mind, and illicit drug use is a
second), Valiumania was clearly a major one. In the late 1970s,
Betty Ford's physician, Joseph Pursch, concluded that benzodi¬
azepines were the "nation's number one health problem," and that
was because he knew they were driving people to detox centers,
emergency rooms, and psychiatric wards.
As the personal stories of Geraldine, Hal, and Jill attest, benzodi¬
azepines continue to be a pathway to disability for many. These
three are part of the surge of people with an "affective disorder"
who have swelled the SSI and SSDI rolls in the past twenty years.
Although the Social Security Administration doesn't detail the
number of disabled mentally ill who have anxiety as a primary di¬
agnosis, a 2006 report by the U.S. General Accountability Office
provides a proxy for estimating that number. It noted that 8 percent
of the younger adults (eighteen to twenty-six years old) on the SSI
and SSDI rolls were disabled by anxiety, and if that percentage
holds true for all ages, then there were more than 300,000 adults in
the United States who received government support in 2006 due to
an anxiety disorder. 51 That is roughly sixty times the number of psy¬
choneurotics hospitalized in 1955.
Although it was thirty years ago that governmental review panels
in the United States and the United Kingdom concluded that the
benzodiazepines shouldn't be prescribed long-term, with dozens of
studies subsequently confirming the wisdom of that advice, the pre¬
scribing of benzodiazepines for continual use goes on. Indeed, a
2005 study of anxious patients in the New England area found that
more than half regularly took a benzodiazepine, and many bipolar
patients now take a benzodiazepine as part of a drug cocktail. 52 The
scientific evidence simply doesn't seem to affect the prescribing
habits of many doctors. "The lesson has either never been learned,
or it has passed people by," Malcolm Lader said. 53
8
An Episodic Illness Turns Chronic
"With the range of available treatments for
depression, one might wonder why depression-related
disability is on the rise."
— CAROLYN DEWA,
CENTRE FOR ADDICTION AND MENTAL HEALTH,
ONTARIO (2001)
M-Power in Boston is a peer-run advocacy group for the mentally
ill, and while I was at one of their meetings in April 2 008, a young,
quiet woman came up to me and whispered, "I'd be willing to
talk to you." Red hair fell about her shoulders, and she seemed so
shy as to almost be frightened. Yet when Melissa Sances told me
her story a few days later, she spoke in the most candid manner
possible, her shyness transformed into an introspective honesty so
intense that when she was recounting her struggles growing up in
Sandwich on Cape Cod, she suddenly stopped and said: "I was
unhappy, but I didn't have an awareness that I was depressed.” It
was important that I understood the difference between those two
emotions.
Her unhappiness as a child was comprised of familiar ingredi¬
ents. She felt socially awkward and "different" from other kids at
school, and after her parents divorced when she was eight, she and
her brothers lived with their mother, who struggled with depression.
In middle school, Melissa began to come out of her shell, making
friends and feeling "more normal,” only then she ran head-on into
the torments of puberty. "When I was fourteen, I was overweight, I
had acne. I felt like a social outcast, and the kids at high school were
very cruel. I was called a freak and ugly. I would sit at my desk with
AN EPISODIC ILLNESS TURNS CHRONIC
149
my head down, and my hair pulled over my face, trying to hide
from the world. Every day I woke up feeling like I wanted to die."
Today, Melissa is an attractive woman, and so it is a bit surpris¬
ing to learn of this ugly-duckling moment from her past. But with
her schoolmates taunting her, her childhood unhappiness metamor¬
phosed into a deep depression, and when she was sixteen, she tried
to commit suicide by gulping down handfuls of Benadryl and Val¬
ium. She woke up in the hospital, where she was told that she had a
mental illness and was prescribed an antidepressant. "The psychia¬
trist tells me that it adjusts serotonin levels, and that I will probably
have to be on it for the rest of my life. I cried when I heard that."
For a time, Zoloft worked great. "I was like a new person,”
Melissa recalls. "I became open to people, and I made a lot of
friends. I was the pitcher on the softball team.” During her senior
year, she began making plans to attend Emerson College in Boston,
thinking that she would study creative writing. Only then, slowly
but surely, Zoloft's magic started to fade. Melissa began to take
higher doses to keep her depression at bay, and eventually her psy¬
chiatrist switched her to a very high dose of Paxil, which left her
feeling like a zombie. "I was out of it. During a softball game, some¬
one hit a ground ball to me and I just held the ball. I didn't know
what to do with it. I told my team I was sorry."
Melissa has struggled with depression ever since. It followed her
to college, first to Emerson and then to UMass Dartmouth, and al¬
though it did lift somewhat when she became immersed in writing
for the UMass newspaper, it never entirely went away. She tried this
drug and that drug, but none brought any lasting relief. After grad¬
uating, she found a job as an editorial assistant at a magazine, but
depression caught up with her there, too, and in late 2007, the gov¬
ernment deemed her eligible to receive SSDI because of her illness.
"I have always been told that a person has to accept that the ill¬
ness is chronic,” she says, at the end of our interview. "You can be
'in recovery,' but you can never be 'recovered.' But I don't want to
be on disability forever, and I have started to question whether
depression is really a chemical thing. What are the origins of my de¬
spair? How can I really help myself? I want to honor the other parts
of me, other than the sick part that I'm always thinking about.
150
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
I think that depression is like a weed that I have been watering,
and I want to pull up that weed, and I am starting to look to people
for solutions. I really don't know what the drugs did for me all these
years, but I do know that I am disappointed in how things have
turned out."
Such is Melissa Sances's story. Today it is a fairly common one. A
distressed teenager is diagnosed with depression and put on an
antidepressant, and years later he or she is still struggling with the
condition. But if we return to the 1950s, we will discover that de¬
pression rarely struck someone as young as Melissa, and it rarely
turned into the chronic suffering that she has experienced. Her
course of illness is, for the most part, unique to our times.
The Way Depression Used to Be
Melancholy, of course, visits nearly everyone now and then. ”1 am a
man, and that is reason enough to be miserable," wrote the Greek
poet Menander in the fourth century B . C ., a sentiment that has been
echoed by writers and philosophers ever since. 2 In his seventeenth-
century tome Anatomy of Melancholy, English physician Robert
Burton advised that everyone "feels the smart of it ... it is most ab¬
surd and ridiculous for any mortal man to look for a perpetual
tenure of happiness in this life.” It was only when such gloomy
states became a "habit,” Burton said, that they became a "dis¬
ease." 3
This was the same distinction that Hippocrates had made more
than two thousand years earlier, when he identified persistent
melancholy as an illness, attributing it to an excess of black bile
(melaina chole in Greek). Symptoms included "sadness, anxiety,
moral dejection, [and] tendency to suicide” accompanied by "pro¬
longed fear." To curb the excess of black bile and bring the four
humors of the body back into balance, Hippocrates recommended
the administration of mandrake and hellebore, changes in diet, and
the use of cathartic and emetic herbs. 4
During the Middle Ages, the deeply melancholic person was seen
AN EPISODIC
LLNESS TURNS CHRONIC
IS 1
as possessed by demons. Priests and exorcists would be called upon
to drive out the devils. With the arrival of the Renaissance in the fif¬
teenth century, the teachings of the Greeks were rediscovered, and
physicians once again offered medical explanations for persistent
melancholy. After William Harvey discovered in 1628 that blood
circulated throughout the body, many European doctors reasoned
that this illness arose from a lack of blood to the brain.
Psychiatry's modern conception of depression has its roots in
Emil Kraepelin's work. In his 1899 book, Lehrbuch der Psychiatrie,
Kraepelin divided psychotic disorders into two broad categories —
dementia praecox and manic-depressive psychosis. The latter cate¬
gory was mostly comprised of three subtypes —depressive episode
only, manic episode only, and episodes of both kinds. But whereas
dementia praecox patients deteriorated over time, the manic-
depressive group had fairly good long-term outcomes. "Usually all
morbid manifestations completely disappear; but where that is
exceptionally not the case, only a rather slight, peculiar psychic
weakness develops,” Kraepelin explained in a 1921 text.'
Today, Kraepelin's depression-only group would be diagnosed
with unipolar depression, and in the 1960s and early 1970s, promin¬
ent psychiatrists at academic medical centers and at the NIMH de¬
scribed this disorder as fairly rare and having a good long-term
course. In her 1968 book, The Epidemiology of Depression, Char¬
lotte Silverman, who directed epidemiology studies for the NIMH,
noted that community surveys in the 1930s and 1940s had found
that fewer than one in a thousand adults suffered an episode of clin¬
ical depression each year. Furthermore, most who were struck did
not need to be hospitalized. In 1955, there were only 7,250 "first
admissions” for depression in state and county mental hospital's.
The total number of depressed patients in the nation's mental hospi¬
tals that year was around 38,200, a disability rate of one in every
4,345 people. 6
Depression, Silverman and others noted, was primarily an "ail¬
ment of middle aged and older persons." In 1956, 90 percent of the
first-admissions to public and private hospitals for depression were
thirty-five years and older. 7 Depressive episodes, explained Balti¬
more psychiatrist Frank Ayd Jr., in his 1962 book, Recognizing the
152
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Depressed Patient, "occur most often after age thirty, have a peak
incidence between age 40 and 60, and taper off sharply thereafter." 8
Although the manic-depressive patients that Kraepelin studied
were severely ill, as their minds were also buffeted by psychotic
symptoms, their long-term outcomes were pretty good. Sixty per¬
cent of Kraepelin's 450 "depressed-only” patients experienced but a
single episode of depression, and only 13 percent had three or more
episodes.’ Other investigators in the first half of the twentieth cen¬
tury reported similar outcomes. In 1931, Horatio Pollock, of the
New York State Department of Mental Hygiene, in a long-term
study of 2,700 depressed patients hospitalized from 1909 to 1920,
reported that more than half of those admitted for a first episode
had but a single attack, and only 17 percent had three or more
episodes. 10 Thomas Rennie, who investigated the fate of 142 de-
pressives admitted to Johns Hopkins Hospital from 1913 to 1916,
determined that 39 percent had "lasting recoveries" of five years or
more.” A Swedish physician, Gunnar Lundquist, followed 216 pa¬
tients treated for depression for eighteen years, and he determined
that 49 percent never experienced a second attack, and that another
21 percent had only one other episode. In total, 76 percent of the
216 patients became "socially healthy" and resumed their usual
work. After a person has recovered from a depressive episode,
Lundquist wrote, he "has the same capacity for work and prospects
of getting on in life as before the onset of the disease." 12
These good outcomes spilled over into the first years of the anti¬
depressant era. In 1972, Samuel Guze and Eli Robins at Washington
University Medical School in St. Louis reviewed the scientific litera¬
ture and determined that in follow-up studies that lasted ten years,
50 percent of people hospitalized for depression had no recurrence
of their illness. Only a small minority of those with unipolar depres¬
sion-one in ten —became chronically ill, Guze and Robins con¬
cluded. 13
That was the scientific evidence that led NIMH officials during
the 1960s and 1970s to speak optimistically about the long-term
course of the illness. "Depression is, on the whole, one of the psy¬
chiatric conditions with the best prognosis for eventual recovery
with or without treatment. Most depressions are self-limited,"
AN EPISODIC ILLNESS TURNS CHRONIC
153
Jonathan Cole wrote in 1964.” "In the treatment of depression,"
explained Nathan Kline that same year, "one always has as an ally
the fact that most depressions terminate in spontaneous remissions.
This means that in many cases regardless of what one does the
patient eventually will begin to get better." 16 George Winokur, a
psychiatrist at Washington University, advised the public in 1969
that "assurance can be given to a patient and to his family that sub¬
sequent episodes of illness after a first mania or even a first depres¬
sion will not tend toward a more chronic course." 16
Indeed, as Dean Schuyler, head of the depression section at the
NIMH explained in a 1974 book, spontaneous recovery rates were
so high, exceeding 50 percent within a few months, that it was diffi¬
cult to "judge the efficacy of a drug, a treatment [electroshock]
or psychotherapy in depressed patients.” Perhaps a drug or electro¬
shock could shorten the time to recovery, as spontaneous remission
often took many months to happen, but it would be difficult for any
treatment to improve on the natural long-term course of depression.
Most depressive episodes, Schuyler explained, "will run their course
and terminate with virtually complete recovery without specific in¬
tervention."”
Short-Term Blues
The history of trials on the short-term efficacy of antidepressants is
a fascinating one, for it reveals much about the capacity of a society
and a medical profession to cling to a belief in the magical merits of
a pill, even though clinical trials produce, for the most part, dispir¬
iting results. The two antidepressants developed in the 1950s, ipro¬
niazid and imipramine, gave birth to two broad types of drugs for
depression, known as monamine oxidase inhibitors (MAOIs) and
tricyclics, and studies in the late 1950s and early 1960s found both
kinds to be wonderfully effective. However, the studies were of du¬
bious quality, and in 1965, the British Medical Council put both
types through a more rigorous test. While the tricyclic (imipramine)
was modestly superior to placebo, the MAOI (phenelzine) was not.
Treatment with this drug was "singularly unsuccessful." 18
154
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Four years later, the NIMH conducted a review of all anti¬
depressant studies, and it found that the "more stringently controlled
the study, the lower the improvement rate reported for a drug." In
well-controlled studies, 61 percent of the drug-treated patients im¬
proved versus 46 percent of the placebo patients, a net benefit of
only 15 percent. "The differences between the effectiveness of anti¬
depressant drugs and placebo are not impressive," it said. 19 The
NIMH then conducted its own trial of imipramine, and it was only
in psychotically depressed patients that this tricyclic showed any
significant benefit over a placebo. Only 40 percent of the drug-
treated patients completed the seven-week study, and the reason so
many dropped out was that their condition "deteriorated.” For
many depressed patients, the NIMH concluded in 1970, "drugs
play a minor role in influencing the clinical course of their illness." 10
The minimal efficacy of imipramine and other antidepressants
led some investigators to wonder whether the placebo response was
the mechanism that was helping people feel better. What the drugs
did, several speculated, was amplify the placebo response, and they
did so because they produced physical side effects, which helped
convince patients that they were getting a "magic pill” for depres¬
sion. To test this hypothesis, investigators conducted at least seven
studies in which they compared a tricyclic to an "active” placebo,
rather than an inert one. (An active placebo is a chemical that pro¬
duces an unpleasant side effect of some kind, like dry mouth.) In six
of the seven, there was no difference in outcomes. 11
That was the efficacy record racked up by tricyclics in the 1970s:
slightly better than inactive placebo, but no better than an active
placebo. The NIMH visited this question of imipramine's efficacy
one more time in the 1980s, comparing it to two forms of psy¬
chotherapy and placebo, and found that nothing had changed. At
the end of sixteen weeks, "there were no significant differences
among treatments, including placebo plus clinical management, for
the less severely depressed and functionally impaired patients.”
Only the severely depressed patients fared better on imipramine
than on a placebo. 21
Societal belief in the efficacy of antidepressants was reborn
with the arrival of Prozac in 1988. Eli Lilly, it seemed, had come up
AN EPISODIC ILLNESS TURNS CHRONIC
155
with a very good pill for the blues. This selective serotonin reuptake
inhibitor (SSRI) was said to make people feel "better than well.”
Unfortunately, once researchers began poking through the clinical
trial data submitted to the FDA for Prozac and the other SSRIs that
were subsequently brought to market, the "wonder drug” story fell
apart.
The first blow to the SSRIs' image came from Arif Khan at the
Northwest Clinical Research Center in Washington. He reviewed
the study data submitted to the FDA for seven SSRIs and con¬
cluded that symptoms were reduced 42 percent in patients treated
with tricyclics, 41 percent in the SSRI group, and 31 percent in
those given a placebo." The new drugs, it turned out, were no
more effective than the old ones. Next, Erick Turner from Oregon
Health and Science University, in a review of FDA data for twelve
antidepressants approved between 1987 and 2004, determined that
thirty-six of the seventy-four trials had failed to show any statisti¬
cal benefit for the antidepressants. There were just as many trials
that had produced negative or "questionable” results as positive
ones. 24 Finally, in 2008, Irving Kirsch, a psychologist at the Univer¬
sity of Hull in the United Kingdom, found that in the trials of
Prozac, Effexor, Serzone, and Paxil, symptoms in the medicated
patients dropped 9.6 points on the Hamilton Rating Scale of
Depression, versus 7.8 points for the placebo group. This was a
difference of only 1.8 points, and the National Institute for Clinical
Excellence in Britain had previously determined that a three-point
drug-placebo difference was needed on the Hamilton scale to
demonstrate a "clinically significant benefit." It was only in a small
subgroup of patients —those most severely depressed —that the
drugs had been shown to be of real use. "Given these data, there
seems little evidence to support the prescription of antidepressant
medication to any but the most severely depressed patients, unless
alternative treatments have failed to provide benefit,” Kirsch and
his collaborators concluded. 25
All of this provoked some soul-searching by psychiatrists in their
journals. Randomized clinical trials, admitted a 2009 editorial in
the British Journal of Psychiatry, had generated "limited valid evi¬
dence” for use of the drugs. 26 A group of European psychiatrists
156
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
affiliated with the World Health Organization conducted their own
review of Paxil's clinical data and concluded that "among adults
with moderate to severe major depression," this popular SSRI "was
not superior to placebo in terms of overall treatment effectiveness
and acceptability." 2 ’ Belief in these medications' effectiveness, wrote
Greek psychiatrist John Ioannidis, who has an appointment at Tufts
University School of Medicine in Massachusetts, was a "living
myth." A review of the SSRI clinical data had led to a depressing
end for psychiatry, and, as Ioannidis quipped, he and his colleagues
couldn't even now turn to Prozac and the other SSRIs for relief from
this dispiriting news because, alas, "they probably won't work." 28
There is one other interesting addendum to this research history.
In the late 1980s, many Germans who were depressed turned to Hy¬
pericum perforatum, the plant known as Saint-John's-wort, for re¬
lief. German investigators began conducting double-blind trials of
this herbal remedy, and in 1996, the British Medical Journal sum¬
marized the evidence: In thirteen placebo-controlled trials, 55 per¬
cent of the patients treated with Saint-John's-wort significantly
improved, compared with 22 percent of those given a placebo. The
herbal remedy also bested antidepressants in head-to-head competi¬
tion: In those trials, 66 percent given the herb improved compared
to 55 percent of the drug-treated patients. In Germany, Saint-
John's-wort was effective. But would it work similar magic in Amer¬
icans? In 2001, psychiatrists at eleven medical centers in the United
States reported that it wasn't effective at all. Only 15 percent of the
depressed outpatients treated with the herb improved in their eight-
week trial. Yet —and this was the curious part —only 5 percent of
the placebo patients got better in this study, far below the usual
placebo response. American psychiatrists, it seemed, were not eager
to see anyone as having gotten better, lest the herb prove effective.
But then the NIH funded a second trial of Saint-John's-wort that
had a design that complicated matters for any .researcher who
wanted to play favorites. It compared Saint-John's-wort to both
Zoloft and a placebo. Since the herb causes side effects, such as dry
mouth, it would act at the very least as an active placebo. As such,
this truly was a blinded trial, the psychiatrists unable to rely on side
effects as a clue to which patients were getting what, and here were
AN EPISODIC ILLNESS TURNS CHRONIC
157
the results: Twenty-four percent of the patients treated with Saint-
John's-wort had a "full response,” 25 percent of the Zoloft patients,
and 32 percent of the placebo group. "This study fails to support
the efficacy of H perforatum in moderately severe depression," the
investigators concluded, glossing over the fact that their drug had
failed this test too. 29
The Chronicity Factor, Yet Again
The antidepressants' relative lack of short-term efficacy was not, by
itself, a reason to think that the drugs were causing harm. After all,
most of those treated with antidepressants were seeing their symp¬
toms abate. Medicated patients in the short-term trials were getting
better. The problem was that they were not improving significantly
more than those treated with a placebo. However, during the 1960s,
several European psychiatrists reported that the long-term course of
depression in their drug-treated patients seemed to be worsening.
Exposure to antidepressants, wrote German physician H. P. Ho-
heisel in 1966, appeared to be "shortening the intervals" between
depressive episodes in his patients. These drugs, wrote a Yugosla¬
vian doctor four years later, were causing a "chronification" of the
disease. The tricyclics, agreed Bulgarian psychiatrist Nikola Schip-
kowensky in 1970, were inducing a "change to a more chronic
course.” The problem, it seemed, was that many people treated
with antidepressants were only "partially cured." 50 Their symptoms
didn't entirely remit, and then, when they stopped taking the anti¬
depressant, their depression regularly got much worse again.
With this concern having surfaced in a few European journals, a
Dutch physician, J. D. Van Scheyen, examined the case histories of
ninety-four depressed patients. Some had taken an antidepressant
and some had not, and when Van Scheyen looked at how the two
groups had fared over a five-year period, the difference was star¬
tling: "It was evident, particularly in the female patients, that more
systematic long-term antidepressant medication, with or without
ECT [electroconvulsive therapy], exerts a paradoxical effect on the
158
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
recurrent nature of the vital depression. In other words, this thera¬
peutic approach was associated with an increase in recurrent rate
and a decrease in cycle duration. . . . Should [this increase] be re¬
garded as an untoward long-term side effect of treatment with
tricyclic antidepressants?" 31
Over the next twenty years, investigators reported again and
again that people treated with an antidepressant were very likely to
relapse once they stopped taking the drug. In 1973, investigators in
Britain wrote that 50 percent of drug-withdrawn patients relapsed
within six months; 32 a few years later, investigators at the University
of Pennsylvania announced that 69 percent of patients withdrawn
from antidepressants relapsed within this time period. There was,
they confessed, "rapid clinical deterioration in most of the pa¬
tients." 33 In 1984, Robert Prien at the NIMH reported that 71 per¬
cent of depressed patients relapsed within eighteen months of drug
withdrawal. 34 Finally, in 1990, the NIMH added to this gloomy pic¬
ture when it reported the long-term results from its study that had
compared imipramine to two forms of psychotherapy and to a
placebo. At the end of eighteen months, the stay-well rate was best
for the cognitive therapy group (30 percent) and lowest for the
imipramine-exposed group (19 percent). 35
Everywhere, the message was the same: Depressed people who
were treated with an antidepressant and then stopped taking it reg¬
ularly got sick again. In 1997, Ross Baldessarini from Harvard
Medical School, in a meta-analysis of the literature, quantified the
relapse risk: Fifty percent of drug-withdrawn patients relapsed
within fourteen months. 36 Baldessarini also found that the longer a
person was on an antidepressant, the greater the relapse rate fol¬
lowing drug withdrawal. It was as though a person treated with the
drug gradually became less and less able, in a physiological sense, to
do without it. Investigators in Britain came to the same sobering re¬
alization: "After stopping an antidepressant, symptoms tend to
build up gradually and become chronic." 32
AN EPISODIC
LLNESS TURNS CHRONIC
159
Do All Psychotropics Work This Way?
Although a handful of European physicians may have sounded the
alarm about the changing course of depression in the late 1960s and
early 1970s, it wasn't until 1994 that an Italian psychiatrist, Gio¬
vanni Fava, from the University of Bologna, pointedly announced
that it was time for psychiatry to confront this issue. Neuroleptics
had been found to be quite problematic over the long term, the ben¬
zodiazepines had, too, and now it looked like the antidepressants
were producing a similar long-term record. In a 1994 editorial in
Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, Fava wrote:
Within the field of psychopharmacology, practitioners have
been cautious, if not fearful, of opening a debate on whether
the treatment is more damaging [than helpful].... I wonder
if the time has come for debating and initiating research into
the likelihood that psychotropic drugs actually worsen, at
least in some cases, the progression of the illness which they
are supposed to treat. 38
In this editorial and several more articles that followed, Fava of¬
fered a biological explanation for what was going on with the anti¬
depressants. Like antipsychotics and benzodiazepines, these drugs
perturb neurotransmitter systems in the brain. This leads to com¬
pensatory "processes that oppose the initial acute effects of a
drug. . . . When drug treatment ends, these processes may operate
unopposed, resulting in appearance of withdrawal symptoms and
increased vulnerability to relapse,” he wrote. 35 Moreover, Fava
noted, pointing to Baldessarini's findings, it was evident that the
longer one stayed on antidepressants, the worse the problem.
"Whether one treats a depressed patient for three months, or three
years, it does not matter when one stops the drugs. A statistical
trend suggested that the longer the drug treatment, the higher the
likelihood of relapse." 40
But, Fava also wondered, what was the outcome for people who
stayed on antidepressants indefinitely? Weren't they also relapsing
I6o • ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
with great frequency? Perhaps the drugs cause "irreversible receptor
modifications," Fava said, and, as such, "sensitize” the brain to de¬
pression. This could explain the "bleak long term outcome of
depression." He summed up the problem in this way:
Antidepressant drugs in depression might be beneficial in the
short term, but worsen the progression of the disease in
the long term, by increasing the biochemical vulnerability to
depression. . . . Use of antidepressant drugs may propel the
illness to a more malignant and treatment unresponsive
course. 41
This possibility was now front and center in psychiatry. "His
question and the several related matters . . . are not pleasant to
contemplate and may seem paradoxical, but they now require open-
minded and serious clinical and research consideration,” Baldessarini
said. 42 Three physicians from the University of Louisville School of
Medicine echoed the sentiment. "Long-term antidepressant use may
be depressogenic,” they wrote, in a 1998 letter to the Journal of
Clinical Psychiatry. "It is possible that antidepressant agents modify
the hardwiring of neuronal synapses [which] not only render anti¬
depressants ineffective but also induce a resident, refractory depres¬
sive state." 43
It's the Disease, Not the Drug
Once again, psychiatry had reached a moment of crisis. The specter
of supersensitivity psychosis had stirred up a hornets' nest in the
early 1980s, and now, in the mid-1990s, a concern very similar in
kind had appeared. This time, the stakes were perhaps even higher.
Fava was raising this issue even as U.S. sales of SSRIs were soaring.
Prominent psychiatrists at the best medical schools in the United
States had told newspaper and magazine reporters of their wonders.
These drugs were now being prescribed to an ever-larger group of
people, including to more than a million American children. Could
AN EPISODIC ILLNESS TURNS CHRONIC
• 161
the field now confess that these medications might be making peo¬
ple chronically depressed? That they led to a "malignant" long-term
course? That they caused biological changes in the brain that "sen¬
sitized” a person to depression? And if that were so, how could they
possibly be prescribed to young children and teenagers? Why would
doctors do that to children? This concern of Fava's needed to be
hushed up, and hushed up fast. Early in 1994, after Fava first
broached the subject, Donald Klein from Columbia University told
Psychiatric News that this subject was not going to be investigated.
"The industry is not interested [in this question], the NIMF1 is
not interested, and the FDA is not interested," he said. "Nobody is
interested." 44
Indeed, by this time, leaders of American psychiatry were already
coming up with an alternative explanation for the "bleak" long¬
term outcomes, one that spared their drugs any blame. The old epi¬
demiological studies from the pre-antidepressant era, which had
shown that people regularly recovered from a severe depressive
episode and that a majority then stayed well, were "flawed.” A
panel of experts convened by the NIMH put it this way: "Improved
approaches to the description and classification of [mood] disorders
and new epidemiologic studies [have] demonstrated the recurrent
and chronic nature of these illnesses, and the extent to which they
represent a continual source of distress and dysfunction for affected
individuals." 45 Depression was at last being understood, that was
the story that psychiatry embraced, and textbooks were rewritten to
tell of this advance in knowledge. Not long ago, noted the 1999 edi¬
tion of the American Psychiatric Association's Textbook of Psychia¬
try, it was believed that "most patients would eventually recover
from a major depressive episode. However, more extensive studies
have disproved this assumption." 45 It was now known, the APA said,
that "depression is a highly recurrent and pernicious disorder.”
Depression, it seemed, had never been the relatively benign illness
described by Silverman and others at the NIMH in the late 1960s
and early 1970s. And with depression reconceived in this way, as a
chronic illness, psychiatry now had a rationale for long-term use
of antidepressants. The problem wasn't that exposure to an
antidepressant caused a biological change that made people more
162.
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
vulnerable to depression; the problem was that once the drug was
withdrawn, the disease returned. Moreover, psychiatry did have
studies proving the merits of keeping people on antidepressants.
After all, relapse rates were higher for patients withdrawn from
the medications than for those maintained on the drugs. "Anti¬
depressants reduce the risk of relapse in depressive disorder, and
continued treatment with antidepressants would benefit many pa¬
tients with recurrent depressive disorder,” explained a group of
psychiatrists who reviewed this literature. 47
During the 1990s, psychiatrists in the United States and else¬
where fleshed out the spectrum of outcomes achieved with this new
paradigm of care, which emphasized "maintaining” people on the
medications. One-third of all unipolar patients, researchers con¬
cluded, are "non-responders” to antidepressants. Their symptoms
do not abate over the short term, and this group is said to have
a poor long-term outcome. Another third of unipolar patients
are "partial responders" to antidepressants, and in short-term trials,
they show up as being helped by the drugs. The problem, NIMH in¬
vestigators discovered, in a long-term study called the Collaborative
Program on the Psychobiology of Depression, was that these drug-
maintained patients fared poorly over the long term. "Resolution of
major depressive episode with residual subthreshold depressive
symptoms, even the first lifetime episode, appears to be the first step
of a more severe, relapsing, and chronic future course,” explained
Lewis Judd, a former director of the NIMH, in a 2000 report. 4 ' The
final third of patients see their symptoms remit over the short term,
but only about half of this group, when maintained on an antide¬
pressant, stay well for long periods of time. 49
In short, two-thirds of patients initially treated with an antide¬
pressant can expect to have recurrent bouts of depression, and only
a small percentage of people can be expected to recover and stay
well. "Only 15% of people with unipolar depression experience a
single bout of the illness," the APA's 1999 textbook noted, and for
the remaining 85 percent, with each new episode, remissions be¬
come "less complete and new recurrences develop with less provo¬
cation." 50 This outcomes data definitely told of a pernicious
disorder, but then John Rush, a prominent psychiatrist at Texas
AN EPISODIC
LLNESS TURNS CHRONIC
• 163
Southwestern Medical Center in Dallas, suggested that "real-world
outcomes" were even worse. Those outcome statistics arose from
clinical trials that had cherry-picked patients most likely to respond
well to an antidepressant, he said. "Longer-term clinical outcomes
of representative outpatients with nonpsychotic major depressive
disorder treated in daily practice in either the private or public sec¬
tors are yet to be well defined." 51
In 2004, Rush and his colleagues filled in this gap in the medical
literature. They treated 118 "real world” patients with antidepres¬
sants and provided them with a wealth of emotional and clinical
support "specifically designed to maximize clinical outcomes." This
was the best care that modern psychiatry could provide, and here
were their real-world results: Only 26 percent of the patients even
responded to the antidepressant (meaning that their symptoms de¬
creased at least 50 percent on a rating scale), and only about half of
those who responded stayed better for any length of time. Most
startling of all, only 6 percent of the patients saw their depression
fully remit and stay away during the yearlong trial. These "findings
reveal remarkably low response and remission rates," Rush said. 52
This dismal picture of real-world outcomes was soon confirmed
by a large NIMH study known as the STAR*D trial, which Rush
helped direct. Most of the 4,041 real-world outpatients enrolled in
the trial were only moderately ill, and yet fewer than 20 percent re¬
mitted and stayed well for a year. "Most individuals with major de¬
pressive disorders have a chronic course, often with considerable
symptomatology and disability even between episodes,” the investi¬
gators concluded. 53
In the short span of forty years, depression had been utterly
transformed. Prior to the arrival of the drugs, it had been a fairly
rare disorder, and outcomes generally were good. Patients and their
families could be reassured that it was unlikely that the emotional
problem would turn chronic. It just took time —six to twelve
months or so —for the patient to recover. Today, the NIMH informs
the public that depressive disorders afflict one in ten Americans
every year, that depression is "appearing earlier in life” than it did
in the past, and that the long-term outlook for those it strikes is
glum. "An episode of major depression may occur only once in a
164
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
person's lifetime, but more often, it recurs throughout a person's
life," the NIMH warns. 54
Unmedicated v. Medicated Depression
We've now arrived at an intellectual place similar to what we expe¬
rienced with the antipsychotics: Can it really be that antidepres¬
sants, which are so popular with the public, worsen long-term
outcomes? All of the data we've reviewed so far indicates that the
drugs do just that, but there is one piece of evidence that we are
still missing: What does unmedicated depression look like today?
Does it run a better long-term course? Unfortunately, as researchers
from the University of Ottawa discovered in 2008, there aren't
good-quality randomized trials comparing long-term outcomes in
antidepressant-treated and never-medicated patients. As such, they
concluded, randomized trials "provide no guidance for longer treat¬
ment." 55 However, we can search for "naturalistic" studies that
might help us answer this question.' 1 '
Researchers in the UK, the Netherlands, and Canada investigated
this question by looking back at case histories of depressed patients
whose medication use had been tracked. In a 1997 study of out¬
comes at a large inner-city facility, British scientists reported that
ninety-five never-medicated patients saw their symptoms decrease
by 62 percent in six months, whereas the fifty-three drug-treated pa¬
tients experienced only a 33 percent reduction in symptoms. The
medicated patients, they concluded, "continued to have depressive
symptoms throughout the six months." 56 Dutch investigators, in a
retrospective study of the ten-year outcomes of 222 people who had
* The caveat with the naturalistic studies is that the unmedicated cohort, at the
moment of initial diagnosis, may not be as depressed as those who go on drugs.
Furthermore, those who eschew drugs may also have a greater "inner re¬
silience." Even given these caveats, we should be able to gain a sense of the
course of unmedicated depression from the naturalistic studies, and see how it
compares to the course of depression treated with antidepressants.
AN EPISODIC
LLNESS TURNS CHRONIC
165
suffered a first episode of depression, found that 76 percent of those
not treated with an antidepressant recovered and never relapsed,
compared to 50 percent of those prescribed an antidepressant. 57
Finally, Scott Patten, from the University of Calgary, plumbed a
large Canadian health database to assess the five-year outcomes of
9,508 depressed patients, and he determined that the medicated pa¬
tients were depressed on average nineteen weeks each year, versus
eleven weeks for those not taking the drugs. These findings, Patten
wrote, were consistent with Giovanni Fava's hypothesis that "anti¬
depressant treatment may lead to a deterioration in the long-term
course of mood disorders." 58
A study conducted by the World Health Organization in fifteen
cities around the world to assess the value of screening for depres¬
sion led to similar results. The researchers looked for depression in
patients who showed up at health clinics for other complaints, and
then, in a fly-on-the-wall manner, followed those they had identified
as depressed for the next twelve months. They reasoned that the
general practitioners in the clinics would detect depression in some
of the patients but not all, and hypothesized that outcomes would
fall into four groups: those diagnosed and treated with antidepres¬
sants would fare the best, those diagnosed and treated with benzo¬
diazepines would fare the second best, those diagnosed and treated
without psychotropics the third best, and those undetected and un¬
treated the worst. Alas, the results were the opposite. Altogether,
the WHO investigators identified 740 people as depressed, and it
was the 484 who weren't exposed to psychotropic medications
(whether diagnosed or not) that had the best outcomes. They en¬
joyed much better "general health" at the end of one year, their de¬
pressive symptoms were much milder, and a lower percentage were
judged to still be "mentally ill." The group that suffered most from
"continued depression" were the patients treated with an antide¬
pressant. The "study does not support the view that failure to
recognize depression has serious adverse consequences," the investi¬
gators wrote. 59
Next, researchers in Canada and the United States studied
whether antidepressant use affected disability rates. In Canada,
Carolyn Dewa and her colleagues at the Centre for Addiction and
166
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
One-Year Outcomes in WHO Screening Study for Depression
The WHO investigators reported that a higher percentage ofthe unmedicated group recovered,
and th at "conti n u i n g depression" was highest in those treated with an antidepressant. Source:
Goldberg, D.'The effects of detection and treatment of major depression in primary care." British
Journal of General Practice 48 (1 998): 1840-44.
Mental Health in Ontario identified 1,281 people who went on
short-term disability between 1996 and 1998 because they missed
ten consecutive days of work due to depression. The 564 people
who subsequently didn't fill a prescription for an antidepressant re¬
turned to work, on average, in 77 days, while the medicated group
took 105 days to get back on the job. More important, only 9 per¬
cent of the unmedicated group went on to long-term disability, com¬
pared to 19 percent of those who took an antidepressant. 51 * "Does
the lack of antidepressant use reflect a resistance to adopting a sick
role and consequently a more rapid return to work?" Dewa won-
* This study powerfully illustrates why we, as a society, may be deluded about
the merits of antidepressants. Seventy-three percent of those who took an anti¬
depressant returned to work (another 8 percent quit or retired), and undoubt¬
edly many in that group would tell of how the drug treatment helped them.
They would become societal voices attesting to the benefits of this paradigm of
care, and without a study of this kind, there would be no way to know that the
medications were, in fact, increasing the risk of long-term disability.
AN EPISODIC
LLNESS TURNS CHRONIC
• 167
The Risk of Disability for Depressed Patients
0% 20% 40% 60% 80% 100%
This was a study of 1,281 employees in Canada who went on short-term disability due to
depression. Those who took an antidepressant were more than twice as likely to go on to long¬
term disability. Source: Dewa, C "Pattern of antidepressant use and duration of depression-
related absence from work." British Journal of Psychiatry 1 83 (2003): 507-1 3.
dered. 60 In a similar vein. University of Iowa psychiatrist William
Coryell and his NIMH-funded colleagues studied the six-year
"naturalistic" outcomes of 547 people who suffered a bout of de¬
pression, and they found those who were treated for the illness were
three times more likely than the untreated group to suffer a "cessa¬
tion" of their "principal social role” and nearly seven times more
likely to become "incapacitated.” Moreover, while many of the
treated patients saw their economic status markedly decline during
the six years, only 17 percent of the unmedicated group saw their
incomes drop, and 59 percent saw their incomes rise. "The un¬
treated individuals described here had milder and shorter-lived ill¬
nesses [than those who were treated], and, despite the absence of
treatment, did not show significant changes in socioeconomic status
in the long term," Coryell wrote. 61
Several countries also observed that following the arrival of the
SSRIs, the number of their citizens disabled by depression dramati¬
cally increased. In Britain, the "number of days of incapacity" due
to depression and neurotic disorders jumped from 38 million in
1984 to 117 million in 1999, a threefold increase. 62 Iceland re¬
ported that the percentage of its population disabled by depression
168 •
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
NIMH's Study of Untreated Depression
In this study, the NIMH investigated the naturalistic outcomes of people diagnosed with major
depression who got treatment and those who did not. At the end of six years, the treated patients
were much more likely to have stopped functioning in their usual societal roles and to have
become incapacitated. Source: Coryell, W. "Characteristics and significance of untreated major
depressive disorder." American Journal of Psychiatry 152 (1995): 1124-29.
nearly doubled from 1976 to 2000. If antidepressants were truly
helpful, the Iceland investigators reasoned, then the use of these
drugs "might have been expected to have a public health impact
by reducing disability, morbidity, and mortality due to depressive
disorders." 63 In the United States, the percentage of working-age
Americans who said in health surveys that they were disabled by de¬
pression tripled during the 1990s.“
There is one final study we need to review. In 2006, Michael
Posternak, a psychiatrist at Brown University, confessed that "un¬
fortunately, we have little direct knowledge regarding the untreated
course of major depression." The poor long-term outcomes detailed
in APA textbooks and the NIMH studies told the story of medicated
depression, which might be a very different beast.,To study what
untreated depression might be like in modern times, Posternak and
his collaborators identified eighty-four patients enrolled in the
NIMH's Psychobiology of Depression program who, after recover¬
ing from an initial bout of depression, subsequently relapsed but did
not then go back on medication. Although these patients were not a
AN EPISODIC
LLNESS TURNS CHRONIC
• 169
"never-exposed” group, Posternak could still track their "un¬
treated” recovery from this second episode of depression. Here
were the results: Twenty-three percent recovered in one month, 67
percent in six months, and 85 percent within a year. Kraepelin,
Posternak noted, had said that untreated depressive episodes usu¬
ally cleared up within six to eight months, and these results pro¬
vided "perhaps the most methodologically rigorous confirmation of
this estimate." 65
The old epidemiological studies were apparently not so flawed
after all. This study also showed why six-week trials of the drugs
had led psychiatry astray. Although only 23 percent of the unmedi¬
cated patients were recovered after one month, spontaneous remis¬
sions continued after that at the rate of about 2 percent per week,
and thus at the end of six months, two-thirds were depression free.
It takes time for unmedicated depression to lift, and that is missed in
short-term trials. "If as many as 85% of depressed individuals who
go without somatic treatment spontaneously recover within one
year, it would be extremely difficult for any intervention to demon¬
strate a superior result to this," Posternak said. 66
It was just as Joseph Zubin had warned in 1955: "It would be
foolhardy to claim a definite advantage for a specified therapy with¬
out a two- to five-year follow-up." 67
Nine Million and Counting
We can now see how the antidepressant story all fits together, and
why the widespread use of these drugs would contribute to a rise in
the number of disabled mentally ill in the United States. Over the
short term, those who take an antidepressant will likely see their
symptoms lessen. They will see this as proof that the drugs work, as
will their doctors. However, this short-term amelioration of symp¬
toms is not markedly greater than what is seen in patients treated
with a placebo, and this initial use also puts them onto a problem¬
atic long-term course. If they stop taking the medication, they are at
high risk of relapsing. But if they stay on the drugs, they will also
1 7 0 •
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
likely suffer recurrent episodes of depression, and this chronicity in¬
creases the risk that they will become disabled. The SSRIs, to a
certain extent, act like a trap in the same way that neuroleptics do.
We can also track the rise in the number of people disabled by de¬
pression during the antidepressant era. In 1955, there were 38,200
people in the nation's mental hospitals due to depression, a per-
capita disability rate of 1 in 4,345. Today, major depressive disorder
is the leading cause of disability in the United States for people ages
fifteen to forty-four. According to the NIMH, it affects 15 million
American adults, and researchers at Johns Hopkins School of Public
Health reported in 2008 that 58 percent of this group is "severely
impaired." 68 That means nearly nine million adults are now dis¬
abled, to some extent, by this condition.
It's also important to note that this disability doesn't arise solely
from the fact that people treated with antidepressants are at high
risk of suffering recurrent episodes of depression. SSRIs also cause a
multitude of troubling side effects. These include sexual dysfunc¬
tion, suppression of REM sleep, muscle tics, fatigue, emotional
blunting, and apathy. In addition, investigators have reported that
long-term use is associated with memory impairment, problem¬
solving difficulties, loss of creativity, and learning deficiencies. "Our
field," confessed Maurizio Fava and others at Massachusetts
General Hospital in 2006, "has not paid sufficient attention to the
presence of cognitive symptoms emerging or persisting during long¬
term antidepressant treatment. . . . These symptoms appear to be
quite common." 69
Animal studies have also produced alarming results. Rats fed
high doses of SSRIs for four days ended up with neurons that were
swollen and twisted like corkscrews. "We don't know if the cells are
dying,” the researchers from Jefferson Medical College in Philadel¬
phia wrote. "These effects may be transient and reversible. Or they
may be permanent." 70 Other reports have suggested.that the drugs
may reduce the density of synaptic connections in the brain, cause
cell death in the hippocampus, shrink the thalamus, and trigger ab¬
normalities in frontal-lobe function. None of these possibilities has
been well studied or documented, but something is clearly going
AN EPISODIC ILLNESS TURNS CHRONIC
171
amiss if symptoms of cognitive impairment in long-term users of
antidepressants are "quite common."
Melissa
I interviewed a number of people who receive SSI or SSDI due to de¬
pression, and many told stories similar to Melissa Sances's. They
first took an antidepressant when they were in their teens or early
twenties, and the drug worked for a time. But then their depression
returned, and they have struggled with depressive episodes ever
since. Their stories fit to a remarkable degree with the long-term
chronicity detailed in the scientific literature. I also caught up with
Melissa a second time, nine months after our first interview, and her
struggles remained much the same. In the fall of 2008, she started
taking a high dose of a monoamine oxidase inhibitor, which pro¬
vided a few weeks of relief, and then her depression returned with a
vengeance. She was now considering electroshock therapy, and as
we ate lunch at a Thai restaurant, she spoke, in a wistful manner, of
how she wished her treatment could have been different.
"I do wonder what might have happened if [at age sixteen] I
could have just talked to someone, and they could have helped me
learn about what I could do on my own to be a healthy person. I
never had a role model for that. They could have helped me with
my eating problems, and my diet and exercise, and helped me learn
how to take care of myself. Instead, it was you have this problem
with your neurotransmitters, and so here, take this pill Zoloft, and
when that didn't work, it was take this pill Prozac, and when that
didn't work, it was take this pill Effexor, and then when I started
having trouble sleeping, it was take this sleeping pill," she says, her
voice sounding more wistful than ever. "I am so tired of the pills."
9
The Bipolar Boom
"I would like to point out that in the history of
medicine, there are many examples of situations
where the vast majority of physicians did something
that turned out to be wrong. The best example is
bloodletting, which was the most common medical
practice from the first century A.D. until the
nineteenth century."
— NASSIR GHAEMI, TUFTS MEDICAL CENTER,
APA CONFERENCE (2008)
At the American Psychiatric Association's 2008 annual meeting in
Washington, D.C., there were press conferences each day, and dur¬
ing the presentations that told of the great advances that lay ahead,
the leaders of the APA regularly urged the reporters and science
writers in attendance to help "get out the message that [psychiatric]
treatment works and is effective, and that our diseases are real dis¬
eases just like cardiovascular diseases and cancer," said APA presi¬
dent Carolyn Robinowitz. "We need to work together as partners
so we can get the word out to patients and families." The press had
an important role to play, explained incoming president Nada
Logan Stotland, because "the public is vulnerable to misinforma¬
tion." She urged the reporters to "help us inform the public that
psychiatric illnesses are real, psychiatric treatments work, and that
our data is as solid as in other areas of medicine.”
I scribbled all of these quotes in my notebook, even though it
didn't seem that Anatomy of an Epidemic was going to quite fit the
partnership model that the APA had in mind, and then each day I
would go for a stroll in the great exhibit hall, which I always en¬
joyed. Eli Lilly, Pfizer, Bristol-Myers Squibb, and the other leading
vendors of psychiatric drugs all had huge welcoming centers, where,
THE BIPOLAR BOOM
• 173
if you were a doctor, you could collect various trinkets and gifts.
Pfizer's seemed to be the most popular, as the psychiatrists could
pick up a new personalized gift each day, their names printed on a
mini-flashlight one day and a mobile phone charger the next. They
could also win a gift by playing a video game called the Physician's
Race Challenge, the pace of their virtual self racing toward the fin¬
ish line governed by how well they answered questions about the
wonders of Geodon as a treatment for bipolar illness. After playing
that game, many lined up to have their photo taken and stamped on
a campaign button that said: "Best Doctor on Earth."
The best-attended events of the conference were the industry-
sponsored symposiums. At every breakfast, lunch, and dinner hour,
the doctors could enjoy a sumptuous free meal, which was then
followed by talks on the chosen topic. There were symposiums on
depression, ADHD, schizophrenia, and the prescribing of antipsy-
chotics to children and adolescents, and nearly all of the speakers
hailed from top academic schools. The fact that they all were being
paid by the drug companies was openly acknowledged, as the APA,
as part of a new disclosure policy, had published a chart listing all
the ways that pharmaceutical money flowed to these "thought lead¬
ers.” In addition to receiving research monies, most of the "experts"
served as consultants, on "advisory boards,” and as members of
"speakers' bureaus.” Thus, you could see that Joseph Biederman, a
psychiatrist at Massachusetts General Hospital in Boston who, dur¬
ing the 1990s, led the way in popularizing juvenile bipolar disorder,
received research grants from eight firms, acted as a "consultant" to
nine, and served as a "speaker” for eight. His long list of pharma¬
ceutical clients was not all that unusual, and at times, speakers had
to update their information in the disclosure guide when they strode
to the podium, as they had recently added yet another pharmaceuti¬
cal company to their list of clients. After Harvard Medical School's
Jean Frazier dutifully relayed such information, at a symposium de¬
voted to the merits of putting children on multiple psychiatric drugs,
she said, without any apparent hint of irony, "I hope you find my
presentation unbiased.”
The speakers put on very polished presentations, evidence of
the training in public speaking they had received from the
1 7 4 •
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
pharmaceutical firms. They regularly opened with a joke before
moving on to their PowerPoint slides, which were splashed on ball¬
room screens larger than those found in most theaters. Often the
diners were given handheld remote devices to answer multiple-
choice questions during the presentations, with dramatic music
playing as they keyed in their responses, much as it might during
"Final Jeopardy," and when their collective wisdom was splashed
on the screens, most usually got the answer right. "You guys are so
smart,” one speaker said.
Patty Duke provided the 2008 APA meeting with its celebrity pa¬
tient story. AstraZeneca sponsored her talk, and the company
spokesman who introduced her, apparently worried that somehow
the audience might miss the point of what she had to say, informed
everyone that "the take-home message is that mental illness is diag-
nosable and recognizable, and that treatment works." Then the
Oscar-winning actress, clad in a pumpkin orange dress, told of
how she had suffered from undiagnosed bipolar illness for twenty
years, during which time she drank excessively and was sexually
promiscuous. Diagnosis and medication "made me marriage mate¬
rial," she said, and whenever she speaks to patient groups around
the country, she hammers this point home. "I tell them, 'Take your
medicines!' " she said. The drugs fix the disease "with very little
downside!" The audience clapped loudly at that, and then America's
favorite identical cousin offered the psychiatrists a final benediction:
"We are beyond blessed to have people like you who have chosen to
take care of us and to lead us to a balanced life. ... I get my infor¬
mation from you and NAMI [National Alliance on Mental Illness],
and if I resisted such information, I would deserve to have a net
thrown over me. When I hear someone say, at one of my talks, 'I
don't need the medication, I don't take it,' I tell them to 'sit down,
you are making a fool of yourself.'
That led to a standing ovation, and so, as I put away my note¬
book, it seemed certain that this was a meeting where the bottom-
line message, no matter where you went, would be quite well
controlled. Nearly everything was set up and organized in a way
that told of a profession quite confident in its therapeutics, and
while I knew that Martin Harrow would be giving a talk on his
THE BIPOLAR BOOM
175
long-term study of schizophrenia outcomes, he had been allotted
only twenty minutes, and his session had been assigned to one of the
convention center's smallest rooms. His presentation would be the
one exception to the rule, and so I didn't expect to hear anything
startling on the Tuesday afternoon that I squeezed my way into a
crowded, slightly larger room for a forum titled "Antidepressants in
Bipolar Disorder." I figured that the speakers would simply present
trial results that justified, in one way or another, the use of these
drugs, but soon I was writing furiously away. The discussion, which
was led by the top bipolar experts in the country, including the two
grand old men of biological psychiatry in the United States, Freder¬
ick Goodwin and Robert Post, focused on this question: Do anti¬
depressants worsen the long-term course of bipolar disorder? And
notably so?
"The illness has been altered," said Goodwin, who in 1990 coau¬
thored the first edition of his text Manic-Depressive Illness, which is
considered the bible in the field. Today "we have a lot more rapid
cycling than we described in the first edition, a lot more mixed
states than we described in the first edition, a lot more lithium re¬
sistance, and a lot more lithium treatment failure than there was in
the first edition. The illness is not what Kraepelin described any¬
more, and the biggest factor, I think, is that most patients who have
the illness get an antidepressant before they ever get exposed to a
mood stabilizer."
This was the opening salvo in what turned into an hour-long con¬
fessional. Although not all the speakers agreed that antidepressants
had been disastrous for bipolar patients, that was the general
theme, and nobody questioned Goodwin's bottom-line summary
that bipolar outcomes had noticeably worsened in the past twenty
years. Antidepressants, said Nassir Ghaemi, from Tufts Medical
Center, can cause manic switches and turn patients into "rapid
cyclers,” and may increase the amount of time they spend in depres¬
sive episodes. Rapid cycling, Post added, led to a very bad end.
"The number of episodes, and it's a very rich literature [docu¬
menting this], is associated with more cognitive deficits," he said.
"We are building more episodes, more treatment resistance, more
cognitive dysfunction, and there is data showing that if you have
1 7 6 •
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
four depressive episodes, unipolar or bipolar, it doubles your late-
life risk of dementia. And guess what? That isn't even the half of
it. . . . In the United States, people with depression, bipolar, and
schizophrenia are losing twelve to twenty years in life expectancy
compared to people not in the mental health system.”
These were words that told of a paradigm of care that had
completely failed, of treatment that made patients constantly sympto¬
matic and cognitively impaired, and led to their early death as well.
"Now you just heard that one of the things we do doesn't work very
well in the long term," Post practically screamed. "So what the hell
should we be doing?"
The confessions came fast and furious. Psychiatry, of course, had
its "evidence base” for using antidepressants in bipolar disorder,
but, Post said, the clinical trials conducted by pharmaceutical com¬
panies "are virtually useless for us as clinicians. . . . They don't tell
us what we really need to know, what our patients are going to re¬
spond to, and if they don't respond to that first treatment, what
should be the next iteration, and how long they should stay on
things." Only a small percentage of people, he added, actually "re¬
spond to these crummy treatments, like antidepressants." As for
recent pharma-funded trials that had shown that bipolar patients
withdrawn from antipsychotic medications relapsed at high rates,
which theoretically served as evidence that patients needed to take
these drugs long-term, those studies "were designed to get relapse
[in the placebo group]," Goodwin said. "It isn't evidence that the
drug is still needed; it's evidence that if you suddenly change a brain
that has adapted to the drug, you are going to get relapse.” Added
Post: "Right now, fifty years after the advent of antidepressant
drugs, we still don't really know how to treat bipolar depression.
We need new treatment algorithms that aren't just made up.”
This was all much like the moment in The Wizard of Oz when
the curtain is pulled back and the mighty wizard is revealed as a
frail old man. For anyone in the audience who had spent his or her
morning in Pfizer’s welcoming center, answering video-game ques¬
tions about the wonders of Geodon for bipolar illness, it must have
been crushing. Thirty years earlier, Guy Chouinard and Barry Jones
had rattled the profession with their talks on drug-induced
THE BIPOLAR BOOM
• 177
"supersensitivity psychosis," and now the profession was being
asked to confront the fact that bipolar outcomes were worse today
than they had been thirty years earlier, and that antidepressants
were a likely culprit. Stimulants, it seemed, could make bipolar
patients worse too, and at last Ghaemi told the audience that psy¬
chiatry needed to adopt a "Hippocratic” approach to the use of
psychiatric medications, which would require them to stop prescrib¬
ing them unless they had good evidence they were truly beneficial
over the long term. "Diagnosis, not druggery," he said, and at one
point, several in the audience —which had grown increasingly agi¬
tated by this discussion —booed him.
"Can fifty thousand psychiatrists be wrong?" he asked, speaking
about the profession's use of antidepressants as a treatment for
bipolar disorder. "I think that the answer is yes, probably."
Bipolar Before Lithium
Readers of this book, having come this far in the text, cannot be
surprised to learn that outcomes for bipolar disorder have dramatic¬
ally worsened in the pharmacotherapy era. The only surprising
thing is that this failure was so openly discussed at the APA meeting.
Given what the scientific literature revealed about the long-term
outcomes of medicated schizophrenia, anxiety, and depression, it
stood to reason that the drug cocktails used to treat bipolar illness
were not going to produce good long-term results. The increased
chronicity, the functional decline, the cognitive impairment, and the
physical illness —all of these can be expected to show up in people
treated with a cocktail that often includes an antidepressant, an an¬
tipsychotic, a mood stabilizer, a benzodiazepine, and perhaps a
stimulant, too. This was a medical train wreck that could have been
anticipated, and unfortunately, as we trace the history of this story,
the details will seem all too familiar.
Although "bipolar” illness is a diagnosis of recent origin, first
showing up in the APA's Diagnostic and Statistical Manual in 1980
(DSM-III), medical texts dating back to Hippocrates contain
178 •
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
descriptions of patients suffering from alternating episodes of mania
and melancholia. "Melancholia,” wrote German physician Chris¬
tian Vater in the seventeenth century, "often passes into mania and
vice versa. The melancholies now laugh, now are saddened, now ex¬
press numberless other absurd gestures and forms of behaviour.”
The English mad doctor John Haslam told of how "the most furi¬
ous maniacs suddenly sink into a profound melancholy, and the
most depressed and miserable objects become violent and raving.”
In 1854, a French asylum doctor, Jules Baillarger, dubbed this illness
la folk a double forme. It was an uncommon, but recognizable form
of insanity. 1
When Emil Kraepelin published his diagnostic texts, he put these
patients into his manic-depressive group. This diagnostic category
also included patients who suffered from depression or mania only
(as opposed to both), and Kraepelin reasoned that these varied emo¬
tional states all arose from the same underlying disease. The split¬
ting of manic-depressive disorder into separate unipolar and bipolar
factions got its start in 1957, when a German psychiatrist, Karl
Leonhard, determined that the manic form of the illness seemed to
run more in families than the depressive form did. He called the
manic patients "bipolar,” and other researchers then identified addi¬
tional differences between the unipolar and bipolar forms of manic-
depressive illness. Onset occurred earlier in bipolar patients, often
when they were in their twenties, and it also appeared that bipolar
patients were at somewhat higher risk of becoming chronically ill.
In his 1969 book, Manic Depressive Illness, George Winokur at
Washington University in St. Louis treated unipolar depression and
bipolar illness as separate entities, and with this distinction having
been made, he and others began reviewing the literature on manic-
depressive illness to isolate the data on the "bipolar" patients. On av¬
erage, in the older studies, about one-fourth of the manic-depressive
group had suffered from manic episodes and thus were "bipolar." By
all accounts, this was a rare disorder. There were perhaps 12,750
people hospitalized with bipolar illness in 1955, a disability rate of
one in every 13,000 people. 2 That year there were only about 2,400
"first admissions” for bipolar illness in the country's mental
hospitals. 3
THE BIPOLAR BOOM
• 179
As Winokur discovered, the long-term outcomes of the manic pa¬
tients in the pre-drug era had been pretty good. In his 1931 study,
Horatio Pollock reported that 50 percent of the patients admitted to
New York State mental hospitals for a first attack of mania never
suffered a second attack (during an eleven-year follow-up), and only
20 percent experienced three or more episodes. 1 F. I. Wertham, from
Johns Hopkins Medical School, in a 1929 study of two thousand
manic-depressive patients, determined that 80 percent of the manic
group recovered within a year, and that fewer than 1 percent re¬
quired long-term hospitalization. 5 In Gunnar Lundquist's study, 75
percent of the 103 manic patients recovered within ten months, and
during the following twenty years, half of the patients never had an¬
other attack, and only 8 percent developed a chronic course. Eighty-
five percent of the group "socially recovered” and resumed their
former positions. 6 Finally, Ming Tsuang, at the University of Iowa,
studied how eighty-six manic patients admitted to a psychiatric hos¬
pital between 1935 and 1944 fared over the next thirty years, and he
found that nearly 70 percent had good outcomes, which meant they
married, lived in their own homes, and worked. Half were asympto¬
matic during this lengthy follow-up. All in all, the manic patients
had fared as well as the unipolar patients in Tsuang's study. 7
These results, Winokur wrote, revealed that there "was no basis
to consider that manic depressive psychosis permanently affected
those who suffered from it. In this way it is, of course, different
from schizophrenia." While some people suffered multiple episodes
of mania and depression, each episode was usually only "a few
months in duration," and "in a significant number of patients, only
one episode of illness occurs." Most important of all, once patients
recovered from their bipolar episodes, they usually had "no diffi¬
culty resuming their usual occupations." 8
Gateways to Bipolar
Today, according to the NIMH, bipolar illness affects one in every
forty adults in the United States, and so, before we review the
l8o
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
outcomes literature for this disorder, we need to try to understand
this astonishing increase in its prevalence.’ Although the quick-and-
easy explanation is that psychiatry has greatly expanded the diag¬
nostic boundaries, that is only part of the story. Psychotropic
drugs —both legal and illegal —have helped fuel the bipolar boom.
In studies of first-episode bipolar patients, investigators at
McLean Hospital, the University of Pittsburgh, and the University
of Cincinnati Hospital found that at least one-third had used mari¬
juana or some other illegal drug prior to their first manic or psy¬
chotic episode. 10 This substance abuse, the University of Cincinnati
investigators concluded, may "initiate progressively more severe af¬
fective responses, culminating in manic or depressive episodes, that
then become self-perpetuating.” 11 Even the one-third figure may be
low; in 2008, researchers at Mt. Sinai Medical School reported that
nearly two-thirds of the bipolar patients hospitalized at Silver Hill
Hospital in Connecticut in 2005 and 2006 experienced their first
bout of "mood instability" after they had abused illicit drugs. 12
Stimulants, cocaine, marijuana, and hallucinogens were common
culprits. In 2007, Dutch investigators reported that marijuana use
"is associated with a fivefold increase in the risk of a first diagnosis
of bipolar disorder" and that one-third of new bipolar cases in the
Netherlands resulted from it. 13
Antidepressants have also led many people into the bipolar
camp, and to understand why, all we have to do is return to the dis¬
covery of this class of drugs. We see tuberculosis patients treated
with iproniazid dancing in the wards, and while that magazine re¬
port was probably a bit exaggerated, it told of lethargic patients
suddenly behaving in a manic way. In 1956, George Crane pub¬
lished the first report of antidepressant-induced mania, and this
problem has remained present in the scientific literature ever since. 14
In 1985, Swiss investigators tracking changes in the patient mix at
Burgholzli psychiatric hospital in Zurich reported that the percent¬
age with manic symptoms jumped dramatically following the intro¬
duction of antidepressants. "Bipolar disorders increased; more
patients were admitted with frequent episodes,” they wrote. 15 In a
1993 practice guide to depression, the APA confessed that "all anti¬
depressant treatments, including ECT [electroconvulsive therapy],
THE BIPOLAR BOOM
• 181
may provoke manic or hypomanic episodes." 16 A few years later, re¬
searchers at Yale University School of Medicine quantified this risk.
They reviewed the records of 87,290 patients diagnosed with de¬
pression or anxiety between 1997 and 2001 and determined those
treated with antidepressants converted to bipolar at the rate of 7.7
percent per year, which was three times greater than for those not
exposed to the drugs. 17 As a result, over longer periods, 20 to 40
percent of all patients initially diagnosed with unipolar depression
today eventually convert to bipolar illness.” Indeed, in a recent sur¬
vey of members of the Depressive and Manic-Depressive Associa¬
tion, 60 percent of those with a bipolar diagnosis said they had
initially fallen ill with major depression and had turned bipolar
after exposure to an antidepressant. 19
This is data that tells of a process that routinely manufactures
bipolar patients. "If you create iatrogenically a bipolar patient,” ex¬
plained Fred Goodwin, in a 2005 interview in Primary Psychiatry,
"that patient is likely to have recurrences of bipolar illness even if
the offending antidepressant is discontinued. The evidence shows
that once a patient has had a manic episode, he or she is more likely
to have another one, even without the antidepressant stimula¬
tion." 20 Italy's Giovanni Fava put it this way: "Antidepressant-
induced mania is not simply a temporary and fully reversible
phenomenon, but may trigger complex biochemical mechanisms of
illness deterioration." 21
With illegal and legal drugs greasing the road to bipolar illness, it
is little wonder that a rare disorder in 1955 has become common¬
place today. SSRIs took the country by storm in the 1990s, and
from 1996 to 2004, the number of adults diagnosed with bipolar ill¬
ness rose 56 percent. At the same time, psychiatry's steady expan¬
sion of diagnostic boundaries over the past thirty-five years has
helped fuel the bipolar boom too.
When bipolar disorder was first separated from manic-depressive
illness, the diagnosis required a person to have suffered bouts of
mania and depression so severe that each type had resulted in hos¬
pitalization. Then, in 1976, Goodwin and others at the NIMH
suggested that if a person had been hospitalized for depression but
not for mania, and yet had experienced a mild episode of mania
1 8 2 •
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
(hypomania), he or she could be diagnosed with bipolar II, a less
severe form of the disease. Then the bipolar II diagnosis was ex¬
panded so that it included people who had never been hospitalized
for either depression or mania, but simply had experienced episodes
of both. Next, in the 1990s, the psychiatric community decided that
a diagnosis of hypomania no longer required four days of "elevated,
expansive, or irritable mood,” but rather simply two days of such
moodiness. Bipolar illness was on the march, and with the diagnos¬
tic boundaries expanded in this way, researchers were suddenly an¬
nouncing that it affected up to 5 percent of the population. But even
that didn't end the bipolar boom: In 2003, former NIMH director
Lewis Judd and others argued that many people suffer "subthresh¬
old” symptoms of depression and mania, and thus could be diag¬
nosed with "bipolar spectrum disorder."” There was now bipolar I,
bipolar II, and a "bipolarity intermediate between bipolar disorder
and normality," one bipolar expert explained. 23 Judd calculated
that 6.4 percent of American adults suffer from bipolar symptoms;
others have argued that one in every four adults now falls into the
catchall bipolar bin, this once-rare illness apparently striking almost
as frequently as the common cold. 24
The Lithium Years
With the psychopharmacology revolution in full bloom during the
1960s, it seemed that every major psychiatric disorder should have
its own magic bullet, and once bipolar disorder was separated from
manic-depressive illness, psychiatry found a suitable candidate in
lithium. Salts made from this alkali metal had been hanging around
the fringes of medicine for more than 150 years, and then suddenly,
during the early 1970s, lithium was touted as a cure'of sorts for this
newly identified disease. "I have not found another treatment in
psychiatry that works so quickly, so specifically, and so permanently
as lithium for recurrent manic and depressive mood states," said
Columbia University psychiatrist Ronald Fieve, in his 1975 book,
Moodswing . 25
THE BIPOLAR BOOM
183
Nature's lightest metal, lithium was discovered in 1818, found in
rocks off the Swedish coast. It was reported to dissolve uric acid and
thus was marketed as a therapy that could break up kidney stones
and the uric crystals that gathered in the joints of people who suf¬
fered from gout. In the late 1800s and early 1900s, lithium became
a popular ingredient in elixirs and tonics, and it would even be
added to beers and other beverages. However, lithium was eventu¬
ally found to have no uric-acid-dissolving properties, and in 1949,
the FDA banned it after it was found to cause cardiovascular prob¬
lems. 26
Its revival as a psychiatric drug began in Australia, where the
physician John Cade fed it to guinea pigs and observed that it made
them docile. In 1949, he reported that he had successfully treated
ten manic patients with lithium; however, he neglected to mention
in his published article that the treatment killed one person and
made two others severely ill. As makers of lithium tonics had long
known, lithium can be toxic even in fairly small doses. Both intel¬
lectual function and motor movement may become impaired, and if
too high of a dose is given, a person may lapse into a coma and die.
As a group, psychiatrists in the United States showed little inter¬
est in lithium until bipolar made its appearance as a distinct illness.
Prior to that time, Thorazine and other neuroleptics were used to
curb manic episodes and thus there was no need for another drug
that seemed to have similar brain-dampening effects. But once
George Winokur published his book in 1969 dividing manic-
depressive illness into unipolar and bipolar forms, psychiatry had a
new disease in need of its own antidote.
Since no pharmaceutical company could patent lithium, the APA
took the lead in getting the FDA to approve it. Only a few placebo-
controlled trials of the drug were ever conducted. In 1985, UK re¬
searchers who scoured the scientific literature could only find four
of any merit. However, in those studies, lithium produced a good re¬
sponse in 75 percent of the patients, which was much higher than
the response rate in the placebo group. 27 The second part of the ev¬
idence base for lithium came, as usual, from withdrawal studies. In¬
vestigators who analyzed nineteen such trials in 1994 found that
53.5 percent of the patients withdrawn from lithium relapsed,
184
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
versus 37.5 percent of the lithium-maintained patients. That was
taken as evidence that lithium prevented relapse, although the re¬
searchers noted that in the few studies where patients had been
gradually withdrawn from the drug, only 29 percent relapsed
(which was lower than the rate among the drug-maintained pa¬
tients). 28
All in all, this was not particularly robust evidence that lithium
benefited patients, and during the 1980s, several investigators began
raising concerns about its long-term effects. They noted that re¬
admission rates for mania in both the United States and the United
Kingdom had risen since lithium was introduced, and eventually it
became clear why bipolar patients were turning up at hospital emer¬
gency rooms with such great frequency.
Various studies found that more than 50 percent of lithium-
treated patients would quit taking the drug in fairly short order,
usually because they objected to how the drug dulled their minds
and slowed their physical movements, and when they did, they re¬
lapsed at astonishingly high rates. In 1999, Ross Baldessarini
reported that half of all patients relapsed within five months of quit¬
ting lithium, even though in the absence of exposure to the drug, it
took nearly three years for 50 percent of bipolar patients to relapse.
The time between episodes following lithium withdrawal was seven
times shorter than it was naturally. 29 "The risk of recurrence after
discontinuation of lithium therapy . . . especially of mania, is much
higher than predicted by a patient's course before treatment or
by general knowledge of the natural history of the illness,"
Baldessarini wrote. 30 Other investigators noted the same phenome¬
non: "Manic relapse is readily triggered [by lithium withdrawal],
probably by the release of supersensitized receptors or membrane
pathways," explained Jonathan Himmelhoch from the University of
Pittsburgh. 31
This meant that bipolar patients who were treated with lithium
and then stopped taking it ended up "worse than if they had never
had any drug treatment," wrote UK psychiatrist Joanna Mon-
crieff. 32 A Scottish psychiatrist, Guy Goodwin, concluded in 1993
that if patients were exposed to lithium and then quit taking it
within the first two years, the risk of relapse was so great that the
THE BIPOLAR BOOM
185
drug may be "harmful to bipolar patients." The higher hospitaliza¬
tion readmission rates for bipolar patients since the introduction of
lithium "could be explained entirely” by this drug-induced worsen¬
ing, he said. 33
Yet the patients who stayed on lithium weren't faring particularly
well either. Roughly 40 percent relapsed within two years of their
initial hospitalization, and by the end of five years, more than 60
percent fell sick again. 31 There was a core group of good, long-term
lithium responders —perhaps 20 percent of those initially treated
with the drug —but for the majority of patients, it provided little
long-term relief. In 1996, Martin Harrow and Joseph Goldberg,
from the University of Illinois, reported that at the end of 4.5 years,
41 percent of the patients on lithium had "poor outcomes," nearly
one-half had been rehospitalized, and as a group they weren't
"functioning” any better than those not taking the drug. 35 This was
a dismal finding, and then Michael Gitlin at UCLA reported similar
five-year results for his lithium-treated bipolar patients. "Even ag¬
gressive pharmacological maintenance treatment does not prevent
relatively poor outcome in a significant number of bipolar pa¬
tients,” he wrote. 36
Although lithium is still in use today, it lost its place as a first-line
therapy once "mood stabilizers" were brought to the market in the
late 1990s. As Moncrieff wrote in 1997, summing up lithium's
record of efficacy: "There are indications that it is ineffective in the
long-term outlook of bipolar disorders, and it is known to be associ¬
ated with various forms of harm." 37
Bipolar All the Time
There are really two narratives to be dug out of the scientific litera¬
ture regarding the treatment of bipolar illness with psychiatric
drugs. The first tells of lithium's rise and fall as a magic bullet for
the disorder. The second tells of how bipolar outcomes have dra¬
matically worsened during the psychopharmacology era, with
experts in the field documenting this at every turn.
l86
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
As early as 1965, before lithium had made its triumphant entry
into American psychiatry, German psychiatrists were puzzling over
the change they were seeing in their manic-depressive patients.
Patients treated with antidepressants were relapsing frequently, the
drugs "transforming the illness from an episodic course with free in¬
tervals to a chronic course with continuous illness," they wrote. The
German physicians also noted that in some patients, "the drugs
produced a destabilization in which, for the first time, hypomania
was followed by continual cycling between hypomania and depres¬
sion." 38
This was obviously alarming, for the good outcomes in manic-
depressive patients arose from the fact that they spent a large part of
their lives in symptom-free intervals between episodes, during
which time they functioned well. Antidepressants were destroying
those asymptomatic interludes, or at least dramatically shortening
them. Prior to the drug era, Kraepelin and others reported that only
about one-third of manic patients suffered three or more episodes in
their lives. Yet studies of bipolar patients in the 1960s and 1970s
told of two-thirds who were becoming chronically ill. "The admin¬
istration of tricyclics may account for artificially high relapse rate
estimates," Fred Goodwin wrote in 1979. "Induction of mania,
breakdown of otherwise long episodes into multiple ones . . . induc¬
tion of rapid cycling . . . are some of the mechanisms by which the
administration of tricyclics may contribute to an increase in the
number of episodes." 39
Once again, it was becoming apparent that psychiatric medica¬
tions were worsening the course of a mental illness. In 1983,
Athanasious Koukopoulos, director of a mood disorders clinic in
Rome, said that he and his colleagues were observing the same thing
in their Italian patients. "The general impression of clinicians today
is that the course of recurrences of manic-depressive illness has
substantially changed in the last 20 years," he wrote. "The recur¬
rences of many patients have become more frequent. One sees more
manias and hypomanias . . . more rapid cyclers, and more chronic
depressions." Whereas in the pre-drug era rapid cyclers were un¬
known, 16 percent of Koukopoulos's manic-depressive patients were
now in this predicament, and they were suffering an astonishing 6.5
THE BIPOLAR BOOM
187
mood episodes annually, up from less than one episode a year prior
to being treated with an antidepressant. "It certainly seems para¬
doxical,” he admitted, "that a treatment that is therapeutic for
depression can worsen the further course of the disease." 40
In spite of such information, antidepressants continued to be pre¬
scribed to bipolar patients, and even today, 60 to 80 percent are
exposed to an SSRI or some other antidepressant. As a result, inves¬
tigators have continued to document the harm done. In 2000,
Nassir Ghaemi reported that in a study of thirty-eight bipolar pa¬
tients treated with an antidepressant, 55 percent developed mania
(or hypomania) and 23 percent turned into rapid cyclers. This
antidepressant-treated group also spent "significantly more time de¬
pressed" than a second group of bipolar patients who weren't ex¬
posed to this class of medication. 41 "There are significant risks of
mania and long-term worsening with antidepressants," Ghaemi
wrote a few years later, repeating a message that had been uttered
many times before. 42 At the University of Louisville, Rif El-Mallakh
similarly concluded that antidepressants may "destabilize the ill¬
ness, leading to an increase in the number of both manic and de¬
pressive episodes." The drugs, he added, "increase the likelihood of
a mixed state," in which feelings of depression and mania occur si¬
multaneously. 43
In 2003, Koukopoulos chimed in again, reporting that anti-
depressant-induced rapid cycling fully abates in only one-third of
patients over the long term (even after the offending antidepressant
is withdrawn), and that 40 percent of patients continue to "cycle rap¬
idly with unmodified severity" for years on end. 14 Soon, in 2005, El-
Mallakh pointed out yet another problem: Antidepressants could
induce a "chronic, dysphoric, irritable state” in bipolar patients,
meaning that they were almost continually depressed and miser¬
able. 45 Finally, in 2008, in a large NIMH study called the
Systematic Treatment Enhancement Program for Bipolar Disorder
(STEP-BD), "the major predictor of worse outcome was antidepres¬
sant use, which about 60 percent of patients received,” Ghaemi
noted. 46 The antidepressant users were nearly four times more likely
than the non-exposed patients to develop rapid cycling, and twice
as likely to have multiple manic or depressive episodes. 42 "This
188 •
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
study," wrote Ghaemi, in an editorial that appeared in the Amer¬
ican journal of Psychiatry, "may be one more nail in the coffin of
antidepressant use in bipolar disorder."
During the past ten years, several large studies have documented
just how constantly symptomatic bipolar patients are today. In a
long-term follow-up of 146 bipolar I patients who enrolled in an
NIMH study in 1978-81, Lewis Judd found that they were de¬
pressed 32 percent of the time, manic or hypomanic 9 percent of the
time, and suffering from mixed symptoms 6 percent of the time. 48
The bipolar II patients in that study arguably fared even worse:
They were depressed 50 percent of the time. "The nature of this de¬
ceptively 'milder' form of manic-depressive illness is so chronic as to
seem to fill the entire life," Judd wrote. 45 Russell Joffe, at the New
Jersey Medical School, reported in 2004 that 33 percent of the bipo¬
lar I patients and 22 percent of the bipolar II patients he studied
were rapid cyclers, and both groups were symptomatic nearly half
of the time. 50 Meanwhile, Robert Post announced that nearly two-
thirds of the 258 bipolar patients he studied had four or more
episodes per year. 51
All of these studies showed the same bottom-line result: "It is
now well established that bipolar disorders are chronic, with a
course characterized by frequent affective episode recurrence," Judd
said. 52
The Harm Done
In a 2 000 paper published in the Psychiatric Quarterly, a Harvard
Medical School psychiatrist, Carlos Zarate, and a psychiatrist who
worked for Eli Lilly, Mauricio Tohen, opened up a new line of con¬
cern: Bipolar patients today aren't just much more symptomatic
than in the past, they also don't function as well. "In the era prior to
pharmacotherapy, poor outcome in mania was considered a rela¬
tively rare occurrence,” Zarate and Tohen wrote. "However, mod¬
ern outcome studies have found that a majority of bipolar patients
THE BIPOLAR BOOM
• 189
evidence high rates of functional impairment." What, they won¬
dered, could explain "these differences"? 53
The remarkable decline in the functional outcomes of bipolar pa¬
tients is easy to document. In the pre-lithium era, 85 percent of
mania patients would return to work or to their "pre-morbid" so¬
cial role (as a housewife, for example). As Winokur wrote in 1969,
most patients had "no difficulty resuming their usual occupations."
But then bipolar patients began cycling through emergency rooms
more frequently, employment rates began to decline, and soon in¬
vestigators were reporting that fewer than half of all bipolar pa¬
tients were employed or otherwise "functionally recovered.” In
1995, Michael Gitlin at UCLA reported that only 28 percent of his
bipolar patients had a "good occupational outcome” at the end of
five years. 54 Three years later, psychiatrists at the University of
Cincinnati announced that only 24 percent of their bipolar patients
were "functionally recovered” at the end of one year. 55 David
Kupfer at the University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, in a
study of 2,839 bipolar patients, discovered that even though 60 per¬
cent had attended college and 30 percent had graduated, two-thirds
were unemployed. 56 "In summary," wrote Ross Baldessarini in a
2007 review article, "functional status is far more impaired in type
I bipolar patients than previously believed, [and] remarkably, there
is some evidence that functional outcome in type II bipolar patients
may be even worse than in type I." 57
The antidepressants, by increasing the frequency of episodes
that bipolar patients suffer, naturally reduce their ability to return
to work. But, as has become evident in recent years, the problem
runs much deeper than that. One of the hallmarks of manic-
depressive illness, dating back to Kraepelin, was that once people
recovered from their episodes of mania and depression, they were
as smart as they had been before they became ill. As Zarate and
Tohen noted in their 2000 paper, "studies conducted prior to 1975
found no consistent findings in cognitive deficits in bipolar
patients.” But lithium was known to slow thinking, and suddenly
researchers began reassessing this belief. In 1993, NIMH investiga¬
tors compared cognitive function in bipolar and schizophrenia
igo • ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
patients, and they concluded that while the bipolar patients
showed signs of impairment, the deficits were "more severe and
extensive in schizophrenia." 5 '
This was something of a glass-half-full finding. You could inter¬
pret it to mean that cognitive impairment was not that bad in
bipolar patients, or, if you remembered the pre-lithium days, you
might wonder why these patients were suddenly showing signs of
mental decline. But this was just the beginning salvo of a tragic
story. Once lithium monotherapy fell from favor, psychiatrists
began to turn to "drug cocktails" to treat their patients, and soon
investigators had this to report: "Cognitive impairments [that] exist
in schizophrenia and affective disorders . . . cannot be qualitatively
distinguished with sufficient reliability." 59 The degree of impairment
in these two illnesses was suddenly converging, and in 2 001, Faith
Dickerson at the Sheppard Pratt Health System in Baltimore pro¬
vided a more detailed picture of that convergence. She ran seventy-
four medicated schizophrenia patients and twenty-six medicated
bipolar patients through a series of tests that assessed forty-one cog¬
nitive and social-functioning variables, and found that the bipolar
patients were as impaired as the schizophrenia patients on thirty-six
of the forty-one measures. There was "a similar pattern of cognitive
functioning in patients with bipolar disorder as compared to those
with schizophrenia," she wrote. "On most measures of social func¬
tioning, our patients with bipolar disorder were not significantly
different from those in the schizophrenia group." 60
After that, reports of significant cognitive decline in bipolar
patients seemed to pour in from psychiatric researchers around
the globe —English, Swedish, German, Australian, and Spanish in¬
vestigators all told of it. The Australians reported in 2007 that even
when bipolar patients are only mildly symptomatic, they are
"neuropsychological scarred” —impaired in their decision-making
skills, their verbal fluency, and their ability to remember things. 61
Meanwhile, Spanish investigators, after noting that cognitive func¬
tion in their bipolar and schizophrenia patients "did not differ over
time in any test,” concluded that both groups suffered from dys¬
function in the "prefrontal cortex and temporolimbic structures."
They also observed that "the more medications the patients re-
THE BIPOLAR BOOM • lgi
ceived, the greater the psychosocial functioning impairment." 62 * Fi¬
nally, English researchers who looked at the daily lives of bipolar
patients found that more than two-thirds "rarely or never engaged
in social activities with friends,” their social lives nearly as impover¬
ished as those diagnosed with schizophrenia. 63
This was an astonishing convergence in long-term outcomes be¬
tween the two diagnostic groups, and while the psychiatrists in the
United States and abroad who documented it mostly tried, in their
discussion of the phenomenon, to ignore the medication elephant in
the room, several did confess that it was possible that psychiatric
drugs were to blame. Conventional antipsychotics, said Zarate in
one of his papers, "may have a negative impact on the overall
course of the illness." 64 Later, he and Tohen wrote that "medication
induced changes may be yet another factor in explaining the dis¬
crepancies in recovery rates between earlier and more recent stud¬
ies." The antidepressants, they noted, might cause a "worsening of
the course of illness,” while the antipsychotics might lead to more
"depressive episodes” and "lower functional recovery rates." Cog¬
nitive impairment was a primary reason that medicated schizophre¬
nia patients fared so poorly over the long term, they said, and "it
has been suggested that drug side effects may in part explain the
cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder patients." 65 Baldessarini, in his
2007 review, also acknowledged that "neuropharmacological-neuro-
toxic factors” might be causing "cognitive deficits in bipolar dis¬
order patients.” Finally, Kupfer threw one more concern into the
mix. He detailed all the physical illnesses that now struck bipolar
patients —cardiovascular problems, diabetes, obesity, thyroid dys¬
function, etc. —and wondered whether "treatment factors such as
toxicity from medications” could be causing these devastating ail¬
ments, or at least contributing to them. 66
All of these writers put their concerns into a conditional context,
* In this study, the investigators reported that cognitive impairment, from least
to most, was as follows, according to drug treatment received: lithium
monotherapy, untreated, neuroleptic monotherapy, and then combination drug
therapy. However, no details are given about the "untreated” group and
whether they had previous exposure to psychiatric medications.
192.
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
stating that the drugs might be causing this mental and physical
deterioration in their patients. But it's easy to see that their hesi¬
tancy was scientifically unwarranted. Schizophrenia and manic-
depressive illness had been diagnostically born as distinct in kind
precisely because those with schizophrenia deteriorated cognitively
over time, into dementia, while the manic-depressive group did
not/' The convergence in outcomes developed once both groups
were treated with similar drug cocktails (which usually included an
antipsychotic). "The field is witnessing a convergence of pharmaco¬
logical approaches to the treatment of schizophrenia and bipolar
disorder," wrote Stephen Stahl, author of Antipsychotics and Mood
Stabilizers, in 2005. It was adopting "similar blended treatments for
these two disease states."” Psychiatric drugs, of course, perturb var¬
ious neurotransmitter pathways in the brain, and thus once schizo¬
phrenia and bipolar patients are on similar drug cocktails, they
suffer from similar abnormalities in brain function. The conver¬
gence of outcomes in the two groups reflects an iatrogenic process
at work: The two groups, apart from whatever "natural" problems
they may have, both end up suffering from what could be dubbed
"polypharmacy psychiatric drug illness.”
Today, bipolar illness is a far cry from what it once was. Prior to
the psychopharmacology era, it had been a rare disorder, affecting
perhaps one in ten thousand people. Now it affects one in forty (or
by some counts, one in twenty). And even though most patients
today —at initial diagnosis —are not nearly as ill as the hospitalized
patients of the past, their long-term outcomes are almost incompre¬
hensibly worse. In his 2007 review, Baldessarini even detailed, step
by step, this remarkable deterioration in outcomes. In the pre-drug
* The schizophrenia patients who routinely deteriorated into dementia were
Kraepelin's dementia praecox patients. That group of patients presented with
symptoms very different in kind from schizophrenia patients today, and as we
saw in Martin Harrow’s fifteen-year study, many unmedicated schizophrenia
patients recover. Courtenay Harding reported the same thing in her long-term
study —many of the unmedicated patients had completely recovered. So it's un¬
clear what percentage of people diagnosed with schizophrenia today, if not
continually medicated, would deteriorate cognitively over time.
THE BIPOLAR BOOM
193
The Transformation of Bipolar Disorder in the Modern Era
Pre-Lithium Bipolar
Medicated Bipolar Today
Prevalence
1 in 5,000 to 20,000
1 in 20 to 50
Good long-term
functional outcomes
7 5 % to 9 0%
3 3%
Symptom course
Time-limited acute episodes of
mania and major depression with
recovery to euthymia and a fav¬
orable functional adaptation
between episodes
Slow or incomplete recovery
from acute episodes, con¬
tinued risk of recurrences,
and sustained morbidity over
time
Cognitive function
No impairment between epi¬
sodes or long-term impairment
Impairment even between
episodes; long-term impair¬
ment in many cognitive
domains; impairment is
similar to what is observed in
medicated schizophrenia
This information is drawn from multiple sources. See in particular Huxley, N. "Disability and its
treatment in bipolar disorder patients." Bipolar Disorders 9 (2007): 18S-96.
era, there was "recovery to euthymia [no symptoms] and a favor¬
able functional adaptation between episodes." Now there is "slow
or incomplete recovery from acute episodes, continued risk of
recurrences, and sustained morbidity over time.” Before, 85 percent
of bipolar patients would regain complete "premorbid” functioning
and return to work. Now only a third achieve "full social and occu¬
pational functional recovery to their own premorbid levels." Before,
patients didn't show cognitive impairment over the long term. Now
they end up nearly as impaired as those with schizophrenia. This all
tells of an astonishing medical disaster, and then Baldessarini
penned what might be considered a fitting epitaph for the entire
psychopharmacology revolution:
Prognosis for bipolar disorder was once considered relatively
favorable, but contemporary findings suggest that disability
and poor outcomes are prevalent, despite major therapeutic
advances. 68
194
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
The Graphic That Tells It All
We are now coming to the close of our examination of the outcomes
literature for the major psychiatric disorders (for adults), and a re¬
turn to Martin Harrow’s fifteen-year study on schizophrenia out¬
comes brings it to a climactic end. In addition to following
schizophrenia patients, Harrow studied a group of eighty-one pa¬
tients with "other psychotic disorders” that would have been
described by Kraepelin as a manic-depressive cohort. There were
thirty-seven bipolar and twenty-eight unipolar patients in this group,
and the remaining sixteen had various milder psychotic disorders.
Nearly half of this group stopped taking psychiatric medications
during the study, and thus Harrow really had four groups he
followed: schizophrenia patients on and off meds and manic-
depressive patients on and off meds. Before we review the results,
we can run a quick check of our own thoughts: How should we ex¬
pect the long-term outcomes of all four groups to stack up?
Go ahead —take out a pencil and jot down what you believe the
results will be.
Here are his findings. Over the long term, the manic-depressive
patients who stopped taking psychiatric drugs fared pretty well. But
their recovery took time. At the end of two years, they were still
struggling with their illness. Then they began to improve, and by the
end of the study their collective scores fell into the "recovered” cat¬
egory (a score of one or two on Harrow's global assessment scale).
The recovered patients were working at least part-time, they had
"acceptable" social functioning, and they were largely asympto¬
matic. Their outcomes fit with Kraepelin's understanding of manic-
depressive illness.
The manic-depressive patients who stayed on their psychiatric
medications did not fare so well. At the end of two years, they re¬
mained quite ill, so much so they were now a little bit worse than
the schizophrenia patients off meds. Then, over the next two-and-
one-half years, while the manic-depressive and schizophrenia pa¬
tients who were off meds improved, the manic-depressive patients
who kept taking their pills did not, such that by the end of 4.5
THE BIPOLAR BOOM
195
years, they were doing markedly worse than the schizophrenia off-
med group. That disparity remained through the rest of the study,
and thus here is how the long-term outcomes stacked up, from best
to worst: manic-depressive off meds, schizophrenia off meds,
manic-depressive on meds, and then schizophrenia on meds. 69
Schizophrenia, of course, has long been the psychiatric diagnosis
with the worst long-term prognosis. It is the most severe mental ill¬
ness that nature has to offer. But in this NIMH-funded study, two
groups of medicated patients fared worse than the unmedicated
schizophrenia patients. The results tell of a medical treatment gone
horribly awry, and yet they do not come as a surprise. Anyone who
knew the history of the outcomes literature in psychiatry, a history
that began to unfold more than fifty years ago, could have predicted
that the outcomes would stack up in this way.
In terms of contributing to our modern-day epidemic of disabling
mental illness, the bipolar numbers are staggering. In 1955, there
were about 12,750 people hospitalized with bipolar illness. Today,
according to the NIMH, there are nearly six million adults in the
15-Year Outcomes for Schizophrenia and Manic-Depressive Patients
Worst
outcomes
Follow-up Follow-up Follow-up Follow-up Follow-up
In this graphic, the group labeled "manic depressive" consisted of psychotic patients with bipolar
illness, unipolar depression, and milder psychotic disorders. Source: Harrow, M. "Factors involved
in outcome and recovery in schizophrenia patients not on antipsychotic medications." The Journal
of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195 (2 007): 406-14.
19 6
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
United States with this diagnosis, and according to researchers at
the Johns Hopkins School of Public Health, 83 percent are "se¬
verely impaired” in some facet of their lives. 7 " Bipolar illness is now
said to be the sixth leading cause of medical-related disability in the
world, right behind schizophrenia, and in the near future, as more
and more people are diagnosed with this condition and put on drug
cocktails, we can expect that bipolar will climb past schizophrenia
and take its place behind major depression as the mental illness that
fells the most people in the United States. Such is the fruit, bitter in
kind, born from the psychopharmacology revolution.
Bipolar Narratives
I interviewed more than sixty people with psychiatric diagnoses for
this book, and roughly half at some point had been diagnosed as
bipolar. Yet of the thirty or so who got that diagnosis, only four suf¬
fered from what might be called "organic” bipolar illness, and that
is to say they were hospitalized for a manic episode and had no
prior exposure to illicit drugs or antidepressants. Now that we
know what science has to tell us about the modern bipolar boom,
we can revisit the stories of three people we met in Chapter 2, and
see how their stories fit into that story of science. Then we can hear
from two people diagnosed with bipolar who, if they had been
enrolled in Harrow's fifteen-year study, would have fallen into his
"off-meds" group.
Dorea Vierling-Clausen
If we look at Dorea Vierling-Clausen's story now, we can see that
she has good reason to believe that she should never have been di¬
agnosed with bipolar illness. She went to see a therapist in Denver
because she cried too much. She had no history of mania. But then
she was prescribed an antidepressant and starting having trouble
sleeping, and soon she had a bipolar diagnosis and a prescription
for a drug cocktail that included an antipsychotic. A bright teenager
THE BIPOLAR BOOM
197
had been turned into a mental patient, and Dorea would have con¬
tinued to be one for the rest of her life if she had not weaned herself
from the drugs. When I last spoke to her, in the spring of 2009, she
was aglow with the blush of motherhood, as she had recently given
birth to a son, Reuben. She and Angela were busily raising their
children, with Dorea planning shortly to resume her postdoctoral
research at Massachusetts General Hospital, the memory of her
"bipolar" days receding into an ever-more-distant past.
Monica Briggs
During the time that I worked on this book, Monica Briggs was the
one person who, after an initial interview, got off SSDI (or SSI). She
secured a full-time position with the Transformation Center, a peer-
run organization in Boston that focuses on helping people "re¬
cover" from mental illness, and if you parse her medical story, it's
easy to see that her return to work was related to a change in her
medication.
When we first met, I mentioned to Monica the risk of
antidepressant-induced mania, and as she remembered back to her
breakdown at Middlebury College, a light went on: "I got manic
within six weeks of being put on desipramine," she said. "I'm sure
that's what happened to me.” After that initial manic episode, she
was prescribed a drug cocktail that included an antidepressant, and
she spent the next twenty years cycling in and out of hospitals,
struggling constantly with depression, manic episodes, and suicidal
impulses. Psychiatrists put her on eight or nine different antidepres¬
sants, and she also went through a series of electroshock treatments.
None of this worked. Then, in 2006, she "casually" stopped taking
an antidepressant. For the first time, she was on lithium alone, and
bingo —the suicidal feelings went away, as did the depression and
mania. That symptom relief is what enabled her to work full-time,
and now, as she looks back on the horrible twenty years, she is
stunned by what she sees: "I have not yet recovered from the
immensity of the likelihood that my roulette game with antidepres¬
sants exacerbated my illness."
198
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Steve happen
Steve Lappen, who is a leader of the Depressive and Bipolar Support
Alliance in Boston, was diagnosed with manic-depressive illness in
1969, when he was nineteen years old. He was one of the four peo¬
ple I interviewed whose manic-depressive illness was "organic” in
kind, and on the first day we met, he was in something of a hyper
state, talking so fast that I quickly put my pen away and took out a
tape recorder instead. "OK,” I told him, "fire away."
Raised in Newton, Massachusetts, in a family he describes as
dysfunctional, Steve got tagged with the "bad apple" label early in
life, both by his teachers at school and his parents at home. "I was
disruptive in class,” he says. "Every day, during the pledge of alle¬
giance to the flag, I would go sharpen my pencil. I would also get up
without provocation and just spin around until I was overcome
with dizziness. I would announce that I was a tornado.” He strug¬
gled with mood swings even as a kid, and at age sixteen, while hos¬
pitalized for fainting spells, he jumped out of bed one night and
donned a white coat. "I went around to patient rooms and had
conversations as if I were a doctor. I was manic.”
During his first year at Boston College, he was hit by a bout of
severe depression. His was a classic case of true manic-depressive
illness, and Kraepelin would have recognized the course his illness
took over the next five years. "I didn't take medication," he ex¬
plains, and while he suffered several bouts of depression, he did well
in between those episodes, particularly when he was in a slightly hy-
pomanic state. "When I was feeling well, I would read more, and I
would write papers that weren't due for two or three months," he
says. "When you are hypomanic, your output is remarkable.” He
graduated with a double major in philosophy and English, with
nearly a straight-A average.
However, in his first year of graduate school at Stony Brook in
Long Island, he had a full-blown manic episode followed by a
plunge into depression that left him suicidal. It was then that he was
put on lithium and a tricyclic antidepressant for the first time. "I
didn't have mood swings after that, but instead of having a baseline
of functioning normally, I was depressed. I was in a state of depres-
THE BIPOLAR BOOM
199
sion the entire time I was on the medication. I stayed on it for a year
and said, 'No more.' "
Over the next two decades, Steve mostly stayed away from psy¬
chiatric medications. He married, had two sons, and divorced. He
worked, but skipped from job to job. His life was proceeding
down a chaotic path, a chaos that was clearly related to his manic-
depressive illness, and yet his life was not marked by vocational
disability— he always found work. In 1994, seeking relief from the
mood swings that plagued him, he began taking psychiatric
medications regularly. He cycled through an endless number of anti¬
depressants and mood stabilizers, none of which worked for long.
Those drug failures led to fourteen electroshock treatments, which
in turn left his memory so impaired that when he returned to his
job as a financial planner, "I could no longer recognize my best
client." In 1998, he was put on the tricyclic desipramine, which
promptly turned him into a rapid cycler. "I'd wake up and feel
great, completely emancipated from the demon of depression, and
then two days later, I am back into depression," he explains. "Two
days after that, I'm feeling well again. And there is nothing in my
external environment that would account for that change in
mood.”
He has been on SSDI ever since. The good news is that he hasn't
been hospitalized since 2000, and, as he rightly points out, in spite
of his constant battle with bipolar symptoms, he leads a productive
life. Remarried now, he volunteers as a "reader” for people who are
physically disabled, gives talks about bipolar illness to community
groups, and is one of the leaders of DBSA Boston. He also has pub¬
lished essays and poetry in various small publications. But when I
last spoke to him, in the spring of 2009, he was cycling through
multiple mood swings every day, his symptoms apparently continu¬
ing to worsen.
"I would say in the main, I have been worse when taking med¬
ication. The medication I am taking now is neutral at best. I wish I
could clone myself. I could be my own control group in a trial. I'd
like to know if I'd be better, the same, or worse without it."
200
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Brandon Banks
Brandon Banks can identify the precise moment he became "bipo¬
lar," and while it did involve an antidepressant, there was a series of
life events that led up to it. He grew up poor in Elizabethtown, Ken¬
tucky, without a father at home, and he has painful memories of
sexual abuse, physical abuse, and of a horrible car wreck that killed
his aunt, uncle, and another relative. At school, other kids regularly
taunted him about a facial birthmark, which so traumatized him
that he began wearing a hat pulled low on his head to cover it up.
After graduating from high school in 2000, he moved to Louisville,
where he went to college part-time and worked nights at United
Parcel Service. Soon he noticed that he "wasn't feeling right," and
when he went back home, his family doctor diagnosed him with
"moderate depression" and prescribed an antidepressant. "I went
manic in three days," Brandon says. "It was fast."
His doctor explained that since he'd had that reaction to the
drug, he must be bipolar, rather than just depressed. The drug had
"unmasked” the illness, which Brandon took as a positive thing.
"I'm thinking, This isn't so bad, I could have stayed in the system a
long time without getting immediate confirmation that I'm bipolar
like that.” He was put on a cocktail composed of a mood stabilizer,
an antidepressant, and an antipsychotic, and then it hit him. "This
was a serious shove into seriousness."
Over the next four years, his psychiatrists constantly changed his
prescriptions. "It was like musical chairs with the cocktails," he
says. "They would tell me, 'Let’s take this drug out and put this one
in.' " He took Depakote, Neurontin, Risperdal, Zyprexa, Seroquel,
Haldol, Thorazine, lithium, and an endless succession of antidepres¬
sants, and as time went on he became a rapid cycler who suffered
from mixed states. His medical records also document the develop¬
ment of new psychiatric symptoms: worsening anxiety, panic at¬
tacks, obsessive-compulsive behaviors, voices, hallucinations. He
was hospitalized several times, and at one point he climbed up on
top of a parking garage and threatened to jump off. His ability to
concentrate declined so severely that Kentucky took away his dri¬
ver's license. "What my life became was staying at home all day, get-
THE BIPOLAR BOOM
201
ting up in the morning and laying my pills out on the counter, tak¬
ing them, and then going back to sleep because I couldn't stay
awake if I tried. Then I would get up, play some video games, and
hang out with my family."
Twenty-four years old, he felt like a total failure, and one day,
after a fight with his mother, he moved out and stopped taking his
meds. "I deteriorated badly,” he recalls. "I wasn't bathing and I
wasn't eating.” However, as the weeks turned into months, his
bipolar symptoms lessened, and "I began to think that it's more like
I'm just fucked up," he says. This was a thought that gave him
hope, because now there was the possibility of change, and he took
off traveling around the South. "I might as well be homeless," he
told himself, and that journey ultimately turned into a transforma¬
tive experience. By the time he returned home, he had sworn off eat¬
ing meat and drinking alcohol, on his way to becoming a "health
freak" who practices yoga. "I came back from that trip, and man, I
was on top of it. I felt like a million dollars, and everyone in my
family— cousins, relatives, aunts and uncles —said that they hadn't
seen me glow like this since I was a kid.”
Since then, Brandon has stayed off psychiatric medications. But it
hasn't been easy, and the up-and-down nature of his life came into
sharp relief during his 2008-2009 year at Elizabethtown Commu¬
nity and Technical College. He enrolled there in January of 2008
with dreams of becoming a journalist and a writer, and in the fall,
he became managing editor of the school's newspaper. Under his
leadership, the newspaper won twenty-four awards from the Ken¬
tucky Intercollegiate Press Association during the 2008-2009 year,
and Brandon personally garnered ten such honors for the articles
he'd written, including first place in a deadline-writing competition.
Incredibly, during those nine months, Brandon racked up other suc¬
cesses too. One of his short stories won second place in a competi¬
tion and was published in a Louisville weekly; one of his photos was
picked as cover art for a literary journal; a short film he shot was
nominated for a best documentary award in a local film festival. In
May of 2009, his school honored him with its "outstanding sopho¬
more” award. Yet, even during this season of remarkable accom¬
plishment, Brandon suffered several hypomanic and depressive
202
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
episodes that left him feeling deeply suicidal. "I spent several week¬
ends reading depressive authors with a gun in my hand,” he says.
"My accomplishments at these moments just seem to make every¬
thing worse. It never seems like enough.”
That is where matters stood in his life in the summer of 2009. He
was thriving and struggling at the same time, and his struggles were
such that if psychiatric medications had worked for him the first
time, he would gladly have turned to them for relief. "I'm still pretty
isolated from other people," he explains. "I stick out because of the
birthmark. I'm different. I can't blend in. It becomes an issue with
people. But I'm trying to integrate myself more into life. I have more
people in my life now than I have had in a long time. I'm starting to
make more contacts. I had lunch with a friend the other day. Doing
this is hard for me, and that's because it's just not easy for me to
deal with people and deal with my emotions. I am trying to get
better at it.”
Greg
A math and science whiz, Greg, who asked that I not use his last
name, was the sort of child who, when he was in junior high, built
a Van de Graaff generator from scrounged parts (which included a
vacuum cleaner and a salad bowl, to be precise). However, he had a
troubled relationship with his parents, and at the start of his senior
year, he began to slide into a mad state (and without having used il¬
legal drugs). "I was delusional, very paranoid, and full of anxiety,"
he says. "I was convinced that my parents were trying to kill me.”
Hospitalized for six weeks, Greg was told he was schizoaffective
with bipolar tendencies (a "manic-depressive” type diagnosis), and
he was discharged on a cocktail composed of two antipsychotics
and an antidepressant. But the drugs didn't chase away his paranoid
thoughts, and after he was hospitalized a second time, his psychia¬
trists added a mood stabilizer and a benzodiazepine to the cocktail
and told him he needed to give up his scholastic dreams. "They told
me I would be on medication for the rest of my life, and that I
would probably be a ward of the state, and that maybe, by the time
I was twenty-five or thirty, I could think about getting a part-time
THE BIPOLAR BOOM
203
job. And I believed it, and so I began trying to figure out how to live
with the crushing hopelessness that they are telling you is going to
be your life.”
The next five years passed pretty much as his psychiatrists had
predicted. Although Greg entered Worcester Polytechnic Institute
(WPI) in Massachusetts, he was so heavily medicated that, he says,
"I was living in a haze most of the time. Your mind is just a bag of
sand. And so I did really poorly in school. I rarely even left my
room, and I was kind of out of touch with reality.” He petered
along in school for a couple of years, not really making much
progress, and then, from 2004 to 2006, he dropped out and mostly
stayed in his apartment, smoking marijuana constantly, as "it
helped me accept the condition I was forced into." Six feet, five
inches tall, Greg's weight went from 255 pounds to nearly 500
pounds. "Finally, I said to myself, this is ridiculous. I'd rather be
crazy and have a life than not be crazy and not have a life.”
He went for a medical checkup, thinking this would be a first step
toward reducing his medications, only to be informed that he
needed to stop taking Depakote and Geodon right away, as his liver
was shutting down. The abrupt withdrawal induced such physical
pain — "sweats, joint and muscle pain, nausea, dizziness," he says —
that he didn't even pay attention to whether his paranoia was com¬
ing back. But in very short order, he was off all of his psychiatric
drugs, except for occasional use of a stimulant, and he had also
stopped smoking marijuana. "Honestly, it felt like I was waking up
for the first time in five years," he says. "It felt like I had been
turned off all those years and had just been rolling through life and
I was being pushed around in a wheelchair and finally I had woken
up and had gotten back to being myself again. I felt like the drugs
took away everything that was me, and then when I went off the
drugs, my brain woke up and started working again."
In late 2007, Greg went back to school. We met in the spring of
2009, and after he had told me the story of his bout with mental ill¬
ness, he showed me around his research laboratory at WPI, where
he now spent eighty hours a week, designing and constructing a
robot capable of conducting brain surgery inside an MRI. In a few
weeks, he would receive an undergraduate degree in mechanical en-
204
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
gineering, and since he'd entered a master's program while still
doing undergraduate work, later that summer he would receive a
master's degree in mechatronics, which is a fusion of mechanical
and electrical engineering. The day before my visit, his robotics
research had won second prize in a competition that featured 187
entries by graduate students at WPI. Already he had published three
papers in academic journals on his project, and in a few weeks he
was scheduled to fly to Japan to give a talk about it. He was doing
this project under the guidance of a WPI professor, and they ex¬
pected to conduct animal and cadaver trials with the robot in the
fall of 2009. If all went well, clinical trials with humans would
begin in two years.
While in his laboratory, Greg showed me the robot and the
computer drawings of its circuit boards, which seemed impossibly
complex. Naturally, I thought of John Nash, the Princeton mathe¬
matician whose inspiring story of recovering from schizophrenia,
and doing so while off medication, was told in the book A Beautiful
Mind. "I still feel that I have some bad habits to get out of and some
better habits to get into before I get into the professional life, but I
really do feel that I have left that [mentally ill] part of my life
behind,” says Greg, who has lost more than one hundred pounds.
"Honestly, I almost never think of it. I now think of myself as a per¬
son who is susceptible to building anxiety, but when I start feeling
this anxiety, or start feeling negative about things, I stop and say to
myself, 'Are these really reasonable feelings to feel, or is it just inse¬
curity?' I just have to take the time to check myself.” He is, he
concluded, "pretty optimistic about my future now.”
io
An Epidemic Explained
"With psychiatric medications, you solve one
problem for a period of time, but the next thing you
know you end up with two problems. The treatment
turns a period of crisis into a chronic mental illness."
— AMY UPHAM (2009)
There is a famous optical illusion called the young lady and the old
hag, and depending on how you look at it, you see either a beautiful
young woman or an old witch. The drawing illustrates how one's
perception of an object can suddenly flip, and in a sense, the dueling
histories that we have fleshed out in this book have that same curi¬
ous quality. There is the "young woman" picture of the psycho¬
pharmacology era that most of American society believes in, which
tells of a revolutionary advance in the treatment of mental disor¬
ders, and then there is the "old hag” picture that we have sketched
out in this book, which tells of a form of care that has led to an
epidemic of disabling mental illness.
The young-lady picture of the psychopharmacology era arises
from a powerful combination of history, language, science, and clin¬
ical experience. Prior to 1955, history tells us, the state mental hos¬
pitals were bulging with raving lunatics. But then researchers
discovered an antipsychotic medication, Thorazine, and that drug
made it possible for the states to close their decrepit hospitals and
to treat schizophrenics in the community. Next, psychiatric re¬
searchers discovered anti-anxiety agents, antidepressants, and a
magic bullet —lithium —for bipolar disorder. Science then proved
that the drugs worked: In clinical trials, the drugs were found to
206
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Young or old woman? If you shift your eyes slightly, your perception of the
image will change from one to the other. Courtesy of Exploratorium.
ameliorate a target symptom over the short term better than
placebo. Finally, psychiatrists regularly saw that their drugs were
effective. They gave them to their distressed patients, and their
symptoms often abated. If their patients stopped taking the drugs,
their symptoms frequently returned. This clinical course —initial
symptom reduction and relapse upon drug withdrawal —also gave
patients reason to say: "I need my medication. I can't do well with¬
out it.”
The old-hag picture of the psychopharmacology era arises from a
more careful reading of history and a more thorough review of the
science. When we reviewed the history of deinstitutionalization, we
found that the discharge of chronic schizophrenia patients resulted
from the enactment of Medicare and Medicaid legislation in the
mid-1960s, as opposed to from Thorazine's arrival in asylum medi¬
cine. As for the drugs, we discovered that there was no scientific
breakthrough that led to the introduction of Thorazine and other
first-generation psychiatric medications. Instead, scientists studying
compounds for use as anesthetics and as magic bullets for infectious
AN EPIDEMIC EXPLAINED
207
diseases stumbled upon several agents that had novel side effects.
Then, over the course of the next thirty years, researchers deter¬
mined that the drugs work by perturbing the normal functioning of
neuronal pathways in the brain. In response, the brain undergoes
"compensatory adaptations” to cope with the drug's mucking up of
its messaging system, and this leaves the brain functioning in an
"abnormal" manner. Rather than fix chemical imbalances in the
brain, the drugs create them. We then combed through the out¬
comes literature, and we found that these pills worsen long-term
outcomes, at least in the aggregate. Researchers even put together
biological explanations for why the drugs had this paradoxical
long-term effect.
Those are the dueling visions of the psychopharmacology era. If
you think of the drugs as "anti-disease” agents and focus on short¬
term outcomes, the young lady springs into sight. If you think of the
drugs as "chemical imbalancers" and focus on long-term outcomes,
the old hag appears. You can see either image, depending on where
you direct your gaze.
A Quick Thought Experiment
Just for a moment, before we examine whether we have solved the
puzzle that we set forth in the opening of this book, here is a quick
way to see the old-hag picture a bit more clearly. Imagine that a
virus suddenly appears in our society that makes people sleep
twelve, fourteen hours a day. Those infected with it move about
somewhat slowly and seem emotionally disengaged. Many gain
huge amounts of weight —twenty, forty, sixty, and even one hun¬
dred pounds. Often, their blood sugar levels soar, and so do their
cholesterol levels. A number of those struck by the mysterious ill¬
ness-including young children and teenagers —become diabetic in
fairly short order. Reports of patients occasionally dying from pan¬
creatitis appear in the medical literature. Newspapers and maga¬
zines fill their pages with accounts of this new scourge, which is
dubbed metabolic dysfunction illness, and parents are in a panic
208
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
over the thought that their children might contract this horrible
disease. The federal government gives hundreds of millions of
dollars to scientists at the best universities to decipher the inner
workings of this virus, and they report that the reason it causes such
global dysfunction is that it blocks a multitude of neurotransmitter
receptors in the brain —dopaminergic, serotoninergic, muscarinic,
adrenergic, and histaminergic. All of those neuronal pathways in
the brain are compromised. Meanwhile, MRI studies find that over
a period of several years, the virus shrinks the cerebral cortex, and
this shrinkage is tied to cognitive decline. A terrified public clamors
for a cure.
Now such an illness has in fact hit millions of American children
and adults. We have just described the effects of Eli Lilly's best¬
selling antipsychotic, Zyprexa.
A Mystery Solved
We began this book by raising a question: Why have we seen such a
sharp increase in the number of disabled mentally ill in the United
States since the "discovery” of psychotropic medications? At the
very least, I think we have identified one major cause. In large part,
this epidemic is iatrogenic in kind.
Now there may be a number of social factors contributing to the
epidemic. Our society may be organized in a way today that leads to
a great degree of stress and emotional turmoil. For instance, we may
lack the close-knit neighborhoods that help people stay well. Rela¬
tionships are the foundation of human happiness, or so it seems,
and as Robert Putnam wrote in 2000, we spend too much time
"bowling alone.” We also may watch too much television and get
too little exercise, a combination that is known to be a prescription
for becoming depressed. The food we eat —more processed foods
and so on —might be playing a role too. And the common use of
illicit drugs —marijuana, cocaine, and hallucinogens —has clearly
contributed to the epidemic. Finally, once a person goes on SSI or
SSDI, there is a tremendous financial disincentive to return to work.
AN EPIDEMIC EXPLAINED
209
People on disability call it the "entitlement trap.” Unless they can
get a job that pays health insurance, they will lose that safety net if
they go back to work, and once they start working, they may lose
their rent subsidy, too.
However, in this book, we have been focusing on the role that
psychiatry and its medications may be playing in this epidemic, and
the evidence is quite clear. First, by greatly expanding diagnostic
boundaries, psychiatry is inviting an ever-greater number of chil¬
dren and adults into the mental illness camp. Second, those so diag¬
nosed are then treated with psychiatric medications that increase
the likelihood they will become chronically ill. Many treated with
psychotropics end up with new and more severe psychiatric symp¬
toms, physically unwell, and cognitively impaired. That is the tragic
story writ large in five decades of scientific literature.
The record of disability produced by psychiatric medications can
be easily summarized. With schizophrenia, in the decade prior to
the introduction of Thorazine, roughly 70 percent of people suffer¬
ing a first episode of psychosis were discharged from the hospital
within eighteen months, and the majority didn't return to the hospi¬
tal during fairly lengthy follow-up periods. Researchers in the post-
Thorazine era reported similar results for unmedicated patients.
Rappaport, Carpenter, and Mosher all found that perhaps half of
those diagnosed with schizophrenia would do fairly well if they
were not continuously medicated. But that is now the standard of
care, and as Harrow's study showed, only 5 percent of medicated
patients recover over the long term. Today, there are an estimated 2
million adults disabled by schizophrenia in the United States, and
this disability number could perhaps be halved if we adopted a
paradigm of care that employed antipsychotic medications in a
selective, cautious manner.
With the affective disorders, the iatrogenic effects of our drug-
based paradigm of care are even more apparent. Anxiety used to be
viewed as a mild disorder, one that rarely required hospitalization.
Today, 8 percent of the younger adults on the SSI and SSDI roles
due to a psychiatric disability have anxiety as a primary diagnosis.
Similarly, outcomes for major depression used to be good. In 1955,
there were only thirty-eight thousand people hospitalized with de-
210
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
pression, and the illness could be expected to remit. Today, major
depression is the leading cause of disability in the United States for
people fifteen to forty-four years old. It is said to strike 15 million
adults, and according to researchers at Johns Hopkins School of
Public Health, 60 percent are "severely impaired.” As for bipolar
disorder, an extremely rare illness has become a common one.
According to the NIMH, nearly 6 million adults suffer from it
today. Whereas 85 percent of those struck by it used to recover and
go back to work, now only about a third of bipolar patients func¬
tion this well, and over the long term those bipolar patients who
reliably take their medications end up nearly as impaired as those
with schizophrenia who stay on neuroleptics. The Johns Hopkins
investigators concluded that 83 percent are "severely impaired.”
In sum, there were fifty-six thousand people hospitalized with
anxiety and manic-depressive illness in 1955. Today, according to
the NIMH, at least 40 million adults suffer from one of these affec¬
tive disorders. More than 1.5 million people are on SSI or SSDI
because they are disabled by anxiety, depression, or bipolar illness,
and, according to the Johns Hopkins data, more than 14 million
people who have these diagnoses are "severely impaired” in their
ability to function in society. That is the astonishing bottom-line
result produced by a medical specialty that has dramatically ex¬
panded diagnostic boundaries in the past fifty years and treated its
patients with drugs that perturb normal brain function.
Moreover, the epidemic continues its march. In the eighteen
months it took me to research and write this book, the Social Secu¬
rity Administration released its 2007 reports for its SSI and SSDI
programs, and the numbers were as expected. There were 401,255
children and adults under sixty-five years old added to the SSI and
SSDI rolls in 2007 because of a psychiatric disability. Imagine a
large auditorium filling up every day with 250 children and 850
adults newly disabled by mental illness, and you get a visual sense of
the horrible toll exacted by this epidemic.
AN EPIDEMIC EXPLAINED
211
Physical Illness, Cognitive Impairment,
and Early Death
Fleshing out the nature of a disease usually involves identifying all
the symptoms that may develop, and then following their course
over time. In the previous chapters, we mostly focused on studies
that showed that psychiatric medications worsen target symptoms
over the long run, and only briefly noted that the drugs may cause
physical problems, emotional numbing, and cognitive impairment.
This is also a form of care that leads to early death. The seriously
mentally ill are now dying fifteen to twenty-five years earlier than
normal, with this problem of early death having become much more
pronounced in the past fifteen years. 2 They are dying from cardio¬
vascular ailments, respiratory problems, metabolic illnesses, dia¬
betes, kidney failure, and so forth —the physical ailments tend to
pile up as people stay on antipsychotics (or drug cocktails) for years
on end. 2
Here are three stories that bear witness to these various long¬
term risks.
Amy Up ham
Amy Upham lives in a small one-bedroom apartment in Buffalo,
and as I enter the living room, she points to a table cluttered with
papers. "This is me on psychiatric drugs," she says and hands me a
stack of medical documents. They tell of a drug-induced swelling of
the brain, faltering kidneys, a swollen liver, a swollen gallbladder,
thyroid problems, gastritis, and cognitive abnormalities. A little
over five feet tall, with frizzy reddish brown hair, Amy, who is thirty
years old, weighs ninety pounds. She squeezes a fold of loose skin
near her elbow, the muscle underneath having wasted away. "This
is like what you see with heroin users."
Amy first took a psychiatric medication at age sixteen when she
contracted Lyme disease and suffered a bout of depression. Twelve
years later, she was still on antidepressants, and as she reviews
that history, she identifies several instances when the drugs stirred
212
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
hypomanic episodes and worsened her obsessive-compulsive behav¬
iors. Finally, in 2007, she decided to gradually wean herself from
the two-drug combo she was taking, and at first, it went well. How¬
ever, at the time, she was working for the county mental health
department as an advocate for the mentally ill, and eventually
someone anonymously informed her bosses that she was going off
her medication. This went against what the agency preached, and it
all ended with Amy out of a job and paranoid that someone was
stalking her. "I had a nervous breakdown," she says. "I went into
the hospital to hide.”
This was the first time Amy had ever been hospitalized, and she
was immediately put on a cocktail that included lithium. Within a
few months, her endocrine system began to fail. Her menstrual cycle
ceased, her thyroid went haywire, and an EEG revealed that her
brain was swollen. Then her kidneys started shutting down. She had
to abruptly stop taking the lithium, and that triggered a manic
episode. Doctors put her on Ativan to counter the mania, but that
drug stirred feelings of horrible rage and left her feeling suicidal.
Months passed, and in December 2008, she checked herself into a
psych hospital, where she was diagnosed with Ativan toxicity. "I've
never seen a drug fuck up a person like Ativan fucks you up,” a
nurse told her. The hospital switched her from Ativan to Klonopin
and prescribed Ability, which triggered a seizure. Next a doctor dis¬
covered something wrong with her heart, which appeared to be re¬
lated to the Klonopin, and so Amy was put back on Ativan. "Now I
start hallucinating for the first time in my life," she says. "I was pac¬
ing uncontrollably and crawling out of my skin." Other drug-related
complications ensued, and on February 24, 2009, Amy moved into a
shelter on the hospital grounds, her thoughts now so scattered that a
nurse wondered "if early Alzheimer's runs in the family."
Remarkably, much of that story is documented in the sheaf of pa¬
pers that Amy has given me. She spent the last four months trying to
get off the Ativan, but every time she dropped to a lower dose, she
suffered fits of rage and something akin to delirium. "I am feeling
scared," she says, as I hand the papers back to her. "The with¬
drawals are really bad and I live alone. I'm in a constant state of
panic, anxiety, and I have some agoraphobia. It's not safe.”
AN EPIDEMIC EXPLAINED
213
Rachel Klein
When I first met Rachel Klein in the spring of 2008, she hobbled
into my office with a cane and a service dog by her side, which
flopped by her feet while we spoke. She was not yet forty years old,
but very quickly she rewound the clock for me, and soon she was
telling of a bright fall day in 1984. Only sixteen years old, she was
entering the Massachusetts Institute of Technology, a child prodigy
with an IQ of 173 and her ears ringing with predictions that one
day she would win a Nobel Prize. "I arrived on campus with a
teddy bear sticking out of my backpack," she says, smiling slightly
at the memory. "That's how ill-equipped emotionally I was."
Rachel's emotional crash at MIT got under way at the end of her
sophomore year, when she became involved with an older student
who was "totally psychotic” and she began using illicit drugs —
Ecstasy, acid, mushrooms, and nitrous oxide. Her sense of self
began to crumble, and after a summer of talk therapy left her more
confused than ever, she was hospitalized for psychotic depression.
When she was released, she had prescriptions for an antipsychotic,
an antidepressant, and a benzodiazepine (Xanax). "None of those
drugs helped me," she says. "They numbed me out, and trying to
get off Xanax was a disaster. That is the evilest drug ever. It is so ad¬
dictive, and all of the symptoms that caused you to go into the hos¬
pital in the first place get one thousand times worse when you try to
go off it."
Although Rachel eventually graduated from MIT and was ac¬
cepted into an M.D.-Ph.D. program at the University of Colorado,
she began cycling in and out of hospitals; her crash at MIT trans¬
formed into a case of chronic mental illness. "They told me I was
hopeless, and that I would never get better," she recalls. She enjoyed
a period of stability from 1995 to 2001, when she worked as an as¬
sistant house manager at a group home in Boston, but then her
brother died suddenly and her psychological problems flared up
anew. Her psychiatrist took her off Risperdal and switched her to
high doses of Geodon and Effexor, and he gave her an injection of
another psychiatric medication as well.
"I had a severe serotonergic reaction, a toxic reaction," Rachel
2 14
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
says, shaking her head at the memory. "It caused vasoconstriction
in my brain, and this caused brain damage. I ended up in a wheel¬
chair, and I couldn't think, speak, or walk. Those centers of the
brain need a lot of juice."
Since then, her life has had its ups and downs. She takes comfort
in her volunteer work with M-Power, the Boston peer advocacy
group, and in the spring of 2008, she was working sixteen hours a
week for Advocates, Inc., which provides services to the deaf. But
she also has battled ovarian cancer, and it's possible that illness was
related to the psychiatric medications. She does find such drugs use¬
ful today, but when she looks back at her life, she sees a paradigm of
care that utterly failed her. "It's really a travesty," she says.
Scott Sexton
In the spring of 2005, Scott Sexton received his MBA from Rice
University. A bright future lay ahead at that moment, but then he
broke up with the woman he had intended to marry, and he was
hospitalized for depression. This was his second bout of major de¬
pression (he'd suffered a first episode five years earlier, when his par¬
ents divorced), and since Scott's father had suffered from bipolar
illness, he was now diagnosed with that disorder. He was put on a
cocktail that included Zyprexa.
That fall, he began working as a consultant for Deloitte, the big
accounting firm. Although his first few months on the job went fine,
by early 2006 he was sleeping twelve to sixteen hours a day, zonked
out by the Zyprexa. He soon needed another pill to get up in the
morning, and he began "putting on weight like gangbusters,” his
mother, Kaye, recalls. "He was five feet, ten inches tall and he went
from 185 pounds to 250 pounds. He had a beer belly, and his
cheeks looked like he was a chipmunk. We knew that Zyprexa
caused weight gain, and he was alarmed, and so was I."
By the fall of 2006, Scott was sleeping so much that on weekends
he wouldn't get up until the afternoon. He stopped going into
the office and told Deloitte he was working from home. On Thanks¬
giving, he called his mother to tell her that he was suffering severe
stomach pains, and the next day he was admitted to St. Luke's
AN EPIDEMIC EXPLAINED
215
Episcopal Hospital in Houston. His mother flew in from Midland.
"Scott is beet red, he's sweating, and his hands are so swollen that
they have trouble getting his ring off. He is burning up, and his [lab¬
oratory] tests are wacko. They are off the wall. His cholesterol is
sky-high. His triglycerides are off the charts."
Scott's pancreas was shutting down. Zyprexa was known to
cause pancreatitis, but the doctors at St. Luke's didn't connect the
dots. They kept Scott on that drug until his death on December 7. "I
had always told him to take his meds," his mother says. "I said,
'Scott, if I ever find out you are off your meds, I will come to Hous¬
ton and shoot you.' That's what I said to him. And here he is doing
everything he thinks he needs to do to be functional in our society,
to be a productive member of society, and it kills him.”
I
I
The Epidemic Spreads to Children
"For many parents and families, the experience (of
having a child diagnosed with a mental illness] can be
a disaster; we must say that.
-E. JANE COSTELLO, PROFESSOR OF PSYCHIATRY AT
DUKE UNIVERSITY ( 2 0 0 6 )'
The prescribing of psychiatric drugs to children and adolescents is a
recent phenomenon, as relatively few youth were medicated prior to
1980, and so as we investigate this story, we have an opportunity to
put the thesis of this book to a second test. Do we find, in the scien¬
tific literature and in societal data, that the medicating of children and
teenagers is doing more harm than good? Is it putting many children,
who initially may be struggling with a relatively minor problem —a
disinterest in school, or a bout of sadness —onto a path that leads to
lifelong disability? One of the principles of science is that the re¬
sults from an experiment should be replicable, and in essence the
medicating of children makes for a second experiment. First we
medicated adults diagnosed with mental illness, and as we saw in the
previous chapters, that did not lead to good long-term outcomes.
Next, over the past thirty years, we diagnosed children and adoles¬
cents with various disorders and put them on psychiatric drugs, and
now we can see if the results this second time around are the same.
I realize that this frames our investigation of the medicating
of children in a rather cold, analytical way, given the frightening
possibility at stake here. If the outcomes are the same in children
and teenagers as in adults, then the prescribing of psychiatric drugs
to millions of American youth is causing harm on an almost
THE EPIDEMIC SPREADS TO CHILDREN
2ly
unfathomable scale. But that possibility lends itself to an emotional
review of the medical literature, which is precisely why we are going
to conduct our inquiry in the most dispassionate manner possible.
We need the facts to speak for themselves.
The story of progress that psychiatry tells about the medicating
of children is slightly different in kind from the one it tells about its
advances in care for adults. In 1955, when Thorazine arrived, there
were hundreds of thousands of adults in mental hospitals, and they
were diagnosed with illnesses that had a recognizable past. But
when the psychopharmacology era began, very few children were
diagnosed as "mentally ill." There were bullies and goof-offs in ele¬
mentary schools, but they were not diagnosed with attention-deficit/
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), as that diagnosis had yet to be
born. There were moody and emotionally volatile teenagers, but so¬
ciety's expectation was that they would grow up into more-or-less
normal adults. However, once psychiatry began treating children
with psychotropic medications, it rethought that view of childhood.
The story that psychiatry now tells is that during the past fifty years
it discovered that children regularly suffer from mental illnesses,
which are said to be biological in kind. First psychiatry fleshed out
ADHD as an identifiable disease, and then it determined that major
depression and bipolar illness regularly struck children and adoles¬
cents. Here's how Harvard Medical School psychiatrist Ronald
Kessler summed up this "history" in 2 001:
Although epidemiological studies of child and adolescent
mood disorders have been carried out for many years,
progress long was hampered by two misconceptions: that
mood disorders are rare before adulthood and that mood dis¬
turbance is a normative and self-limiting aspect of child and
adolescent development. Research now makes it clear that
neither of these beliefs is true. Depression, mania, and mania¬
like symptoms are all comparatively common among children
and adolescents in the general population." 2
Illnesses that used to go undetected, it seems, have now been
identified. The second part of this story of scientific progress tells of
2 1 8 •
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
how psychiatric medications are both helpful and necessary.
Millions of children who used to suffer in silence are now getting
treatment that helps them thrive. Indeed, the story now emerging in
pediatric psychiatry is that psychotropic medications help create
healthy brains. In his 2006 book Child and Adolescent Psycho-
pharmacology Made Simple, psychiatrist John O'Neal explained to
readers why it was so essential that children with mental illness be
treated with medication:
Increasing evidence shows that some psychiatric disorders are
subject to progressive neurobiological impairment if they go
untreated. . . . Toxic levels of neurotransmitters, such as gluta¬
mates, or stress hormones, such as Cortisol, may damage neural
tissue or interfere with normal pathways of neuromaturation.
Pharmacological treatment of those disorders may be not only
successful in improving symptoms, but also neuroprotective (in
other words, medical treatments may either protect against
brain damage or promote normal neuromaturation)." 3
If this is true, psychiatry has indeed made a great leap ahead
in the past thirty years. The field has learned to diagnose brain
illnesses in children that used to go unnoticed, and its "neuro¬
protective" drugs now turn them into normal adults.
The Rise of ADHD
Although attention-deficit disorder did not show up in psychiatry's
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual until 1980, the field likes to point
out that it didn't just appear out of thin air. This is a disorder that
traces its medical roots back to 1902. That year, Sir George Freder¬
ick Still, a British pediatrician, published a series of lectures on
twenty children who were of normal intelligence but "exhibited vio¬
lent outbursts, wanton mischievousness, destructiveness, and a lack
of responsiveness to punishment." 4 Moreover, he reasoned that
their bad behavior arose from a biological problem (as opposed to
THE EPIDEMIC SPREADS TO CHILDREN
219
bad parenting). Children with known diseases —epilepsy, brain tu¬
mors, or meningitis —were often aggressively defiant, and thus Still
figured that these twenty children suffered from "minimal brain
dysfunction,” even though there was no obvious illness or trauma
that had caused it.
Over the next fifty years, a handful of others advanced the notion
that hyperactivity was a marker for brain injury. Children who re¬
covered from encephalitis lethargica, a viral epidemic that swept
around the globe from 1917 to 1928, often exhibited antisocial be¬
haviors and severe emotional swings, leading pediatricians to con¬
clude that the illness had caused mild brain damage, even though
the nature of that damage couldn't be identified. In 1947, Alfred
Strauss, who was the director of a school for disturbed youth in
Racine, Wisconsin, called his extremely hyperactive students "nor¬
mal brain injured children." 5 Psychiatry's first Diagnostic and Sta¬
tistical Manual, published in 1952, said such children suffered from
an "organic brain syndrome.”
The notion that stimulants might be beneficial for such children
arose in 1937, when Charles Bradley gave a newly synthesized am¬
phetamine, Benzedrine, to hyperactive children who complained of
headaches. Although the drug didn't cure their head pain, Bradley
reported that it "subdued” the children and helped them concen¬
trate better on their schoolwork. The children dubbed Benzedrine
the "arithmetic pill."" Although his report was mostly forgotten
for the next twenty years, in 1956 Ciba-Geigy brought Ritalin
(methylphenidate) to market as a treatment for narcolepsy, touting
it as a "safe” alternative to amphetamines, and physicians at Johns
Hopkins University School of Medicine, who were aware of
Bradley's findings, soon deemed this new drug useful for quieting
"disturbed" children who were thought to be suffering from a
"brain damage syndrome." 7
There was no great rush by psychiatrists during the 1960s to pre¬
scribe Ritalin to fidgety children who went to regular schools. At
that time, there was a sense that psychoactive drugs, because of
their many risks, should be administered only to hospitalized chil¬
dren, or children in residential facilities. The population of children
so hyperactive that they might be diagnosed with "organic brain
220
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
dysfunction" was small. However, psychiatry's use of Ritalin slowly
began to climb during the 1970s, such that by the end of the decade
perhaps 150,000 children in the United States were taking the drug.
Then, in 1980, the field published a third edition of its Diagnostic
and Statistical Manual (DSM-III), and it identified "attention-deficit
disorder" as a disease for the first time. The cardinal symptoms were
"hyperactivity,” "inattention," and "impulsivity," and given that
many children fidget in their seats and have trouble paying attention
in school, the diagnosis of ADD began to take off. In 1987, psy¬
chiatry further loosened the diagnostic boundaries, renaming it
attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a revised edition of DSM-
III. Next, Ciba-Geigy helped fund Children and Adults with Atten¬
tion Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (CHADD), a "patient-support
group” that immediately began promoting public awareness of this
"disease.” Finally, in 1991, CHADD successfully lobbied Congress
to include ADHD as a disability that would be covered by the Indi¬
viduals with Disabilities Education Act. Children diagnosed with
ADHD were now eligible for special services, which were to be
funded with federal money, and schools regularly began identifying
children who seemed to have this condition. As the Harvard Review
of Psychiatry noted in 2009, even today the diagnosis of ADHD
arises primarily from teacher complaints, as "only a minority of
children with the disorder exhibit symptoms during a physician's
office visit." 8
Suddenly, ADHD children could be found in every classroom.
The number of children so diagnosed rose to nearly 1 million in
1990, and more than doubled over the next five years. Today,
perhaps 3.5 million American children take a stimulant for ADHD,
with the Centers for Disease Control reporting in 2 007 that one
in every twenty-three American children four to seventeen years
old is so medicated. This prescribing practice is mostly a U.S.
phenomenon —children here consume three times the quantity of
stimulants consumed by the rest of the world's children combined.
Although the public often hears that research has shown that
ADHD is a "brain disease,” the truth is that its etiology remains
unknown. "Attempts to define a biological basis for ADHD have
been consistently unsuccessful," wrote pediatric neurologist Gerald
THE EPIDEMIC SPREADS TO CHILDREN
221
Golden in 1991. "The neuroanatomy of the brain, as demonstrated
by imaging studies, is normal. No neuropathologic substrate has
been demonstrated." 9 Seven years later, a panel of experts convened
by the National Institutes of Health reiterated this same point:
"After years of clinical research and experience with ADHD, our
knowledge about the cause or causes of ADHD remains largely
speculative." 10 During the 1990s, CHADD advised the public that
children with ADHD suffered from a chemical imbalance, charac¬
terized by an underactive dopamine system, but that was simply
a drug-marketing claim. Ritalin and other stimulants increase
dopamine levels in the synaptic cleft, and thus CHADD was at¬
tempting to make it seem that such drugs "normalized" brain
chemistry, but, as the American Psychiatric Press's 1997 Textbook of
Neuropsychiatry confessed, "efforts to identify a selective neuro¬
chemical imbalance [in ADHD children] have been disappointing.” 11
So we see in this history that nothing new was discovered that
told of a "mental illness" called ADHD. There was a long record of
speculation within medicine that extremely hyperactive children suf¬
fered from brain dysfunction of some kind, which was certainly a
reasonable thought, but the nature of that dysfunction was never dis¬
cerned, and then, in 1980, psychiatry simply created, with a stroke of
its pen in DSM-III, a dramatically expanded definition of "hyperac¬
tivity.” The fidgety seven-year-old boy who might have been dubbed
a "goof-off" in 1970 was now suffering from a psychiatric disorder.
Given that the biology of ADHD remains unknown, it is fair to
say that Ritalin and other ADHD drugs "work” by perturbing
neurotransmitter systems. Ritalin could best be described as a
dopamine reuptake inhibitor. At a therapeutic dose, it blocks 70
percent of the "transporters" that remove dopamine from the
synaptic cleft and bring it back into the presynaptic neuron. Co¬
caine acts on the brain in the same way. However, methylphenidate
clears much more slowly from the brain than cocaine does, and thus
it blocks dopamine reuptake for hours, as opposed to cocaine's rel¬
atively brief disruption of this function. 10
* The fact that cocaine is so short-acting is why it is more addictive than
methylphenidate, for as soon as it leaves the brain, the addict may want to
222
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
In response to methylphenidate, the child's brain goes through a
series of compensatory adaptations. Dopamine is now remaining in
the synaptic cleft too long, and so the child's brain dials down its
dopamine machinery. The density of dopamine receptors on the
postsynaptic neurons declines. At the same time, the amount of
dopamine metabolites in the cerebrospinal fluid drops, evidence that
the presynaptic neurons are releasing less of it. Ritalin also acts on
serotonin and norepinephrine neurons, and that causes similar com¬
pensatory changes in those two pathways. Receptor densities for
serotonin and norepinephrine decline, and the output of those two
chemicals by presynaptic neurons is altered as well. The child's brain
is now operating, as Steven Hyman said, in a manner that is "quali¬
tatively as well as quantitatively different from the normal state." 12
Now we can turn our attention to the outcomes data. Does this
treatment help children diagnosed with ADHD over the long term?
What does the scientific literature show?
Passive, Sitting Still, and Alone
Ritalin and other ADHD drugs do reliably change a child's behav¬
ior, and in his 1937 report, Charles Bradley set the stage for the effi¬
cacy story that eventually emerged: "Fifteen of the thirty children
responded to Benzedrine by becoming distinctly subdued in their
emotional responses. Clinically in all cases this was an improvement
from the social viewpoint." 15 Ritalin, which the FDA approved for
use in children in 1961, was found to have a similar subduing effect.
In a 1978 double-blind study, Ohio State University psychologist
Herbert Rie studied twenty-eight "hyperactive" children for three
months, half of whom were prescribed methylphenidate. Here is
what he wrote:
experience again the "rush" that comes when dopaminergic pathways are first
sent into a hyperactive state.
THE EPIDEMIC SPREADS TO CHILDREN
223
Children who were retrospectively confirmed to have been on
active drug treatment appeared, at the times of evaluation,
distinctly more bland or "flat” emotionally, lacking both the
age-typical variety and frequency of emotional expression.
They responded less, exhibited little or no initiative or spon¬
taneity, offered little indication of either interest or aversion,
showed virtually no curiosity, surprise, or pleasure, and
seemed devoid of humor. Jocular comments and humorous
situations passed unnoticed. In short, while on active drug
treatment, the children were relatively but unmistakably
affectless, humorless, and apathetic. 11
Numerous investigators reported similar observations. Children
on Ritalin show "a marked drug-related increase in solitary play
and a corresponding reduction in their initiation of social interac¬
tions,” announced Russell Barkley, a psychologist at the Medical
College of Wisconsin, in 1978.” This drug, observed Bowling
Green State University psychologist Nancy Fiedler, reduced a child's
"curiosity about the environment." 16 At times, the medicated child
"loses his sparkle," wrote Canadian pediatrician Till Davy in
1989. 17 Children treated with a stimulant, concluded a team of
UCLA psychologists in 1993, often become "passive, submissive”
and "socially withdrawn." 18 Some children on the drug "seem
zombie-like,” noted psychologist James Swanson, director of an
ADHD center at the University of California, Irvine.' 9 Stimulants,
explained the editors of the Oxford Textbook of Clinical Psycho-
phamacology and Drug Therapy, curb hyperactivity by "reducing
the number of behavioral responses." 10
All of these reports told the same story. On Ritalin, a student
who previously had been an annoyance in the classroom, fidgeting
too much in his or her chair or talking to a nearby classmate while
the teacher scribbled on the blackboard, would be stilled. The stu¬
dent wouldn't move around as much and wouldn't engage as much
socially with his or her peers. If given a task like answering arith¬
metic problems, the student might focus intently on it. Charles
Bradley thought this change in behavior was "an improvement
from the social viewpoint," and it is that perspective that shows up
224
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
in efficacy trials of Ritalin and other ADHD drugs. Teachers and
other observers fill out rating instruments that view a reduction in
the child's movements and engagement with others as positive, and
when the results are tabulated, 70 to 90 percent of the children are
reported to be "good responders" to ADHD medications. These
drugs, NIMH investigators wrote in 1995, are highly effective in
"dramatically reducing a range of core ADHD symptoms such as
task-irrelevant activity (e.g., finger tapping, fidgetiness, fine motor
movement, off-task [behavior] during direct observation) and class¬
room disturbance." 21 ADHD experts at Massachusetts General
Hospital summed up the scientific literature in a similar way: "The
extant literature clearly documents that stimulants diminish behav¬
iors prototypical of ADHD, including motoric overactivity, impul-
sivity, and inattentiveness."”
However, none of this tells of drug treatment that benefits the
child. Stimulants work for the teacher, but do they help the child?
Here, right from the start, researchers ran into a wall. "Above all
else,” wrote Esther Sleator, a physician at the University of Illinois
who asked fifty-two children what they thought of Ritalin, "we
found a pervasive dislike among hyperactive children for taking
stimulants." 2 ' Children on Ritalin, University of Texas psychologist
Deborah Jacobvitz reported in 1990, rated themselves as "less
happy and [less] pleased with themselves and more dysphoric."
When it came to helping a child make friends and sustain friend¬
ships, stimulants produced "few significant positive effects and a
high incidence of negative effects," Jacobvitz said. 24 Other re¬
searchers detailed how Ritalin harmed a child's self-esteem, as the
children felt they must be "bad” or "dumb" if they had to take such
a pill. "The child comes to believe not in the soundness of his own
brain and body, not in his own growing ability to learn and to con¬
trol his behavior, but in 'my magic pills that make me into a good
boy,' " said University of Minnesota psychologist Alan Sroufe. 25
All of this told of harm done, of a drug that made a child de¬
pressed, lonely, and filled with a sense of inadequacy, and when
researchers looked at whether Ritalin at least helped hyperactive
children fare well academically, to get good grades and thus succeed
as students, they found that it wasn't so. Being able to focus intently
THE EPIDEMIC SPREADS TO CHILDREN
225
on a math test, it turned out, didn't translate into long-term aca¬
demic achievement. This drug, Sroufe explained in 1973, enhances
performance on "repetitive, routinized tasks that require sustained
attention," but "reasoning, problem solving and learning do not
seem to be [positively] affected." 26 Five years later, Herbert Rie was
much more negative. He reported that Ritalin did not produce any
benefit on the students' "vocabulary, reading, spelling, or math,"
and hindered their ability to solve problems. "The reactions of the
children strongly suggest a reduction in commitment of the sort that
would seem critical for learning." 22 That same year, Russell Barkley
at the Medical College of Wisconsin reviewed the relevant scientific
literature and concluded "the major effect of stimulants appears to
be an improvement in classroom manageability rather than aca¬
demic performance." 28 Next it was James Swanson's turn to weigh
in. The fact that the drugs often left children "isolated, withdrawn
and overfocused” could "impair rather than improve learning," he
said. 29 Carol Whalen, a psychologist from the University of Califor¬
nia at Irvine, noted in 1997 that "especially worrisome has been the
suggestion that the unsalutary effects [of Ritalin] occur in the realm
of complex, high-order cognitive functions such as flexible problem¬
solving or divergent thinking.'" 0 Finally, in 2002, Canadian inves¬
tigators conducted a meta-analysis of the literature, reviewing
fourteen studies involving 1,379 youths that had lasted at least
three months, and they determined that there was "little evidence
for improved academic performance.””
There was one other disappointment with Ritalin. When re¬
searchers looked at whether stimulants improved a child’s behavior
over the long term, they couldn't find any benefit. When a child
stopped taking Ritalin, ADHD behaviors regularly flared up, the
"excitability, impulsivity, or talkativeness" worse than ever. "It is
often disheartening to observe how rapidly behavior deteriorates
when medication is discontinued," Whalen confessed. 32 Nor was
there evidence that staying on a stimulant led to a sustained im¬
provement in behavior. "Teachers and parents should not expect
long-term improvement in academic achievement or reduced anti¬
social behavior," Swanson wrote in 1993." The 1994 edition of the
APA's Textbook of Psychiatry admitted to the same bottom-line
226
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
conclusion: "Stimulants do not produce lasting improvements in
aggressivity, conduct disorder, criminality, education achievement,
job functioning, marital relationships, or long-term adjustment." 34
Thirty years of research had failed to provide any good-quality evi¬
dence that stimulants helped "hyperactive” children thrive, and in
the early 1990s, a team of prominent ADHD experts picked to lead
a long-term NIMH study, known as the Multisite Multimodal Treat¬
ment Study of Children with ADHD, acknowledged that this was so.
"The long-term efficacy of stimulant medication has not been
demonstrated for any domain of child functioning,” they wrote. 35
Stimulants Flunk Out
The NIMH touted its ADHD study as "the first major clinical trial”
the institute had ever conducted of "a childhood mental disorder.”
However, it was a rather flawed intellectual exercise right from the
start. Although the investigators, led by Peter Jensen, associate di¬
rector of child and adolescent research at the NIMH, acknowledged
during the planning stages that there was no evidence in the scien¬
tific literature that stimulants improved long-term outcomes, they
did not include a placebo control in the study, reasoning that it
would have been "unethical" to withhold "treatment of known effi¬
cacy” for an extended period. The study basically compared drug
treatment to behavioral therapy, but in that latter group, 20 percent
were on a stimulant at the start of the trial, and there never was a
time during the fourteen months that all of the children in that
group were off such medication. 36
Despite this obvious design flaw, the NIMH-funded investigators
declared victory for the stimulants at the end of fourteen months.
"Carefully crafted medication management" had proven to be "su¬
perior" to behavioral treatment in terms of reducing core ADHD
symptoms. There was also a hint that the medicated children had
fared better on reading tests (although not in other academic sub¬
jects), and as a result, psychiatry now had a long-term study that
documented the continuing benefits of stimulants. "Since ADHD is
THE EPIDEMIC SPREADS TO CHILDREN
227
now regarded by most experts as a chronic disorder, ongoing treat¬
ment often seems necessary,” the researchers concluded. 37
After that initial fourteen-month period of treatment, the investi¬
gators followed up periodically with the students, assessing how
they were doing and whether they were taking an ADHD medica¬
tion. This was now a naturalistic study much like the one that Mar¬
tin Harrow had conducted of schizophrenia outcomes, and readers
of this book, having become familiar with the scientific literature,
can easily guess what is coming next. At the end of three years,
Jensen and the others discovered that "medication use was a signifi¬
cant marker not of beneficial outcome, but of deterioration. That is,
participants using medication in the 24-to-36 month period actually
showed increased symptomatology during that interval relative to
those not taking medication." 38
In other words, those on medications saw their core ADHD symp¬
toms—the impulsiveness, the inattentiveness, the hyperactivity —
worsen, at least in comparison to those not on drugs. In addition,
those on meds had higher "delinquency scores" at the end of three
years, which meant they were more likely to get into trouble in
school and with the police." They were also now shorter and
weighed less than their off-med counterparts, evidence that the
drugs suppressed growth. These results told of a drug therapy
causing long-term harm, and when the NIMH-funded investigators
reported on six-year outcomes, the findings remained the same. Med¬
ication use was "associated with worse hyperactivity-impulsivity and
oppositional defiant disorder symptoms" and with greater "overall
functional impairment." 40
Controversy has long raged over whether ADHD is a "real" dis¬
ease, but this study showed that when it comes to using stimulants to
treat it, the controversy is moot. Even if ADHD is real, stimulants
aren't going to provide long-term help. "We had thought that chil¬
dren medicated longer would have better outcomes. That didn't hap¬
pen to be the case," said William Pelham from the State University of
New York at Buffalo, who was one of the principal investigators.
"There were no beneficial effects, none. In the short term, I medica¬
tion] will help the child behave better, in the long run it won't. And
that information should be made very clear to parents.” 41
228
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Tallying Up the Harm
With any medication, there is a benefit-risk assessment to be made,
and the expectation is that the benefit will outweigh the risks. But in
this case, the NIMH found that over the long term there was noth¬
ing to be entered on the benefit side of the ledger. That leaves only
risks to be tallied up, and so now we need to look at all the ways
that stimulants can harm children.
Ritalin and the other ADHD medications cause a long list of
physical, emotional, and psychiatric adverse effects. The physical
problems include drowsiness, appetite loss, lethargy, insomnia,
headaches, abdominal pain, motor abnormalities, facial and vocal
tics, jaw clenching, skin problems, liver disorders, weight loss,
growth suppression, hypertension, and sudden cardiac death. The
emotional difficulties include depression, apathy, a general dullness,
mood swings, crying jags, irritability, anxiety, and a sense of hostil¬
ity toward the world. The psychiatric problems include obsessive-
compulsive symptoms, mania, paranoia, psychotic episodes, and
hallucinations. Methylphenidate also reduces blood flow and glu¬
cose metabolism in the brain, changes that usually are associated
with "neuropathologic states." 42
Animal studies of stimulants are also cause for alarm. Repeated
exposure to amphetamines, scientists at the Yale School of Medicine
reported in 1999, caused monkeys to exhibit "aberrant behaviors"
that remained long after the drug exposure had stopped. 43 Various
rat studies suggested that lengthy exposure to methylphenidate
might cause dopaminergic pathways to become permanently desen¬
sitized, and since dopamine is the brain's "reward system," med¬
icating the child may produce an adult with a "reduced ability to
experience pleasure.Scientists at Texas Southwestern Medical
Center in Dallas found that "preadolescent” rats exposed to
methylphenidate for fifteen days turned into anxious, depressed
"adult" rats. The adult rats moved around less, were less responsive
to novel environments, and showed a "deficit in sexual behavior.”
They concluded that "administration of methylphenidate” while
THE EPIDEMIC SPREADS TO CHILDREN
229
the brain is still developing "results in aberrant behavioral adapta¬
tions during adulthood." 45
Such is the outcomes literature for Ritalin and other ADHD
medications. The drugs alter a hyperactive child's behavior over the
short term in a manner that teachers and some parents find helpful,
but other than that, the medications diminish a child's life in many
ways, and they may turn a child into an adult with a reduced phys¬
iological capacity to experience joy. And, as we'll see later in this
chapter, there is one other heartbreaking risk with stimulants that
remains to be explored.
Depressing Results
As recently as 1988, the year that Prozac came to market, only one
in 250 children under nineteen years of age in the United States was
taking an antidepressant. 46 That was partly due to a cultural belief
that youth were naturally moody and recovered quickly from de¬
pressive episodes, and partly because study after study had shown
that tricyclics worked no better than placebo in this age group.
"There is no escaping the fact that research studies certainly have
not supported the efficacy of tricyclic antidepressants in treated
depressed adolescents," a Journal of Child and Adolescent Psycho¬
pharmacology editorial acknowledged in 1992. 47
However, when Prozac and other SSRIs were brought to market
and touted as wonder drugs, the prescribing of antidepressants to
children took off. The percentage of children so medicated tripled
between 1988 and 1994, and by 2002 one in every forty children
under nineteen years of age in the United States was taking an anti¬
depressant. 48 Presumably these drugs provide a short-term benefit to
children and adolescents that the tricyclics fail to provide, but unfor¬
tunately, we can't review the scientific literature to see if that is true
because, as is widely acknowledged today, the literature is hopelessly
poisoned. The trials were biased by design; the results that were
published in the scientific journals didn't square with the actual
230
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
data; adverse events were downplayed or omitted; and negative
studies went unpublished or were spun into positive ones. "The
story of research into selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor use in
childhood depression is one of confusion, manipulation, and institu¬
tional failure,” the Lancet wrote in a 2004 editorial. The fact that
psychiatrists at leading medical schools had participated in this
scientific fraud constituted an "abuse of the trust patients place in
their physicians." 49
However, a somewhat accurate picture of the merits of the drugs'
efficacy in children has emerged through a roundabout process. Dur¬
ing the course of SSRI-related lawsuits, expert witnesses for the
plaintiffs —most notably David Healy in England and Peter Breggin
in the United States —got a look at some of the trial data, and they
observed that the drugs increased the suicide risk. They spoke out
about what they had found, and with an increasing number of an¬
guished parents telling of how their children had killed themselves
after going on an SSRI, the FDA was forced to hold a hearing in
2004 on this risk. That, in turn, led to a stunning admission by the
FDA's Thomas Laughren about the drugs' efficacy in children.
Twelve of the fifteen pediatric antidepressant trials that had been
conducted had failed. The FDA, in fact, had rejected the applications
of six manufacturers seeking approval to sell their antidepressants to
children. "These are sobering findings," Laughren confessed. 50
The FDA did approve Prozac for use in children, as two of the
three positive studies reviewed by Laughren had come from trials of
this drug. But, as many critics have pointed out, from a scientific
perspective, there is no reason to think that Prozac is any better
than the other SSRIs. The percentage of children who responded to
Prozac in the two positive trials was similar to the drug response
rate in the twelve failed trials; Eli Lilly simply had been better at
using biased trial designs to make it appear that its drug worked.
For example, in one of the two Prozac trials, all of the children were
initially put on placebo for one week, and if they got better during
that period, they were excluded from the study. This helped knock
down the placebo response rate. Next, the children who were ran¬
domized onto Prozac were evaluated for a week, and only those
"who adapted well" to the drug were then enrolled in the study.
THE EPIDEMIC SPREADS TO CHILDREN
231
This helped increase the drug response rate. "Before the study even
started,” explained Jonathan Leo, editor in chief of the journal Eth¬
ical Human Psychology and Psychiatry, "there was a mechanism in
place to maximize any difference between the drug and placebo
groups —the placebo group was preselected for nonresponders,
while the drug group was preselected for responders Yet, even
with this extremely biased trial design, the Prozac-treated children
still fared no better than the placebo group on self-rating scales or
ratings by their parents. In addition, the trial failed to show efficacy
for fluoxetine on its "primary endpoint,” and thus efficacy arose en¬
tirely from a secondary "improvement" scale filled out by the psy¬
chiatrists paid by Eli Lilly to run the trial.
Such was the record of efficacy produced by the SSRIs in pedi¬
atric trials for depression. Most trials failed to show any benefit,
and Eli Lilly had to use a grossly biased trial design to make Prozac
appear effective. In 2003, the Medicines and Healthcare Regulatory
Agency (MHRA) in the United Kingdom essentially banned the use
of SSRIs, except for fluoxetine, in patients under eighteen years old.
English scientists then reviewed all the relevant data and reported in
the Lancet that they supported "the conclusions reached by the
MHRA." 52 The truth, explained the Lancet editors in an accompa¬
nying editorial, was that these drugs "were both ineffective and
harmful in children." 53 Australian scientists chimed in with a simi¬
lar review in the British Medical journal, their article enlivened by
descriptions of the shenanigans that American psychiatrists had
employed to make the SSRIs look beneficial in the first place. The
authors of the positive studies, they said, had "exaggerated the
benefits, downplayed the harms, or both.” The Australians also re¬
viewed Lilly's fluoxetine trials in children and determined that the
"evidence for efficacy is not convincing.” As such, they concluded
that "recommending [any antidepressant] as a treatment option, let
alone as first line treatment, would be inappropriate." 54
In the absence of any efficacy benefit, we are now left with
the unhappy task of tallying up the harm done by the prescribing
of antidepressants to children and teenagers. We can start with
the physical problems. SSRIs may cause insomnia, sexual dysfunc¬
tion, headaches, gastrointestinal problems, dizziness, tremors, ner-
232
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
vousness, muscle cramps, muscle weakness, seizures, and a severe
inner agitation known as akathisia, which is associated with an in¬
creased risk of violence and suicide. The psychiatric problems they
can trigger are even more problematic. Timothy Wilens and Joseph
Biederman at Massachusetts General Hospital conducted a chart re¬
view of eighty-two children treated with SSRIs, and determined that
22 percent of the children had suffered an adverse psychiatric event.
Ten percent had become psychotic, and another 6 percent manic.
"One of the most disturbing adverse outcomes is a worsening of
emotional, cognitive or behavioral symptoms,” they wrote. "These
psychiatric adverse events to medication can be significantly impair¬
ing." 55 North Carolina psychiatrist Thomas Gualtieri determined
that 28 percent of the 128 children and adolescents he treated with
SSRIs developed some type of "behavioral toxicity." 56 Other physi¬
cians have told of their SSRI-treated younger patients suffering
panic attacks, anxiety, nervousness, and hallucinations.
Those findings tell of children and adolescents being made sick by
SSRIs, and that is over the short term. To appreciate the long-term
risks, we can look at the problems that have cropped up in adults
and in animal studies. If the children go off the medication, they can
expect to suffer withdrawal symptoms, both physical and mental.
Should they remain on the drugs for years, they are at high risk of
becoming chronically depressed. They may also develop —as the
American Psychiatric Association warns in one of its textbooks —an
"apathy syndrome,” which "is characterized by a loss of motivation,
increased passivity, and often feelings of lethargy and 'flatness.' " S7
There is also memory loss and cognitive decline to worry about, and,
as we saw earlier, animal studies suggest that the drugs may cause
serotonergic neurons to become swollen and misshapen.
Yet Another Illness Appears
First there was the ADHD explosion, and then came the news that
childhood depression was rampant, and not long after that, in the
late 1990s, juvenile bipolar disorder burst into public view. News-
THE EPIDEMIC SPREADS TO CHILDREN
233
papers and magazines ran features on this phenomenon, and once
more psychiatry explained its appearance with a story of scientific
discovery. "It has long been thought in the psychiatric community
that children could not be given a diagnosis of bipolar disorder until
the mid-to-late teens, and that mania in children was extremely
rare,” wrote psychiatrist Demitri Papolos, in his bestselling book
The Bipolar Child. "But scientists in the research vanguard are be¬
ginning to prove that the disorder can begin very early in life and
that it is far more common than was previously supposed." 5 ' Yet
the rise in the number of children and adolescents with this diagno¬
sis was so astonishing —a fortyfold increase from 1995 to 2003 —
that Time, in an article titled "Young and Bipolar," wondered if
something else might be going on. 59 "New awareness of the disor¬
der may not be enough to account for the explosion of juvenile
bipolar cases,” the magazine explained. "Some scientists fear that
there may be something in the environment or in modern lifestyles
that is driving into a bipolar state children and teens who might
otherwise escape the condition." 60
That speculation made perfect sense. How could a severe mental
illness have gone unrecognized for so long, with doctors only now
noticing that thousands of kids were going wildly manic? But if
there were something new in the environment stirring this behavior,
as Time suggested to its readers, there would be a logical explan¬
ation for the epidemic. Infectious agents stir epidemics, and thus, as
we trace the rise of juvenile bipolar disorder, this is what we'll want
to discover: Can we identify "outside agents" that are causing this
modern-day plague?
As we learned earlier, manic-depressive illness was a rare condi¬
tion prior to the psychopharmacology era, affecting perhaps one in
ten thousand people. Although initial onset sometimes occurred
in those fifteen to nineteen years old, it usually didn't appear until
people were in their twenties. But more to the point, it virtually
never appeared in children under thirteen years of age, and both pe¬
diatricians and medical researchers regularly emphasized this point.
In 1945, Charles Bradley said that pediatric mania was so rare
that "it is best to avoid the diagnosis of manic-depressive psycho¬
sis in children." 61 An Ohio physician, Louis Lurie, reviewed the
234
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
literature in 1950 and found that "observers have concluded that
mania does not occur in children.'"' Two years later, Barton Hall
reviewed the case histories of 2,200 psychiatric patients five to six¬
teen years old, and found only two instances of manic-depressive ill¬
ness. In both instances, the patients were over thirteen years of age.
"These facts endorse the general belief that manic-depressive states
are illnesses of the maturing or matured personality," Hall said. 6 ' In
1960, Washington University psychiatrist James Anthony scoured
the medical literature for case reports of manic-depressive illness in
children and could find only three. "Occurrence of manic depres¬
sion in early childhood as a clinical phenomenon has yet to be
demonstrated," he wrote. 64
But then, slowly but surely, such case reports began to appear. In
the late 1960s and early 1970s, psychiatrists began prescribing Rit¬
alin to hyperactive children, and suddenly, in 1976, Washington
University's Warren Weinberg, a pediatric neurologist, was writing
in the American journal of Diseases of Childhood that it was time
for the field to realize that children could go manic. "Acceptance of
the concept that mania occurs in children is important in order that
affected children can be identified, the natural history defined, and
appropriate treatment established and offered to these children," he
wrote. 65
This was the moment in the medical literature that pediatric
bipolar disorder was, in essence, "discovered." In his article, Wein¬
berg reviewed the case histories of five children suffering from this
previously unrecognized illness, but he rushed past the fact that at
least three of the five children had been treated with a tricyclic or
Ritalin prior to becoming manic. Two years later, doctors at Massa¬
chusetts General Hospital announced that they had identified nine
children with manic-depressive illness, and they, too, skipped over
the fact that seven of the nine had been previously treated with am¬
phetamines, methylphenidate, or "other medications to affect be¬
havior." 66 Then, in 1982, Michael Strober and Gabrielle Carlson at
the UCLA Neuropsychiatries Institute put a new twist into the juve¬
nile bipolar story. Twelve of the sixty adolescents they had treated
with antidepressants had turned "bipolar" over the course of three
THE EPIDEMIC SPREADS TO CHILDREN
235
years, which —one might think —suggested that the drugs had
caused the mania. Instead, Strober and Carlson reasoned that their
study had shown that antidepressants could be used as a diagnostic
tool. It wasn't that antidepressants were causing some children to
go manic, but rather the drugs were unmasking bipolar illness, as
only children with the disease would suffer this reaction to an anti¬
depressant. "Our data imply that biologic differences between la¬
tent depressive subtypes are already present and detectable during
the period of early adolescence, and that pharmacologic challenge
can serve as one reliable aid in delimiting specific affective syn¬
dromes in juveniles," they said."
The "unmasking" of bipolar illness in children soon speeded up.
The prescribing of Ritalin and antidepressants took off in the late
1980s and early 1990s, and as this occurred, the bipolar epidemic
erupted. The number of hostile, aggressive, and out-of-control chil¬
dren admitted to psychiatric wards soared, and in 1995 Peter
Lewinsohn from the Oregon Research Institute concluded that
1 percent of all American adolescents were now bipolar. 68 Three
years later, Carlson reported that 63 percent of the pediatric pa¬
tients treated at her university hospital suffered from mania, the
very symptom that doctors in the pre-psychopharmacologic era al¬
most never saw in children. "Manic symptoms are the rule, rather
than the exception,” she noted. 69 Indeed, Lewinsohn's epidemiolog¬
ical data was now already out of date. The number of children
discharged from hospitals with a bipolar diagnosis rose fivefold be¬
tween 1996 and 2004, such that this "ferocious mental illness" was
now said to strike one in every fifty prepubertal children in Amer¬
ica. "We don't have the exact numbers yet,” University of Texas
psychiatrist Robert Hirschfeld told Time in 2002, "except we know
it's there, and it's underdiagnosed." 90
An epidemic had come of age, and history reveals that it rose in
lockstep with the prescribing of stimulants and antidepressants to
children.
236
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Creating the Bipolar Child
Given that chronology, we should be able to find data that explains
why stimulants and antidepressants would have that iatrogenic ef¬
fect. There should be data showing that if you treat 5 million chil¬
dren and adolescents with these drugs, then 20 percent or so will
deteriorate in ways that will lead to a bipolar diagnosis. There
should be evidence of iatrogenic harm that adds up mathematically
to an epidemic.
We'll start with Ritalin.
Even before the prescribing of Ritalin took hold, it was well
known that amphetamines could stir psychotic and manic episodes.
Indeed, amphetamines did this with such regularity that psychiatric
researchers pointed to this effect as evidence supporting the dopa¬
mine hypothesis of schizophrenia. Amphetamines upped dopamine
levels in the brain, suggesting that psychosis was caused by too
much of this neurotransmitter. In 1974, David Janowsky, a physi¬
cian at the University of California at San Diego School of Medi¬
cine, tested this hypothesis by giving three dopamine-elevating
agents —d-amphetamine, 1-amphetamine, and methylphenidate —to
his schizophrenia patients. While all three drugs made them more
psychotic, methylphenidate turned out to be tops in this regard,
doubling the severity of their symptoms. 71
Given this understanding of methylphenidate, psychiatry could
expect that giving Ritalin to young children would cause many to suf¬
fer a manic or psychotic episode. Although this risk isn't well quanti¬
fied, Canadian psychiatrists reported in 1999 that nine of ninety-six
ADHD children they treated with stimulants for an average of
twenty-one months developed "psychotic symptoms." 77 In 2006, the
FDA issued a report on this risk. From 2000 to 2005, the agency
had received nearly one thousand reports of stimulant-induced
psychosis and mania in children and adolescents, and given that
these Med Watch reports are thought to represent only 1 percent of
the actual number of adverse events, this suggests that 100,000
youths diagnosed with ADHD suffered psychotic and or manic
episodes during that five-year period. The FDA determined that
THE EPIDEMIC SPREADS TO CHILDREN
237
these episodes regularly occurred in "patients with no identifiable
risk factors" for psychosis, meaning that they were clearly drug-
induced, and that a "substantial portion" of the cases occurred in
children ten years or less. "The predominance in young children of
hallucinations, both visual and tactile, involving insects, snakes and
worms is striking," the FDA wrote. 73
Once this drug-induced psychosis occurs, the children are usually
diagnosed with bipolar disorder. Moreover, this diagnostic progres¬
sion, from medicated ADHD to bipolar illness, is well recognized by
experts in the field. In a study of 195 bipolar children and adoles¬
cents, Demitri Papolos found that 65 percent "had hypomanic,
manic and aggressive reactions to stimulant medications." 74 In
2001, Melissa DelBello, at the University of Cincinnati Medical
Center, reported that twenty-one of thirty-four adolescent patients
hospitalized for mania had been on stimulants "prior to the onset of
an affective episode.” These drugs, she confessed, may "precipitate
depression and/or mania in children who would not have otherwise
developed bipolar disorder." 75
Yet there is an even bigger problem with stimulants. They cause
children to cycle through arousal and dysphoric states on a daily
basis. When a child takes the drug, dopamine levels in the synapse
increase, and this produces an aroused state. The child may show
increased energy, an intensified focus, and hyperalertness. The child
may become anxious, irritable, aggressive, hostile, and unable to
sleep. More extreme arousal symptoms include obsessive-compulsive
and hypomanic behaviors. But when the drug exits the brain,
dopamine levels in the synapse sharply drop, and this may lead to
such dysphoric symptoms as fatigue, lethargy, apathy, social with¬
drawal, and depression. Parents regularly talk of this daily "crash."
But —and this is the key —such arousal and dysphoric symptoms are
the very symptoms that the National Institute of Mental Health
identifies as characteristic of a bipolar child. Symptoms of mania in
children, the NIMH says, include increased energy, intensified goal-
directed activity, insomnia, irritability, agitation, and destructive
outbursts. Symptoms of depression in children include loss of en¬
ergy, social isolation, a loss of interest in activities (apathy), and a
sad mood.
238
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
The ADHD to Bipolar Pathway
Stimulant-Induced Symptoms
Bipolar Symptoms
Arousal
Dysphoric
Arousal
Dysphoric
INCREASED ENERGY
SOMNOLENCE
INCREASED ENERGY
SAD MOOD
INTENSIFIED FOCUS
FATIGUE, LEIHARGY
INTENSIFIED GOAL-
LOSS OF ENERGY
HYPERALERTNESS
SOCIAL WITHDRAWAL,
DIRECTED ACIMIY
LOSS OF INTEREST IN
EUPHORIA
AGITATION, ANXIETY
INSOMNIA
IRRITABILITY
HOSimiY
ISOLATION
DECREASED SPONTANEITY
REDUCED CURIOSITY
CONSTRICTION OF AFFECT
DEPRESSION
DECREASED NEED FOR
SLEEP
SEVERE MOOD CHANGE
IRRITABILITY
AGITATION
DESTRUCTIVE OUTBURSTS
ACTIVITIES
SOCIAL ISOLATION
POOR COMMUNI¬
CATION
FEELINGS OF WORTH¬
LESSNESS
HYPOMANIA
EMOTIONAL LABILITY
INCREASED TALKING
UNEXPLAINED
MANIA
DISTRACTTBILITY
CRYING
PSYCHOSIS
HYPOMANIA
MANIA
SIMULANTS USED TO MAT ADHD INDUCE BOTH AROUSAL AND DYSPHORIC SYMPTOMS. THESE DRUG-
INDUCED SYMPTOMS OVERLAP TO A REMARKABLE DEGREE THE SYMPTOMS SAID TO BE CHARACTERISTIC OF
JUVENILE BIPOLAR DISORDER
In short, every child on a stimulant turns a bit bipolar, and the
risk that a child diagnosed with ADHD will move on to a bipolar
diagnosis after being treated with a stimulant has even been quanti¬
fied. Joseph Biederman and his colleagues at Massachusetts General
Hospital reported in 1996 that 15 of 140 children (11 percent) di¬
agnosed with ADHD developed bipolar symptoms —which were
not present at initial diagnosis —within four years.’ 6 This gives us
our first mathematical equation for solving the juvenile bipolar
epidemic: If a society prescribes stimulants to 3.5 million children
and adolescents, as is the case in the United States today, it should
expect that this practice will create 400,000 bipolar youth. As Time
noted, most children with bipolar illness are diagnosed with a
different psychiatric disorder first, with "ADHD the likeliest first
call.”
Now let's look at the SSRIs.
It is well established that antidepressants can induce manic
episodes in adults, and naturally they have this effect on children,
too. As early as 1992, when the prescribing of SSRIs to children was
just getting started. University of Pittsburgh researchers reported
THE EPIDEMIC SPREADS TO CHILDREN
239
that 23 percent of boys eight to nineteen years old treated with
Prozac developed mania or maniclike symptoms, and another 19
percent developed "drug-induced" hostility.” In Eli Lilly's first
study of Prozac for pediatric depression, 6 percent of the children
treated with the drug suffered a manic episode; none in the placebo
group did. 7 ' Luvox, meanwhile, was reported to cause a 4 percent
rate of mania in children under 18. 79 In 2004, Yale University re¬
searchers assessed this risk of antidepressant-induced mania in
young and old, and they found that it is highest in those under
thirteen years of age. 80
The incidence rates cited above are from short-term trials; the
risk rises when children and teenagers stay on antidepressants for
extended periods. In 1995, Harvard psychiatrists determined that
25 percent of children and adolescents diagnosed with depression
convert to bipolar illness within two to four years. "Antidepressant
treatment may well induce switching into mania, rapid cycling or
affective instability in the young, as it almost certainly does in
adults," they explained. 81 Washington University's Barbara Geller
extended the follow-up period to ten years, and in her study, nearly
half of prepubertal children treated for depression ended up bipo¬
lar. 82 These findings give us our second mathematical equation for
solving the bipolar epidemic: If 2 million children and adolescents
are treated with SSRIs for depression, this practice will create
500,000 to 1 million bipolar youth.
We now have numbers that tell of an iatrogenic epidemic:
400,000 bipolar children arriving via the ADHD doorway, and at
least another half million through the antidepressant doorway.
There is also a way that we can double-check that conclusion:
When investigators survey juvenile bipolar patients, do they find
that most traveled down one of those two iatrogenic paths?
Here are the results. In a 2003 study of seventy-nine juvenile
bipolar patients, University of Louisville psychiatrist Rif El-
Mallakh determined that forty-nine (62 percent) had been treated
with a stimulant or an antidepressant prior to their becoming
manic. 83 That same year, Papolos reported that 83 percent of the
195 bipolar children he studied had been diagnosed with some
other psychiatric illness first, and that two-thirds had been exposed
240
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
to an antidepressant. 81 Finally, Gianni Faedda found that 84 percent
of the children treated for bipolar illness at the Luci Bini Mood Dis¬
orders Clinic in New York City between 1998 and 2000 had been
previously exposed to psychiatric drugs. "Strikingly, in fewer than
10% [of the cases] was diagnosis of bipolar disorder considered
initially," Faedda wrote. 85
Not surprisingly, parents bear witness to this iatrogenic course.
In May 1999, Martha Hellander, executive director of the Child
and Adolescent Bipolar Foundation, and Tomie Burke, founder of
Parents of Bipolar Children, jointly wrote this letter to the Journal
of the Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry:
Most of our children initially received the ADF1D diagnosis,
were given stimulants and or antidepressants, and either did
not respond or suffered symptoms of mania such as rages,
insomnia, agitation, pressured speech, and the like. In lay lan¬
guage, parents call this "bouncing off the wall.” First hospi¬
talization occurred often among our children during manic or
mixed states (including suicidal gestures and attempts) trig¬
gered or exacerbated by treatment with stimulants, tricyclics,
or serotonin reuptake inhibitors. 86
With so many teenagers prescribed SSRIs, an epidemic of mania
has erupted on college campuses as well. In a 2002 article titled "Cri¬
sis on the Campus," Psychology Today reported that an increasing
number of students, having arrived at college with an antidepres¬
sant prescription in hand, were crashing badly during the school
term. "We are seeing more first episodes of mania every year," said
Morton Silverman, head of counseling services at the University of
Chicago. "It's very disruptive. It generally means hospitalization for
the student.” The magazine was even able to identify a precise date
when this mania epidemic began to emerge: 1988. 81 Readers need
only remember when Prozac came to market to connect the dots.
One final bit of evidence comes from the Netherlands. In 2001,
Dutch psychiatrists reported only thirty-nine cases of pediatric bi¬
polar illness in their country. Dutch investigator Catrien Reichart then
studied the offspring of parents with bipolar disorder in both the
AN EPIDEMIC UNFOLDS
24 2 -
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
United States and the Netherlands, and determined that the Ameri¬
cans were ten times more to likely to exhibit bipolar symptoms
before age twenty than the Dutch children. The likely reason for this
difference, Reichart concluded, is that "the prescription of anti¬
depressants and stimulants to children in the U.S. is much higher."”
All of this tells of an epidemic that is mostly iatrogenic in kind.
Fifty years ago, physicians virtually never saw manic-depressive ill¬
ness in preteens, and they rarely diagnosed it in adolescents. Then
pediatricians and psychiatrists began prescribing Ritalin to hyper¬
active children, and suddenly the medical journals began running
case reports of manic children. This problem grew as the prescribing
of Ritalin increased, and then it exploded with the introduction of
the SSRIs. Research then showed that both of these drugs trigger
bipolar symptoms in children and adolescents on a regular basis.
These are the two "outside agents” fueling the epidemic, and it
should be remembered that they do perturb normal brain function.
The manic children showing up at hospital emergency rooms have
dopaminergic and serotonergic pathways that have been altered by
the drugs and are now functioning in an "abnormal" manner. There
is a step-by-step logic that explains this epidemic.
In addition, there are at least three more pathways to a diagnosis
of juvenile bipolar illness. As El-Mallakh, Papolos, and Faedda all
found, there are some children and adolescents so diagnosed who
have no prior exposure to antidepressants or stimulants, and it's
fairly easy to see where the majority of those patients are coming
from. First, Harvard psychiatrist Joseph Biederman led the way in
expanding the diagnostic boundaries in the 1990s, proposing that
extreme "irritability" could be seen as evidence of bipolar illness.
The child no longer needs to have gone manic to be diagnosed as
bipolar. Second, foster children in many states are now regularly
given a bipolar diagnosis, their anger apparently not the result of
having been born into a dysfunctional family, but rather due to a bi¬
ological illness. Finally, teenagers who get into trouble with the law
are now regularly funneled into psychiatric roles. Many states have
set up "mental health courts” that send them off to hospitals and
psychiatric shelters rather than to correctional facilities, and these
youth are adding to the bipolar numbers as well.
THE EPIDEMIC SPREADS TO CHILDREN
243
The Fate That Awaits
As we saw earlier in this book, outcomes for adult bipolar patients
have deteriorated dramatically in the past forty years, and the worst
outcomes are seen in those with "mixed state” and "rapid cycling”
symptoms. That clinical course in adults was virtually never seen
prior to the psychopharmacology era, but rather it was one associ¬
ated with exposure to antidepressants, and, tragically, those are the
very symptoms that afflict the overwhelming majority of juvenile
bipolar patients. They exhibit symptoms "similar to the clinical pic¬
ture reported for severely ill, treatment-resistant adults,” explained
Barbara Geller in 1997. 89
Thus, this is not just a story of children turned bipolar; it's a story
of children afflicted with a particularly severe form of it. Papolos
found that 87 percent of his 195 juvenile bipolar patients suffered
from "ultra, ultra rapid cycling,” which meant that they were con¬
stantly switching between manic and depressed mood states. 99 Simi¬
larly, Faedda determined that 66 percent of the juvenile bipolar
patients treated at the Luci Bini Mood Disorders Clinic were "ultra,
ultra rapid-cyclers," and another 19 percent suffered from rapid cy¬
cling only a little bit less extreme. "In contrast to a biphasic, episodic
and relatively slow cycling course in some adults with bipolar disor¬
der, pediatric forms usually involve mixed mood states and a sub¬
chronic, unstable, and unremitting course," Faedda wrote. 91
Outcome studies have found that the long-term prognosis for
these children is grim. The NIMH, as part of its STEP-BD study,
charted the outcomes of 542 children and adolescent bipolar pa¬
tients, and it reported that pre-adult onset "was associated with
greater rates of comorbid anxiety disorders and substance abuse,
more recurrences, shorter periods of euthymia [normal mood], and
greater likelihood of suicide attempts and violence." 92 Boris
Birmaher, at the University of Pittsburgh, determined that "early
onset" bipolar patients are symptomatic about 60 percent of the
time, and that, on average, they shift "polarity”— from depression
to mania or vice versa —an astonishing sixteen times a year. The
prepubertal patients were "two times less likely than those with
244
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
postpubertal onset bipolar to recover," he said, and it was "ex¬
pected that children will be poor responders to treatment when they
become adults." 9 ' DelBello followed a group of adolescents hospi¬
talized for a first bipolar episode and concluded that only 41
percent functionally recovered within a year. 94 This impairment,
Birmaher determined, then worsens after the first year. "Functional
impairment in bipolar appears to increase during adolescence re¬
gardless of age of onset." 95
Youth diagnosed with bipolar illness are typically put on drug
cocktails that include an atypical antipsychotic and a mood stabi¬
lizer. This means that they now have multiple neurotransmitter path¬
ways in their brains that are being mucked up, and naturally, this
treatment does not lead them back to emotional and physical health.
In 2002, DelBello reported that lithium, antidepressants, and mood
stabilizers all failed to help bipolar youth fare better at the end of
two years. Those who were treated with a neuroleptic, she added,
"were significantly less likely to recover than those who did not re¬
ceive a neuroleptic." 96 Six years later, Hayes, Inc., a Pennsylvania
consulting firm that conducts "unbiased” assessments of drugs for
health-care providers, concluded that there was no good scientific
evidence that the mood stabilizers and atypical antipsychotics
prescribed for pediatric bipolar were either safe or effective. "Our
findings indicate that at this time, anticonvulsants and atypical anti¬
psychotics cannot be recommended for children diagnosed with
bipolar disorders," said Elisabeth Houtsmuller, senior analyst for
Hayes. 97 These reports attest to a lack of drug efficacy, but as
Houtsmuller noted, the side effects from these "pharmacological
treatments" are "alarming.” In particular, atypical antipsychotics
may cause metabolic dysfunction, hormonal abnormalities, diabetes,
obesity, emotional blunting, and tardive dyskinesia.* Eventually, the
* In a 2008 report published by the European College of Neuropsychophar¬
macology, Spanish investigators observed that "children and adolescents seem
to have a higher risk than adults for experiencing adverse events such as
extrapyramidal symptoms [movement disorders], prolactin elevation [high
hormone levels], sedation, weight gain, and metabolic effects when taking
antipsychotics." Investigators have also reported that these risks may be higher
for girls than for boys.
THE EPIDEMIC SPREADS TO CHILDREN
245
drugs will induce cognitive decline, and the child who stays on the
cocktails into adulthood can expect to die early as well.
That is the long-term course of this iatrogenic illness: A child
who may be hyperactive or depressed is treated with a drug that
triggers a manic episode or some degree of emotional instability,
and then the child is put on a drug cocktail that leads to a lifetime of
disability.
The Disability Numbers
There are no good studies yet on the percentage of "early onset”
bipolar patients who, when they reach adulthood, end up on the SSI
and SSDI disability rolls. However, the astonishing jump in the
number of "severely mentally ill" children receiving SSI speaks vol¬
umes about the havoc that is being wreaked. There were 16,200
psychiatrically disabled youth under eighteen years old on the SSI
rolls in 1987, and they comprised less than 6 percent of the total
number of disabled children. Twenty years later, there were 561,569
disabled mentally ill children on the SSI rolls, and they comprised
50 percent of the total. This epidemic is even hitting preschool
children. The prescribing of psychotropic drugs to two-year-olds
and three-year-olds began to become more commonplace about a
decade ago, and sure enough, the number of severely mentally ill
children under six years of age receiving SSI has tripled since then,
rising from 22,453 in 2000 to 65,928 in 2007. 98
Moreover, the SSI numbers only begin to hint at the scope of the
harm being done. Everywhere there is evidence of a worsening of
the mental health of children and teenagers. From 1995 to 1999,
psychiatric-related emergency room visits by children increased 59
percent. 99 The deteriorating mental health of the nation's children,
declared U.S. surgeon general David Satcher in 2001, constituted "a
health crisis." 100 Next, colleges were suddenly wondering why so
many of their students were suffering manic episodes or behaving in
disturbed ways; a 2007 survey discovered that one in six college stu¬
dents had deliberately "cut or burned self” in the prior year. 101 All
246
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
The Epidemic Hits America's Children
SSI Recipients Under 18 Years Old Disabled by Mental Illness, 1 987-2007
Prior to 1992, the government's SSI reports did not breakdown children recipients into subgroups
by age. Source: Social Security Administration reports, 1987-2007.
of this led the U.S. Government Accountability Office to investigate
what was going on, and it reported in 2008 that one in every fifteen
young adults, eighteen to twenty-six years old, is now "seriously
mentally ill." There are 680,000 in that age group with bipolar dis¬
order and another 800,000 ill with major depression, and, the GAO
noted, this was in fact an undercount of the problem, as it didn't
include young adults who were homeless, incarcerated, or institu¬
tionalized. All of these youth are "functionally impaired" to some
degree, the GAO said. 102
That is where we stand as a nation today. Twenty years ago, our
society began regularly prescribing psychiatric drugs to children
and adolescents, and now one out of every fifteen Americans enters
adulthood with a "serious mental illness." That is proof of the most
tragic sort that our drug-based paradigm of care is doing a great
deal more harm than good. The medicating of children and youth
became commonplace only a short time ago, and already it has put
millions onto a path of lifelong illness.
12
Suffer the Children
"You wonder all the time:
Are you helping or harming your child?
— jasmine's mom ( 2009 )
There are an endless number of stories of medicated children that
can be told, and as I worked on this book, each visit to a place
where such children can be found —to a family's home or to a foster
care provider or to a psychiatric hospital —offered at least a brief
glimpse of this new society we have created in the past thirty years.
There are, of course, many parents who will tell of how their chil¬
dren have been helped by psychiatric drugs, and given the spectrum
of outcomes that occur with this paradigm of care, that is undoubt¬
edly true (at least over the short term). But this book is about the
epidemic of disabling mental illness that has erupted in our country,
and so the stories that follow tell, at best, of ambivalent long-term
outcomes, and of how diagnosis and treatment during childhood
may lead to a life of disability.
248
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Lost in Seattle
I met the young woman I'll call Jasmine for only a short time, and
even that brief encounter left her visibly agitated.* Born in 1988,
Jasmine resides today in a somewhat dilapidated group home for
the severely mentally ill in a suburb of Seattle, and even as her
mother and I approached the facility, we could see Jasmine through
a window, pacing back and forth. Once we stepped inside, Jasmine
took once glance at me and quickly retreated, huddling next to the
wall, very much liked a frightened creature of the wild. She wore
jeans and a light blue jacket, and she also kept her distance from her
mother —Jasmine won't let anyone hug her now. We drove in two
cars to a nearby Dairy Queen, as Jasmine would not have been will¬
ing to go if I had been in the car with her, and after we got there,
Jasmine stayed in the backseat, staring straight ahead and rocking
back and forth. "If she ever speaks again," her mother says quietly,
"she will have quite the story to tell."
Photos of Jasmine as a young girl are a good place to start her
story. Her mother had shown them to me earlier, and they all told of
a happy childhood. In one, Jasmine is joyfully lined up next to her
two sisters in front of a Disneyland ride; in another, she is showing
off a gap-toothed grin; in a third, she is playfully sticking out her
tongue. "She was very smart and funny, pretty much the light of our
lives," her mother recalls. "She would be outside playing, riding her
bike up and down the street, just like a typical kid. She would even
go around to the neighbors and tell them she would sing 'Row,
Row, Row Your Boat' for fifty cents. She was such a hellion—you
can see in these photos how spunky she was."
All was fine in Jasmine's life until the summer after fifth grade.
Because she still occasionally wet her bed, she was anxious about
going away to camp, and so a doctor prescribed a "bed-wetting"
* Since "Jasmine" could not give consent to having her name used, her mother
and I agreed to keep her identity hidden. I've also kept her mother unnamed
for that same reason.
SUFFER THE CHILDREN
249
pill, which happened to be a tricyclic antidepressant. Very quickly,
Jasmine became agitated and hostile, and one afternoon she told her
mom: "I'm having all these horrible thoughts. I feel like I'm going to
kill people.”
In hindsight, it is easy to see what was happening to Jasmine. Her
extreme agitation was a sign that she was suffering from akathisia,
a side effect of antidepressants closely linked to suicide and vio¬
lence. "But nobody ever asked about whether the drug might have
triggered the homicidal ideation," her mother says. "I didn't learn
that imipramine could do that until years later when I went on the
Internet." Instead, Jasmine was referred to a psychiatrist, who diag¬
nosed her with obsessive-compulsive disorder and bipolar illness.
He put her on a drug cocktail composed of Zoloft, Luvox, and
Zyprexa, and by the time she entered middle school that fall, she
was a changed person.
"It was horrible,” her mother says. "She gained over a hundred
pounds on Zyprexa, and she is petite, five feet, three inches tall.
Kids who knew her from elementary school said, 'What happened
to you?' Boys began calling her 'the beast.' She ended up with no
friends, and she would cry and cry, and ask to eat lunch in the prin¬
cipal's office to stay out of the cafeteria.” Meanwhile, Jasmine’s
rages at home continued, and her psychiatrist upped her dosage of
Zyprexa so high that her eyes would roll up into her head and get
stuck. "It was like she was being tortured. She would lie on her bed
and scream, 'Why is this happening to me?’ "
Eventually, after the Zoloft was finally withdrawn. Jasmine stab¬
ilized fairly well on a combination of Zyprexa and Depakote.
Although she rarely socialized with classmates, she did well academ¬
ically, and during her first years in high school, she regularly earned
A's and kudos for her photography and artwork. She immersed her¬
self in volunteer work, too, helping out at a humane society, a senior
center, and a food bank, her school giving her an "unsung hero”
award for this work. She had come to accept that she was bipolar,
and even made plans to write a book that would help other teenagers
understand it. "She used to tell me, 'Mom, when I graduate from
high school, I am going to stand up and ask, Has anybody ever won¬
dered what happened to me?' She was so brave.”
250
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Toward the end of her junior year, Jasmine read on the Internet
that Zyprexa could cause weight gain, hypoglycemia, and diabetes.
She suffered from the first two of those problems, but when she
asked her psychiatrist about Zyprexa's side effects, he dismissed her
concerns. Enraged, Jasmine "fired” him, and in June of 2005, she
took herself off both medications, stopping them rather abruptly.
Ten days after she took a final dose of Zyprexa, she was on an ex¬
cursion with her mother when she suddenly turned ashen, sweat
beading up on her lip. "This is really bad," she muttered. "Mom,
fight for me.”
Jasmine has been more or less lost to the world ever since. By the
time they arrived at the hospital. Jasmine was screaming and tearing
at her hair. She was deep into a withdrawal psychosis, and doctors
began giving her one powerful drug after another, trying to get it to
abate. "They put her on eleven medications in thirteen days, which
essentially fried her brain,” her mother says. Jasmine began cycling
in and out of hospitals, and every time she was discharged home, it
ended badly. At times, she was so psychotic that she would call the
police to tell them that she was being kidnapped or that men were
building bombs in her front yard. On several occasions, she "es¬
caped” from her house and ran screaming into the streets. Another
time she kicked and punched her mom; afterward, she ripped a soda
can open and slashed at her wrist. "This is the most psychotic per¬
son we have ever seen in the history of this ER," hospital staff told
Jasmine's mom after one such episode.
In late 2006, a doctor put Jasmine on a single antipsychotic,
Clozaril, and that led to a brief respite. Although Jasmine rarely
spoke, she calmed down and entered a school for disabled children.
At night, her mother read to her for hours, seeking to nurture the
spark of sanity she now saw in Jasmine. "I also noticed that if I sang
to her, like to an Alzheimer's patient, she would sing back, commu¬
nicating through singing.” But in early 2007, Jasmine suffered an¬
other severe bout of psychosis, which ended with her screaming in
the middle of a busy road. "There is no hope for her," doctors said,
and soon Jasmine was placed at the residential facility, where today
she passes her days, shying away from contact with other people
and, except for an occasional word now and then, mute.
SUFFER THE CHILDREN
251
"The doctors tell me she was always going to be schizophrenic,”
her mother says. "But no doctor ever asked about this history,
about what she was like before she was put on drugs. And you
know what's so hard to accept? We came in for help that summer
when she was eleven years old for a minor problem that had noth¬
ing to do with psychiatry. In my mind, I can hear her laughing, like
she was back then. But her life has been stolen away. We've lost her,
even though her body remains. I see every minute what I've lost."
Ambivalent in Syracuse
Senior year was a good time for Andrew Stevens. Diagnosed with
ADHD and put on medication when he was in first grade, he'd had
up-and-down times in school until his senior year. But then he took
a course in auto mechanics, and bingo, he excelled in a way he
never had before. "I'm in the zone," he explains. "I enjoy it. It
doesn't feel like school."
On this afternoon, Andrew, who is slight of build and perhaps
five feet, six inches tall, looks very much like the skateboarder he is:
short-cropped hair, black earring, and wearing a T-shirt, shorts, and
tennis shoes splashed with a kaleidoscope of colors. I had met his
mother, Ellen, a year earlier, at a conference in Albany, New York,
and she had expressed a sentiment that, I thought, neatly summar¬
ized the moral aspect of our society's medicating of youth: "Andrew
has been a guinea pig for the medical field," she'd said.
Very early on, she and her husband had realized that Andrew was
different from their other two children. He had speech problems;
his behavior seemed eccentric; he had "rage issues.” In first grade,
he was so wound up he regularly needed to go into the hallway and
bounce on a mini-trampoline in order to refocus. "I remember cry¬
ing when he was diagnosed with ADHD, and it wasn't because my
kid was labeled,” his mother says. "It was, 'Thank God, we know
something real is going on with him and they know how to help
him. It's not our imagination.'
Although she and her husband worried about putting Andrew on
252
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Ritalin, doctors and school authorities led her to believe that she
would be "remiss as a parent" if she didn't give him the medication.
And at first, "it was like a miracle," she says. Andrew's fears abated,
he learned to tie his shoes, and his teachers praised his improved
behavior. But after a few months, the drug no longer seemed to
work so well, and whenever its effects wore off, there would be this
"rebound effect.” Andrew would "behave like a wild man, out of
control.” A doctor increased his dosage, only then it seemed that
Andrew was like a "zombie,” his sense of humor reemerging only
when the drug's effects wore off. Next, Andrew needed to take
clonidine in order to fall asleep at night. The drug treatment didn't
really seem to be helping, and so Ritalin gave way to other stimu¬
lants, including Adderall, Concerta, and dextroamphetamine. "It
was always more drugs," his mother says.
Meanwhile, Andrew's success in the classroom fluctuated accord¬
ing to the talents of his teacher. In fourth and fifth grade, he had
teachers who knew how to work with him, and he did fairly well.
But his sixth-grade teacher was impatient with him, and Andrew's
self-esteem took such a nosedive that his mother homeschooled him
the following year. Andrew's anxieties worsened during this period,
and often he would be "hyperfocused,” worrying all the time that
his mother might die. He also was notably smaller than his peers,
and his parents thought the drugs were probably curbing his
growth. "That has been the most frustrating part. I never know
what is my son and what is the drug," his mother says.
Today, her ambivalence about the medications is such that she
wishes she could turn back the clock and try a different tack. "My
Andrew is not a circle or a square, he is not even a triangle," she ex¬
plains. "He is a rhombus trapezoid, and he will never fit into those
other molds. And I do think that if we had never put him on medi¬
cine, he would have learned many more coping mechanisms, be¬
cause he would have had to. And we should be able to help kids like
Andrew without making them feel so different, without suppressing
their appetite, and without worrying about the long-term effects of
the drugs —all the things I am sitting here worrying about."
When Andrew was younger, he was allowed "medication
breaks" now and then, and when I ask him what that was like, he
SUFFER THE CHILDREN
253
recalls how nice it was to fall asleep without having to take cloni-
dine. Being off meds, he says, "feels less constricted, more free.”
Still, he tells me, he is about to graduate from high school, and he
has ended up at a good place. He has a girlfriend, he enjoys skate¬
boarding and playing the guitar, and thanks to the auto mechanics
class, he now has career plans, as he intends to one day open his
own garage. "It's hard to think back to a time when it could have
been different,” he says, shrugging, thinking about his life on med¬
ications. "I don't think there was a right or wrong choice —this is
just how it's been."
If You're a Ward of the State, You Must Be Bipolar
The medicating of foster children in the United States took off in the
late 1990s, and so I thought, in order to gain a perspective on this
phenomenon, I would visit with Theresa Gately. She and her hus¬
band, Bill, took ninety-six foster children into their Boston home
from 1996 to 2000, and thus she personally witnessed this change
in how our society treats foster kids. The first children that Social
Services sent them weren't medicated, but by the end, "it felt like all
of them were on psych drugs,” she says.
Over the course of several hours, we sat on her front porch,
which looks out over a busy street in a fairly rough part of Boston,
and nearly everyone who walked by waved and affectionately
shouted hello, no matter what their ethnicity. Theresa Gately is a
thin woman with straw blond hair, and she has her own history as
a foster child. Born in 1964, she was sexually abused by her step¬
father, and she turned so defiant as a teenager that she landed in a
Maryland psychiatric hospital. There she was put on Thorazine and
other neuroleptics, and, she said, it wasn't until she started "tongu-
ing" the drugs —pretending to take them while nurses were watch¬
ing and then spitting them out —that her head started to clear.
However, she isn't "anti-medication" at all, and during a difficult
time a few years back, she found an antidepressant and a mood sta¬
bilizer to be extremely helpful, and she remains on those drugs.
254
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
As a foster mother, Gately was required to follow "medical ad¬
vice" and give psychiatric medications to the children who arrived
on them. Most of the children were on cocktails, and it seemed to
her that the drugs were primarily being used to make the children
quieter and easier to manage. "One young girl, Liz, was so heavily
medicated that she couldn't think at all," she recalls. "You would
ask her if she wanted a pork chop and she wouldn't answer.” An¬
other was "almost mute when she came to me. The last thing you
need to do is give somebody who already doesn't talk more drugs."
Theresa ran through the histories of several more of her foster chil¬
dren, concluding that "maybe nine to eleven [of the ninety-six chil¬
dren] needed to have the drugs and were being helped.”
She has kept track of a number of the ninety-six children, and as
could be expected, many have struggled mightily as adults. Had she,
I wondered, noticed a difference in the fate of those who stayed on
the drug cocktails, versus those who stopped taking them?
"When I look back on the kids that stayed on the drugs and those
who got off, it is the ones that are off that are the successes," she
says. "Liz should never have been on the drugs. She got off the drugs
and is doing great. She is a full-time student in nursing school and al¬
most ready to graduate, and is about to get married. The thing is, if
you get off the drugs, you start building these coping mechanisms.
You learn internal controls. You start building these strengths. Most
of these kids have had very bad stuff happen to them. But they are
able to rise above their past once they are off the medications, and
then they can move on. The kids who were drugged and continue to
be drugged never have that opportunity to build coping skills. And
because they never had that opportunity as a teenager, as an adult
they don't know what to do with themselves.”
Hers isn't a scientific study. But her experience does offer a peek
into the toll that the medicating of foster kids is taking. Most of those
who stayed on the drugs, she says, ended up "filing for disability.”
Like Theresa Gately, Sam Clayborn, who is a social worker in New
Rochelle, New York, can tell from personal experience what it is
like to have been a foster kid in the United States. When he was
SUFFER THE CHILDREN
255
born in Harlem in 1965, his mother was unable to take care of him,
and by age six he was living in a residential group home. We met in
his apartment in Croton-on-Hudson, and very quickly he put things
into a historical context. "They weren't so hot on psychiatric diag¬
noses back then," he explains. "They were more into beating your
ass, restraining you, and just throwing you into an empty fucking
room. I'm glad I grew up when it was like that rather than what it is
like today, because if I grew up now, I'd be fucking drugged up. I'd
be doped out and zonked out.”
For the past two decades, he and his partner Eva Dech have
worked as advocates for foster children and poor youth in West¬
chester County. She also had a tough childhood, which included a
stint in a mental hospital where she was forcibly medicated, and
they see a racial aspect to this medicating of foster children. Starting
around 2000, rates of black youth diagnosed with bipolar disorder
soared, and based on hospital discharges, they are now said to suf¬
fer from bipolar disorder at a greater rate than whites. 1 The diagno¬
sis provides a rationale for medicating the kids, and that in turn
puts yet one more burden on them, Clayborn believes.
"The Tuskegee syphilis experiments were nothing compared to
this. That's mild shit compared to what they are doing to black kids
today. The pharmaceutical companies and the government are fuck¬
ing in cahoots, and they are doing a wicked dance with a lot of peo¬
ple's lives. They don't give a fuck about these kids. It's all about
capitalism, and they will sacrifice all the niggers in the hood. We are
damaging these kids for life, and the majority of these kids will
never rebound. These kids will be destroyed and they are going to
make the SSI rolls more overwhelmed."
One of the area youth that Clayborn has mentored is Jonathan
Barrow, who had been splayed out on the living room floor during
our conversation, half sleeping and half listening. Born in 1985 in
Harlem to a mother on crack, Jonathan bounced around as a child,
eventually ending up at his grandfather's home in White Plains. At
age seven, he was diagnosed with ADHD and put on Ritalin. In ju¬
nior high, he started becoming rebellious and got into a few fights,
and that led to a diagnosis of bipolar disorder and a prescription for
Depakote and Risperdal. Up until that time, Jonathan had been an
256
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
active adolescent who spent most of his free time on the basketball
court, but now he began spending most of his time "in his room iso¬
lated," Clayborn says. He went onto the SSI disability rolls before
he turned eighteen, apparently "severely impaired” by this bipolar
illness, and he remains on SSI today. "I'm doped up," Jonathan ex¬
plains, still somewhat heavy-lidded from his afternoon nap. "I don't
like it. It makes me sleepy and feel like a dope fiend.”
At this, Clayborn rose from his chair, more agitated than ever.
"This is happening to a lot of the brothers today, and once they are
on the medication, it takes them away from themselves. They lose
all the willpower to struggle, to change, to make something out of
themselves and have success. They succumb to the chemical hand¬
cuffs of the motherfucking medications. It's medical bondage is
what it is."
Not long after that interview, I attended a meeting of the Statewide
Youth Advisory Council at Westborough State Hospital in Massa¬
chusetts. The council is composed of young adults who entered the
mental health system before they were eighteen, and it provides ad¬
vice to the Massachusetts Department of Mental Health on what it
can do to help teenagers with psychiatric problems thrive as adults.
In 2008, the coordinator of the council was Mathew McWade, who
was first diagnosed when he was in the seventh grade, and it was he
who made my visit possible.
At the meeting, I went around the table and asked everyone how
they had gotten into the system. I thought I might hear stories of
kids who were first put on a stimulant or an antidepressant and then
moved on to a bipolar diagnosis, and while there was some of that,
several men in this racially mixed group told of yet another societal
route to psychiatric disability.
When Cal Jones* was sixteen years old, he had gotten into a
violent argument that ended with his being treated in the emergency
room at Children's Hospital in Boston. There he told ER staff that
* Cal Jones is a pseudonym. Hospital staff asked that I not reveal the names of
the hospitalized patients.
SUFFER THE CHILDREN
257
he "wanted to kill the other kid," a sentiment that earned him a trip
to a psychiatric facility, where he was diagnosed with bipolar ill¬
ness. "They didn't run any tests," he says. "They just asked me a
bunch of questions and started me on a bunch of medicines.” Since
then, he has been hospitalized twenty-five times. He doesn't like
antipsychotics, and so he regularly stops taking them when he is dis¬
charged, preferring to smoke marijuana instead, and inevitably that
leads to trouble. "I get arrested and get sent back to the (psych) hos¬
pital, and I'm like okay, it's just a business. The more patients they
have, the more the doctors make. But I hate it. I can't stand it. I feel
like a slave in a Nazi camp.”
At least three others at the meeting told similar stories. One
young man said that shortly after he graduated from high school in
2002 , he got upset over a family matter and smashed the windows
of his car. "I was having a bad time. They wanted to label me as
mentally ill. I don't know if I am.” Another explained that six
months earlier, after he had committed a minor criminal act, a judge
had given him the choice of going to prison or to Westborough State
Hospital. "It's safer in here than in prison,” he says, explaining his
choice. A third member of the council said that he had been diag¬
nosed with bipolar illness at age thirteen after "I killed somebody.”
Their stories bore witness to another pathway into the mental
health system for poor youth. Delinquency and crime can get them
diagnosed, medicated, and routed into a mental institution. While
many of the young men on the council were on heavy-duty cock¬
tails, moving about and speaking in a sluggish manner, the one who
had told of having killed somebody was now living in the commu¬
nity and not taking any medications. "If the state really wants to
help us, it should put money into a jobs program,” he says.
Back to Syracuse
As a last stop, I returned to visit the two Syracuse families —Jason
and Kelley Smith and Sean and Gwen Oates —that I had met in the
spring of 2008. Families, friends, therapists, and doctors had given
258
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
the two families conflicting advice about whether they should medi¬
cate their child, and faced with such bewildering advice, the two
families had come to opposite decisions.
Jessica
I knew from an earlier telephone conversation that Jessica Smith
had been doing well, and when I arrived at their home, she bounded
to the door to welcome me, much as she had a year earlier. When
she was diagnosed with bipolar disorder at age four, her parents had
rejected the recommendations of staff at the State University of
New York Health Sciences Center that she be put on a cocktail of
three drugs that included an antipsychotic. Today, they have an
eight-year-old girl reminiscent of Maurice Sendak's endearing
"Really Rosie” character on their hands. Jessica, who is very much
the extroverted child, had recently starred in a school musical. "She
just loves it,” her father says, and he pointed to her behavior on
opening night as evidence of how much better she had become at
controlling her emotions. "She was playing a brainiac, and another
girl in the show stole her chair, which she wasn't supposed to do.
We could see that Jessica was upset. But then she let it pass. It
showed that she is getting better at de-escalating situations.”
Although Jessica no longer sees a therapist, "there are still strug¬
gles," her mother says. "She still has a hard time with groups, with
playing with more than one kid at a time. And she will still lash out
if someone hurts her feelings. She wants to be the boss, and she can
be loud and boisterous. But the kicking and biting is gone.”
Adds her father: "She has a big personality, but that is like others
in my family. I was the same way. I was very loud. I wouldn't sit
still. And I turned out all right."
Nathan
Nathan Oates had gone through a more topsy-turvy twelve months.
I had called his mother several times during the year, and in the
summer of 2008, Nathan —who had been diagnosed with ADHD at
age four and subsequently with bipolar illness —had been doing
SUFFER THE CHILDREN
259
well. He took Concerta for the ADHD and Risperdal for the bipolar
disorder, and that summer he discovered that he "loves track,” his
mother told me. "They are teaching him how to do hurdles and the
long jump.” Even more important, his mood swings had become
less severe, his hostility toward his sister had lessened, and he was
sleeping better, too. "He said he wants to start being more responsi¬
ble,” his mother said. "He gets up in the morning and makes his
bed, and now he is at a point he will take a shower by himself. He is
starting to do things without my hounding him. It seems he is kind
of maturing on his own."
This was a heartening report, but that relatively peaceful time
ended when Nathan returned to school in the fall. He became quite
anxious and moody, and started resisting going to school. The
physician's assistant overseeing his care upped his Risperdal, hoping
that would quiet his anxiety. "They are trying to figure out whether
his anxiety is bipolar related or a separate disorder,” his mother ex¬
plained, in a phone interview in early 2009. "The ADHD is fine and
under control. If this doesn't work, they will give him an anti¬
anxiety medication. They want to make sure that he doesn't get too
lethargic under the higher dose of Risperdal.”
When I returned to Syracuse in the spring, Nathan's parents were
close to despair over the difficulties that he was experiencing.
Nathan's anxiety hadn't abated, and to make matters worse, he had
lost control of his bladder. A few days earlier, his mother had wit¬
nessed in heartbreaking fashion how this was affecting her son. "I
went to pick him up in school, and he was sitting in the middle of
the room at his desk alone," she says. "It was almost like he was in¬
visible to everybody else. The teachers swear he has friends but he
never talks about anybody. There is only one classmate who doesn't
pick on him." This isolation, his mother adds, followed Nathan
into the home. "He stays in his room all the time.”
Nathan's father remained hopeful that another "medication
adjustment” would help his son. But beyond that, both parents con¬
fessed that they were at a loss about what to do. The psychologist
who counseled Nathan was running out of ideas; the school wasn't
doing much to alleviate Nathan's severe anxiety; and their families
and friends didn't appreciate how difficult this all was. "I feel so
26 o
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
alone in this," his mother says. "It stinks. It's wearing. It's exhaust¬
ing. I cry for him. I just don't know what to do anymore. I don't
know how to help him.”
Before I left, Nathan came down from his room, and he shyly
showed me a few of his favorite possessions, including a Star Wars
helmet. He told me that Zachariah was his best friend (the one
classmate who didn't tease him), and then he taught me how to fold
a piece of paper into an airplane, which he sent flying around the
room. "I like to make movies" with a video recorder, he says, and
eventually I quizzed him on a couple of subjects he loves. "The
Titanic sank in 1912,” he informs me, and after that he proudly
identified various bones in the human body —he is fascinated with
drawings of skeletons. "His teachers all love him,” his mother says,
and at that moment, it was very easy to see why.
part four
Explication of a Delusion
13
The Rise of an Ideology
"It was not surprising that medical students accepted
the dogma of biomedical reductionism in psychiatry
uncritically; they had no time to read and analyze the
original literature. What took me a while to understand,
as I moved through my residency, was that psychiatrists
rarely do the critical reading either."
-COLIN ROSS, CLINICAL ASSOCIATE PROFESSOR OF
PSYCHIATRY AT SOUTHWEST MEDICAL CENTER IN
DALLAS, TEXAS (1 995)'
We have investigated the epidemic of mental illness that has erupted
in the United States during the past fifty years in a step-by-step fash¬
ion, and having reviewed the outcomes literature for each of the
major disorders, there is an obvious next question to address. Why
does our society believe that a "psychopharmacological revolution"
has taken place during the past fifty years, when the scientific litera¬
ture so clearly shows that the revolution failed to materialize? Or, to
put it another way, what is the source of our remarkable societal
delusion?
To answer that, we need to trace the rise of "biological psychia¬
try" and then look at the stories that psychiatry —once it embraced
that belief system —came to tell.
Psychiatry's Season of Discontent
During the heady days of the 1950s, when it seemed that a new
breakthrough drug was being discovered every year, psychiatry had
reason to be optimistic about its future. It now had magic pills like
264
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
the rest of medicine, and once NIMH researchers and others ad¬
vanced the chemical imbalance theory of mental disorders, it
seemed that these pills might indeed be antidotes to physical dis¬
eases. "American psychiatry," exclaimed former NIMH director
Gerald Klerman, "accepted psychopharmacology as its domain." 2
But two decades later, those heady days were long gone, and psychi¬
atry was mired in a deep crisis, beleaguered on so many fronts that
it worried about its survival. There was a sense, said American Psy¬
chiatric Association (APA) director Melvin Sabshin in 1980, that
the "profession is under severe siege and is cut off from allies." 3
The first problem that had arisen for psychiatry was an intellec¬
tual challenge to its legitimacy, an attack launched in 1961 by
Thomas Szasz, a psychiatrist at the State University of New York in
Syracuse. In his book The Myth of Mental Illness, he argued that
psychiatric disorders weren't medical in kind, but rather labels ap¬
plied to people who struggled with "problems in living” or simply
behaved in socially deviant ways. Psychiatrists, he said, had more in
common with ministers and police than they did with physicians.
Szasz's criticism rattled the field, since even mainstream publica¬
tions like the Atlantic and Science found his argument to be both
cogent and important, the latter concluding that his treatise was
"enormously courageous and highly informative . . . bold and often
brilliant." 4 As Szasz later told the New York Times, "In smoke-filled
rooms, time and time again. I've heard the view that Szasz has killed
psychiatry. I hope so." 5
His book helped launch an "antipsychiatry" movement, and
other academics in the United States and Europe —Michel Foucault,
R. D. Laing, David Cooper, and Erving Goffman, just to name a
few—joined the fray. All questioned the "medical model” of mental
disorders and suggested that madness could be a "sane” reaction to
an oppressive society. Mental hospitals might better be described as
facilities for social control, rather than for healing, a viewpoint
crystallized and popularized in One Flew Over the Cuckoo's Nest,
which swept the Oscars for 1975. Nurse Ratched was the malevo¬
lent cop in that movie, which ended with Randle McMurphy
(played by Jack Nicholson) being lobotomized for failing to stay in
line.
THE RISE OF AN IDEOLOGY
265
The second problem that psychiatry faced was a growing compe¬
tition for patients. In the 1960s and 1970s, a therapy industry
blossomed in the United States. Thousands of psychologists and
counselors began offering services to the "neurotic" patients that
psychiatry had laid claim to ever since Freud had brought his couch
to America. By 1975, the nonphysician therapists outnumbered the
shrinks in the United States, and with benzodiazepines falling out of
favor, the neurotic patients who had been content to pop "happy
pills" in the 1960s were embracing primal scream therapy, Esalen
retreats, and any number of other "alternative” therapies said to
help heal the wounded soul. Partly as a result of this competition,
the median earnings of a U.S. psychiatrist in the late 1970s were
only $70,600, and while this was a good wage at the time, it still
put psychiatry near the bottom of the medical profession. "Non¬
psychiatric mental health professionals are laying claim to some, or
even all, of psychiatry's task domains,” wrote Tufts University psy¬
chiatrist David Adler. There was reason, he said, to worry about the
"death of psychiatry." 6
Internal divisions also ran deep. Although the field had turned
toward biological psychiatry after the arrival of Thorazine, with
most psychiatrists eager to speak well of the drugs, the Freudians
who dominated many medical schools in the 1950s had never com¬
pletely climbed on that bandwagon. While they found some use for
the drugs, they still conceived of most disorders as psychological in
kind. As such, during the 1970s, there was a deep philosophical
split between the Freudians and those who embraced a "medical
model” of psychiatric disorders. In addition, there was a third fac¬
tion in the field, composed of "social psychiatrists." This group
thought that psychosis and emotional distress often arose from an
individual's conflict with his or her environment. If that was so, al¬
tering that environment or creating a supportive new one —as Loren
Mosher had done with his Soteria Project —would be a good way to
help a person heal. Like the Freudians, the social psychiatrists did
not see drugs as the centerpiece of care, but rather as agents that
were sometimes helpful and sometimes not. With these three
approaches in conflict, the field was suffering from an "identity cri¬
sis," Sabshin said. 7
266
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
By the end of the 1970s, the leaders of the APA regularly spoke of
how their field was in a fight for "survival.” In the 1950s, psychia¬
try had become the fastest growing specialty in medicine, but during
the 1970s, the percentage of medical school graduates choosing to
go into it dropped from 11 percent to less than 4 percent. This lack
of interest in the field, the New York Times reported in an article
titled "Psychiatry's Anxious Years," was "seen as a particularly
painful indictment." 8
Avoiding the Obvious
Such was psychiatry's self-assessment in the 1970s. It looked into
the mirror and saw the field under attack by an "antipsychiatry"
movement, threatened economically by nonphysician therapists,
and split by internal disagreements. But, in fact, it was turning a
blind eye to the root problem, which was that its medications were
failing in the marketplace. This was what had allowed the crisis to
take hold and spread.
If the first generation of psychotropics had truly worked, the
public would have been pounding on psychiatrists' doors seeking
prescriptions for these medicines. Szasz's argument that mental ill¬
ness was a "myth" might have been seen by some as intellectually
interesting, worthy of debate in academic circles, but it wouldn't
have curtailed the public's appetite for drugs that made them feel
and function better. Similarly, psychiatry could have brushed off the
competition from psychologists and counselors as a harmless nui¬
sance. Depressed and anxious people might have indulged in
screaming therapies and mud baths, and sought out talk therapy
from psychologists, but the prescription bottles would have re¬
mained in their medicine cabinets. Nor would the internal divisions
have persisted. If the pills had proved to provide long-term relief,
then all of psychiatry would have embraced the medical model, for
the other proffered forms of care —psychoanalysis and nurturing
environments —would have been perceived as too labor-intensive
THE RISE OF AN IDEOLOGY
267
and unnecessary. Psychiatry fell into a crisis during the 1970s be¬
cause the "miracle pill” aura around its drugs had disappeared.
From the moment that Thorazine and the neuroleptics were in¬
troduced into asylum medicine, many hospitalized patients had
found them objectionable, so much so that many "tongued” the
pills. This practice was so pervasive that Smith, Kline and French, in
the early 1960s, developed a liquid Thorazine, which the patients
could be made to swallow. Other manufacturers developed in¬
jectable forms of their neuroleptics so that hospitalized patients
could be forcibly medicated. "Warning!" an ad for liquid Thorazine
screamed. "Mental Patients Are Notorious DRUG EVADERS." 9 In
the early 1970s, patients who had experienced such forced treat¬
ment began forming groups with names such as the "Insane Libera¬
tion Front" and the "Network Against Psychiatric Assault." At
their rallies, many carried signs that read hugs, not drugs!
One Flew Over the Cuckoo's Nest helped legitimatize that
protest in the public's mind, and that movie appeared shortly after
psychiatry suffered the embarrassment of news reports that the
Soviet Union was using neuroleptics to torture dissidents. These
drugs apparently inflicted such physical pain that quite sane people
would recant their criticisms of a Communist government rather
than endure repeated doses of Haldol. Dissident writings told of
psychiatric drugs that turned people into "vegetables," the New
York Times concluding that this practice could be seen as "spiritual
murder." 10 Then, in 1975, when Indiana senator Birch Bayh
launched an investigation of the use of neuroleptics in juvenile insti¬
tutions, ex-mental patients hijacked the public hearing to testify
that the drugs caused "excruciating pain" and had turned them into
emotional "zombies." Antipsychotics, said one ex-patient, "are used
not to heal or help, but to torture and control. It is that simple.” 11
These drugs were no longer being presented to the public as
agents that made a raving madman "sit up and talk sense," as Time
had reported in 1954, and even as this new view of antipsychotics
was sinking into the public mind, the benzodiazepines fell into dis¬
repute. The federal government classified them as schedule IV
drugs, and soon Edward Kennedy was announcing that benzos
268
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
had "produced a nightmare of dependence and addiction." 12 Anti-
psychotics and the benzodiazepines were the two classes of drugs
that had launched the psychopharmacology revolution, and with
both now seen by the public in a negative light, sales of psychiatric
drugs plunged in the 1970s, from 223 million drugstore prescrip¬
tions in 1973 to 153 million in 1980. 13 In its article on psychiatry's
"anxious years," the New York Titties explained that a primary rea¬
son that medical school graduates were avoiding the field was
because its treatments were perceived to be "low in efficacy."
This was a topic that psychiatry did not like to talk about or ac¬
knowledge. Yet, at the same time, everyone understood what gave
psychiatrists a competitive advantage in the therapy marketplace.
New Jersey psychiatrist Arthur Piatt was at a professional meeting
in the late 1970s when a keynote speaker laid it out for them: "He
said, 'What is going to save us is that we're physicians,' " Piatt re¬
calls. 14 They could write prescriptions and the psychologists and
social workers couldn't, and that was an economic landscape that
presented the field with an obvious solution. If the image of psy¬
chotropic drugs could be rehabilitated, psychiatry would thrive.
Putting on the White Coat
The process that led to the rehabilitation of psychiatric drugs in the
public's mind got under way in the 1970s. Threatened by Szasz's
criticism that psychiatrists did not really function as "doctors," the
APA argued that psychiatrists needed to more explicitly embrace
this role. "A vigorous effort to remedicalize psychiatry should be
strongly supported,” said the APA's Sabshin in 1977. 15 Numerous
articles appeared in the American Journal of Psychiatry and other
journals explaining what this meant. "The medical model,” wrote
University of Kentucky psychiatrist Arnold Ludwig, is based on the
"premise that the primary identity of the psychiatrist is as a physi¬
cian." 16 Mental disorders, said Paul Blaney, from the University of
Texas, were to be seen as "organic diseases." 17 The psychiatrist's
focus should be on making the proper diagnosis, which arose from
THE RISE OF AN IDEOLOGY
269
a cataloguing of the "symptoms and signs of illness," said Samuel
Guze, from Washington University. It was only psychiatrists, he
added, that had the "medical training necessary for the optimal ap¬
plication of the most effective treatments available today for psychi¬
atric patients: psychoactive drugs and ECT [electroshock]." 18
Theirs was a model of care straight out of internal medicine. The
doctor in that setting took a patient's temperature, or tested blood
glucose levels, or did some other diagnostic test, and then once the
illness was identified, prescribed the appropriate drug. "Remedical-
ization" of psychiatry meant that the Freudian couch was to be trot¬
ted off to the Dumpster, and once that happened, psychiatry could
expect to see its public image restored. "The medical model is most
strongly linked in the popular mind to scientific truth,” explained
Tufts University psychiatrist David Adler. 19
In 1974, the APA picked Robert Spitzer from Columbia Univer¬
sity to head up the task force that would, through a revision of the
APA’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, prompt psychiatrists to
treat patients in this way. DSM-II, which had been published in
1967, reflected Freudian notions of "neurosis," and Spitzer and
others argued that such diagnostic categories were notoriously "un¬
reliable.” He was joined by four other biologically oriented psychi¬
atrists on the task force, including Samuel Guze at Washington
University. DSM-III, Spitzer promised, would serve as "a defense of
the medical model as applied to psychiatric problems."” The man¬
ual, said APA president Jack Weinberg in 1977, would "clarify to
anyone who may be in doubt that we regard psychiatry as a spe¬
cialty of medicine." 21
Three years later, Spitzer and his colleagues published their hand¬
iwork. DSM-III identified 265 disorders, all of which were said to
be distinct in kind. More than one hundred psychiatrists had con¬
tributed to the five-hundred-page tome, authorship that indicated it
represented the collective wisdom of American psychiatry. To make
a DSM-III diagnosis, a psychiatrist would determine if a patient had
the requisite number of symptoms said to be characteristic of the
disease. For instance, there were nine symptoms common to "major
depressive episode,” and if five were present, then a diagnosis of this
illness could be made. The new manual, Spitzer boasted, had been
270
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
"field tested," and those trials had proven that clinicians in different
facilities, when faced with the same patient, were likely to arrive at
the same diagnosis, proof that diagnosis would no longer be as sub¬
jective as before. "These [reliability] results were so much better
than we had expected” they would be, he said. 22
Psychiatry now had its medical-model "bible," and the APA and
others in the field rushed to extol it. DSM-III is an "amazing docu¬
ment ... a brilliant tour de force," Sabshin said. 23 "The develop¬
ment of DSM-III,” said Gerald Klerman, "represents a fateful point
in the history of the American psychiatric profession . . . [and] its
use represents a reaffirmation on the part of American psychiatry to
its medical identity and its commitment to scientific medicine." 24
Thanks to DSM-III, wrote Columbia University psychiatrist Jerrold
Maxmen, "the ascendance of scientific psychiatry became offi¬
cial . . . the old [psychoanalytical] psychiatry derives from theory,
the new psychiatry from fact." 25
But as critics at the time noted, it was difficult to understand why
this manual should be regarded as a great scientific achievement.
No scientific discoveries had led to this reconfiguring of psychiatric
diagnoses. The biology of mental disorders remained unknown, and
the authors of DSM-III even confessed that this was so. Most of the
diagnoses, they said, "have not yet been fully validated by data
about such important correlates as clinical course, outcome, family
history, and treatment response." 26 It was also evident that the
boundary lines between disease and no disease had been arbitrarily
drawn. Why did it require the presence of five of nine symptoms
said to be characteristic of depression for a diagnosis of the illness
to be made? Why not six such symptoms? Or four? DSM-III, wrote
Theodore Blau, president of the American Psychological Associa¬
tion, was more of "a political position paper for the American
Psychiatric Association than a scientifically-based classification
system." 22
None of that mattered, however. With the publication of DSM-
III, psychiatry had publicly donned a white coat. The Freudians had
been vanquished, the concept of neurosis basically tossed into the
trash bin, and everyone in the profession was now expected to em¬
brace the medical model. "It is time to state forcefully that the
THE RISE OF AN IDEOLOGY
271
identity crisis is over," Sabshin said. 28 Indeed, the American Journal
of Psychiatry urged its members to "speak with a united voice, not
only to secure support, but to buttress [psychiatry's] position
against the numerous other mental health professionals seeking pa¬
tients and prestige." 29 The medical model and DSM-III, observed
University of Tennessee psychiatrist Ben Bursten in 1981, had been
used to "rally the troops ... to thwart the attackers [and] to rout
the enemy within." 30
Indeed, it wasn't only the Freudians who had been vanquished.
Loren Mosher and his band of social psychiatrists also had been
roundly defeated and sent packing.
When Mosher started his Soteria Project in 1971, everyone un¬
derstood that it threatened the "medical model” theory of psychi¬
atric disorders. Newly diagnosed schizophrenia patients were being
treated in an ordinary home, staffed by nonprofessionals, without
drugs. Their outcomes were to be compared with patients treated
with drugs in a hospital setting. If the Soteria patients fared better,
what would that say about psychiatry and its therapies? From the
minute that Mosher proposed it, the leaders of American psychiatry
had tried to make sure it would fail. Although Mosher headed up
the Center for Schizophrenia Studies at the NIMH, he'd still needed
to obtain funding for Soteria from the grants committee that over¬
saw NIMH's extramural research program, which was composed of
psychiatrists from leading medical schools, and that committee
slashed his initial request of $700,000 for five years to $150,000 for
two years. This ensured that the project would struggle with fin¬
ances from the outset, and then, in the mid-1970s, when Mosher
began reporting good results for his Soteria patients, the committee
struck back. The study had "serious flaws” in its design, it said.
Evidence that Soteria patients had superior outcomes was "not
compelling." 31 Mosher must be biased, the academic psychiatrists
concluded, and they demanded that Mosher be removed as the pri¬
mary investigator. "The message was clear,” Mosher said, in an in¬
terview twenty-five years later. "If we were getting outcomes this
good, then I must not be an honest scientist." 32 Soon after that, the
grants committee shut off funding for the experiment altogether,
and Mosher was pushed from his job at the NIMH, even though the
272
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
committee had grudgingly concluded, in its final review of the proj¬
ect, that "this project has probably demonstrated that a flexible, com¬
munity based, non-drug residential psychosocial program manned
by non-professional staff can do as well as a more conventional
community mental health program.”
The NIMH never funded an experiment of this type again.
Furthermore, Mosher's ouster provided everyone in the field with a
clear message: Those who did not get behind the biomedical model
would not have much of a future.
Psychiatry's Mad Men
Once DSM-III was published, the APA set out to market its "med¬
ical model" to the public. Although professional medical organiza¬
tions have always sought to advance the economic interests of their
members, this was the first time that a professional organization so
thoroughly adopted the marketing practices familiar to any com¬
mercial trade association. In 1981, the APA established a "division
of publications and marketing” to "deepen the medical identifica¬
tion of psychiatrists," and in very short order, the APA transformed
itself into a very effective marketing machine. 3 ' "It is the task of the
APA to protect the earning power of psychiatrists," said APA vice
president Paul Fink in 1986. 34
As a first step, the APA established its own press in 1981, which
was expected to bring "psychiatry's best talent and current knowl¬
edge before the reading public." 35 The press was soon publishing
more than thirty books a year, with Sabshin happily noting in 1983
that the books "will provide much positive public education about
the profession." 36 The APA also set up committees to review the
textbooks it published, intent on making sure that authors stayed
on message. Indeed, in 1986, as it readied publication of Treatment
of Psychiatric Disorders, the APA's Roger Peele —one of the or¬
ganization's elected officials —worried anew about this concern.
"How do we organize 32,000 members for advocacy?" he asked.
"Who should be allowed to speak to the issue of the treatment of
THE RISE OF AN IDEOLOGY
273
psychiatric illness? Only researchers? Only the academic elite? . . .
Only members appointed by APA presidents?" 37
Very early on, the APA realized that it would be valuable to de¬
velop a nationwide roster of "experts" that could promote the
medical-model story to the media. It established a "public affairs in¬
stitute” to oversee this effort, which involved training members "in
techniques for dealing with radio and television." In 1985 alone, the
APA ran nine "How to Survive a Television Interview” workshops. 3 '
Meanwhile, every district branch in the country identified "public
affairs representatives" who could be called on to speak to the
press. "We now have an experienced network of trained lead¬
ers who can effectively cope with all varieties of media,” Sabshin
said. 39
Much like any commercial organization selling a product, the
APA regularly courted the press and exulted when it received posi¬
tive coverage. In December 1980, it held a daylong media confer¬
ence on "new advances in psychiatry" that "was attended by
representatives of some of the nation's most prestigious and widely
circulated newspapers," Sabshin crowed. 40 Next, it placed "public
service spots" on television to tell its story, an effort that included
sponsoring a two-hour program on cable television titled Your
Mental Health. It also developed "fact sheets” for distribution to
the media that told of the prevalence of mental disorders and the
effectiveness of psychiatric drugs. Harvey Rubin, chair of the APA's
public affairs committee, taped a popular radio program that car¬
ried the medical-model message to listeners around the country. 41
The APA had launched an all-out media blitz —it handed out
awards to journalists whose stories it liked —and every year Sabshin
detailed the good publicity this effort was generating. In 1983, he
noted that "with the help and urging of the Division of Public
Affairs, U.S. News and World Report published a major cover story
on depression, which included substantial quotes from prominent
psychiatrists." 42 Two years later, Sabshin announced that "APA
spokespersons were placed on the Phil Donahue program, Nightline
and other network programs." That same year, it "helped develop a
Reader's Digest book chapter on mental health." 43
All of this paid big dividends. Newspaper and magazine head-
274
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
lines now regularly told of a "revolution" under way in psychiatry.
Readers of the New York Times learned that "human depression is
linked to genes" and that scientists were uncovering the "biology of
fear and anxiety." Researchers, the paper reported, had discovered
"a chemical key to depression.Societal belief in biological psy¬
chiatry was clearly taking hold, just as the APA hoped, and in 1984,
Jon Franklin of the Baltimore Evening Sun wrote a seven-part series
titled "The Mind-Fixers" on the astonishing advances that were
being made in the field. 45 Fie put this revolution into a historical
context:
Since the days of Sigmund Freud the practice of psychiatry
has been more art than science. Surrounded by an aura of
witchcraft, proceeding on impression and hunch, often inef¬
fective, it was the bumbling and sometimes humorous
stepchild of modern science. But for a decade and more, re¬
search psychiatrists have been working quietly in laborato¬
ries, dissecting the brains of mice and men and teasing out the
chemical formulas that unlock the secrets of the mind. Now,
in the 1980s, their work is paying off. They are rapidly iden¬
tifying the interlocking molecules that produce human
thought and emotion. ... As a result, psychiatry today stands
on the threshold of becoming an exact science, as precise and
quantifiable as molecular genetics. Ahead lies an era of psy¬
chic engineering, and the development of specialized drugs
and therapies to heal sick minds.
Franklin, who interviewed more than fifty leading psychiatrists
for his series, called this new science "molecular psychiatry," which
was "capable of curing the mental diseases that afflict perhaps 20
percent of the population." Fie was awarded the Pulitzer Prize for
expository journalism for this work.
Books written by psychiatrists for the lay press at this time told a
similar story. In The Good News About Depression, Yale University
psychiatrist Mark Gold informed readers that "we who work in this
new field call our science biopsychiatry, the new medicine of the
mind. ... It returns psychiatry to the medical model, incorporating
THE RISE OF AN IDEOLOGY
275
all the latest advances in scientific research, and for the first time in
history, providing a systematic method of diagnosis, treatment, cure
and even prevention of mental suffering.” In the past few years,
Gold added, psychiatry had conducted "some of the most incredible
medical research ever done. . . . We have probed the frontiers of
science and human understanding wherein lie the ultimate compre¬
hension and cure of all mental illnesses.” 46
If there was one book that cemented this belief in the public's
mind, it was The Broken Brain. Published in 1984 and written by
Nancy Andreasen, future editor of the American journal of Psychi¬
atry, it was touted as "the first comprehensive account of the bio¬
medical revolution in the diagnosis and treatment of mental
illness.” In it, Andreasen concisely set forth the tenets of biological
psychiatry: "The major psychiatric illnesses are diseases. They
should be considered medical illnesses just as diabetes, heart dis¬
ease, and cancer are. The emphasis in this model is on carefully di¬
agnosing each specific illness from which the patient suffers, just as
an internist or neurologist would." 47
The broken brain— hers was a book with a brilliant title, one that
conveyed a bottom-line message that the public could easily grasp
and remember. However, what most readers failed to notice was
that Andreasen, in several places in her book, confessed that re¬
searchers had not yet actually found that people diagnosed with
psychiatric disorders have broken brains. Researchers had new
tools for investigating brain function, and they hoped this knowl¬
edge would come. "Nevertheless, the spirit of a revolution —the
sense that we are going to change things dramatically, even if the
process requires a number of years —is very much present," An¬
dreasen explained. 46
Twenty-five years later, that breakthrough moment still lies in the
future. The biological underpinnings of schizophrenia, depression,
and bipolar disorder remain unknown. But the public has long since
been convinced otherwise, and we can see now the marketing
process that got this delusion under way. At the start of the 1980s,
psychiatry was worried about its future. Sales of psychiatric drugs
had notably declined in the past seven years, and few medical
school graduates wanted to go into the field. In response, the APA
276
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
mounted a sophisticated marketing campaign to sell its medical
model to the public, and a few years later the public could only gasp
in awe at the apparent advances that were being made. A revolution
was under way, psychiatrists were now "mind-fixers,” and as a
Johns Hopkins "brain chemist,” Michael Kuhar, told Jon Franklin,
this "explosion of new knowledge” was going to lead to new drugs
and broad changes in society that would be "fantastic!" 49
Four-Part Harmony
Psychiatrists were not the only ones in American society who were
eager to tell of a biomedical revolution in psychiatry. During the
1980s, a powerful coalition of voices came together to tell this story,
and this was a group with financial clout, intellectual prestige, and
moral authority. Together they enjoyed all the resources and social
status necessary to convince the public of almost anything, and this
storytelling coalition has stayed intact ever since.
As we saw earlier, the financial interests of pharmaceutical
companies and physicians became closely aligned in 1951, when
Congress gave doctors their monopolistic prescribing privileges. But
in the 1980s, the APA and the industry took this relationship one
step further and essentially entered into a drug marketing "partner¬
ship." The APA and psychiatrists at academic medical centers
served as the front men in this arrangement, the public thereby see¬
ing "men of science” on stage, while the pharmaceutical companies
quietly provided the funds for this capitalistic enterprise.
The seed for this partnership was planted in 1974 when the APA
formed a task force to assess the importance of pharmaceutical sup¬
port for its future. The answer was "very," and in 1980 that led the
APA to institute a policy change of transformative importance. Up
to that time, pharmaceutical companies had regularly put up fancy
exhibits at the APA's annual meeting and paid for social events, but
they hadn't been allowed to put on "scientific” talks. However, in
1980, the APA's board of directors voted to allow pharmaceutical
companies to start sponsoring scientific symposiums at its annual
THE RISE OF AN IDEOLOGY
277
meeting. The drug firms paid the APA a fee for this privilege, and
soon the most well-attended events at its annual meeting were the
industry-funded symposiums, which provided the attendees a sump¬
tuous meal and featured presentations by a "panel of experts." The
speakers were paid handsomely to give the talks, and the drug com¬
panies made certain that their presentations went off without a
hitch. "These symposia are meticulously prepared with rehearsals
before the meeting, and they have excellent audio-visual content,"
Sabshin explained. 50
The door to a full-fledged "partnership" had been flung open, one
that would sell the medical model and the benefits of psychiatric
medications to the public, and the APA now began to regularly rely
on pharmaceutical money to fund many of its activities. The drug
companies began "endowing” continuing education programs and
psychiatric grand rounds at hospitals, and, as one psychiatrist ob¬
served, the companies were "happy to cap them with free food and
booze to sweeten the love of learning." 51 When the APA launched a
political action committee in 1982 to lobby Congress, this effort was
funded by pharma. The industry helped pay for the APA's media¬
training workshops. In 1985, APA secretary Fred Gottlieb observed
that the APA was now receiving "millions of dollars of drug house
money" each year. 50 Two years later, an issue of the APA's newsletter,
Psychiatric News, featured a photo of Smith, Kline and French
handing a check to APA president Robert Pasnau, which led one
reader to quip that the APA had become the "American Psychophar-
maceutical Association." 53 The APA was prospering financially now,
with its revenues jumping from $10.5 million in 1980 to $21.4 mil¬
lion in 1987, and it settled into a fancy new building in Washington,
D.C. It openly talked about "our partners in industry." 54
For the drug companies, the best part of this new partnership was
that it enabled them to turn psychiatrists at top medical schools into
"speakers,” even while those doctors considered themselves "inde¬
pendent.” The paid-for symposiums at the annual meetings greased
this new relationship. The symposiums were said to be "educational”
presentations, with the drug companies promising not to "control"
what the experts said. Yet their presentations were rehearsed,
and every speaker knew that if he broke from that script and started
2y8
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
talking about the drawbacks of psychiatric medications, he would
not be invited back.* There would be no industry-sponsored
symposiums on "supersensitivity psychosis," or the addictive effects
of benzodiazepines, or how antidepressants were no more effective
than active placebo. These speakers came to be known as "thought
leaders," their presence on the symposium panels elevating them to
the status of "stars" in the field, and by the early 2000s, they were
getting paid $2,000 to $10,000 per speech. "Some of us," confessed
E. Fuller Torrey, "believe that the present system is approaching a
high-class form of prostitution." 55
• These "thought leaders" also became the experts regularly
quoted by the media, and they wrote the textbooks published by the
APA. Psychiatry's thought leaders shaped our society's understand¬
ing of mental disorders, and once they began serving as paid speak¬
ers, the pharmaceutical companies sent money their way through
multiple channels. As the New England Journal of Medicine ob¬
served in 2000, thought leaders "serve as consultants to companies
whose products they are studying, join advisory boards and speak¬
ers' bureaus, enter into patent and royalty arrangements, agree to
be the listed authors of articles ghostwritten by interested compa¬
nies, promote drugs and devices at company-sponsored sympo¬
siums, and allow themselves to be plied with expensive gifts and
trips to luxurious settings." 55 Nor was it just a few psychiatrists
from academia that pharma courted with its dollars. The drug in¬
dustry understood this was a very effective way to market their
drugs, and collectively the companies began paying money to
virtually every well-known figure in the field. In 2000, when the
New England Journal of Medicine tried to find an expert to write an
* The academic psychiatrists also began to regularly give dinner talks to local
psychiatric groups, and in 2000, University of Mississippi psychiatrist John
Norton confessed in a letter to the New England Journal of Medicine that after
he wrote about the side effects of the sponsor's drug, "my invitations to speak
suddenly dropped from four to six times per month to essentially none." Prior
to that experience, he said, "I deluded myself into thinking I was educating
physicians, and not being swayed by the sponsors."
THE RISE OF AN IDEOLOGY
279
editorial on depression, it "found very few who did not have finan¬
cial ties to drug companies that make antidepressants.”
The NIMH also joined this storytelling coalition. The biological
psychiatrists knew that they had successfully captured the NIMH
when the Soteria Project was closed and Mosher was ousted, and
during the 1980s the NIMH actively promoted the biological psy¬
chiatry story to the public, an effort that took wing under the leader¬
ship of Shervert Frazier. Prior to being picked to head the NIMH in
1984, Frazier directed the APA's Commission on Public Affairs,
which had run the media-training workshops underwritten by phar¬
maceutical firms, and soon Frazier was announcing that the NIMH,
for the first time in its forty-year history, would launch a major ed¬
ucational campaign called the Depression Awareness, Recognition
and Treatment (DART) program. This educational effort would in¬
form the public that depressive disorders are "common, serious and
treatable," the NIMH said. Pharmaceutical companies would "con¬
tribute resources, knowledge and other forms of assistance to the
project,” which the NIMH promised would run for at least a
decade.” As it helped expand the market for psychiatric medica¬
tions, the NIMH even assured the public that the broken-brain
story was true. "Two decades of research have shown that [psychi¬
atric disorders] are diseases and illnesses like any other diseases and
illnesses," said NIMH director Lewis Judd in 1990, even though
nobody had ever been able to explain the nature of the pathology.”
The final group to participate in this storytelling campaign was
the National Alliance for the Mentally 111. Founded in 1979 by two
Wisconsin women, Beverly Young and Harriet Shelter, it arose as a
grassroots protest to Freudian theories that blamed schizophrenia
on "aloof, uncaring mothers and preoccupied mothers who were
unable to bond with their infants," a NAMI historian observed.”
NAMI was eager to embrace an ideology of a different kind, and
the message it sought to spread, said former NAMI president Agnes
Hatfield in 1991, was that "mental illness is not a mental health
problem; it is a biological illness. There is considerable clarity on
the part of families that they are focusing on a physical disease." 60
For the APA and pharma companies, the emergence of NAMI
could not have come at a more opportune moment. This was a
28 o
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
parents' group eager to embrace biological psychiatry, and both the
APA and pharmaceutical firms pounced. In 1983, the APA "entered
into an agreement with NAMI" to write a pamphlet on neuroleptic
drugs, and soon the APA was encouraging its branches across the
country "to foster collaborations with local chapters of the National
Alliance for the Mentally 111." 61 The APA and NAMI joined together
to lobby Congress to increase funding for biomedical research, and
the beneficiary of that effort, the NIMH —which saw its research
budget soar 84 percent during the 1980s —thanked the parents for
it. "The NIMH in a very meaningful sense is NAMI's institute,"
Judd told NAMI president Laurie Flynn in a 1990 letter. 62 By that
time, NAMI had more than 125,000 members, most of whom were
middle-class, and it was busily seeking to "educate the media, public
officials, healthcare providers, educators, the business community,
and the general public about the true nature of brain disorders," said
one NAMI leader. 6 ' NAMI brought a powerful moral authority to
the telling of the broken-brain story, and naturally pharmaceutical
companies were eager to fund its educational programs, with eigh¬
teen firms giving NAMI $11.72 million from 1996 to 1999. 64
In short, a powerful quartet of voices came together during the
1980s eager to inform the public that mental disorders were brain
diseases. Pharmaceutical companies provided the financial muscle.
The APA and psychiatrists at top medical schools conferred intellec¬
tual legitimacy upon the enterprise. The NIMH put the govern¬
ment's stamp of approval on the story. NAMI provided a moral
authority. This was a coalition that could convince American soci¬
ety of almost anything, and even better for the coalition, there was
one other voice on the scene that, in its own way, helped make the
story bulletproof in society's eyes.
The Critics Believe in Aliens
The story of a "psychopharmacology revolution" had first been
told in the 1950s and 1960s, and then, as we've seen in this chapter,
it was revived in the 1980s. However, the storytellers in the 1980s
THE RISE OF AN
D E O L O G Y
281
were more vulnerable to criticism than the storytellers of the earlier
decades simply because there was now twenty years of research that
undermined their narrative. None of the drugs had proven to help
people function well over the long term, and the chemical-
imbalance theory of mental disorders was in the process of flaming
out. As NIMH researchers had concluded in 1984, "elevations or
decrements in the functioning of serotonergic systems per se are not
likely to be associated with depression.” Close readers of The Bro¬
ken Brain could also see that, in fact, no great new discoveries had
been made. There was a Grand Canyon-sized gap between what the
broken-brain storytellers were intimating was true and what was
actually known, and that same gap would appear in their stories
when Prozac and the other second-generation drugs came to mar¬
ket. But fortunately for the proponents of biological psychiatry, crit¬
icism of the medical model and of psychiatric drugs became
associated, in the public mind, with Scientology.
L. Ron Hubbard, a science-fiction writer, founded the Church of
Scientology in 1952. One of the church's core tenets is that the earth
is populated by souls that previously lived on other planets, an "ex¬
traterrestrial” creation myth that could have been lifted directly
from a sci-fi novel. In addition, Hubbard had his own ideas about
how to heal the mind. Prior to founding Scientology, he had pub¬
lished Dianetics: The Modern Science of Mental Health, which out¬
lined the use of an "auditing” process to eliminate painful past
experiences from the mind. The scientific and medical community
ridiculed dianetics as quackery and dismissed Hubbard as a huck¬
ster, and he in turn developed an intense hatred for psychiatry. In
1969, Scientology and Thomas Szasz cofounded the Citizens Com¬
mission on Human Rights, and this group began waging campaigns
against lobotomy, electroshock, and psychiatric drugs.
This proved to be very fortuitous for the APA and its storytelling
partners as they raised the flag of biological psychiatry. Indeed, it is
easy to imagine the drug companies deciding to secretly fund Scien¬
tology's protests, eager as they were to shove money to any organ¬
ization that would —wittingly or unwittingly— advance their cause.
For not only did Scientologists believe in extraterrestrials, they also
had gained a reputation for being a secretive, litigious, and even
282
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
malevolent cult. Scientology, Time wrote in 1991, is a "hugely
profitable global racket that survives by intimidating members and
critics in a Mafia-like manner." 65 Thanks to Scientology, the powers
that be in psychiatry had the perfect storytelling foil, for they could
now publicly dismiss criticism of the medical model and psychiatric
drugs with a wave of the hand, deriding it as nonsense that arose
from people who were members of a deeply unpopular cult, rather
than criticism that arose from their own research. As such, the pres¬
ence of Scientology in the storytelling mix served to taint all
criticism of the medical model and psychiatric drugs, no matter
what its source.
Those were the storytelling forces that formed in the 1980s.
When Prozac arrived on the market, they were lined up perfectly for
the creation —and maintenance —of a tale about psychiatry's great
new leap.
14
The Story That Was . . . and
Wasn’t Told
When it comes to dead bodies in current psychotropic
trials, there are a greater number of them in the
active treatment groups than in the placebo groups.
This is quite different from what happens in pencillin
trials or trials of drugs that really work."
-DAVID HEALY, PROFESSOR OF PSYCHIATRY AT
CARDIFF UNIVERSITY, WALES (2008)
During the 1920s, owners of radios in the heartland of America reg¬
ularly tuned into station KFKB, which had perhaps the most power¬
ful signal in the country at that time, even though it emanated from
tiny Milford, Kansas. "This is Dr. John R. Brinkley greeting his
friends in Kansas and everywhere," they'd hear, and Dr. Brinkley
did indeed have a most amazing story to tell. In 1918, he had begun
transplanting goat gonads into the testicles of older men worried
about their declining virility, a fifteen-minute operation, he told
KFKB listeners, that had been proven to "completely restore" sex¬
ual prowess. "A man is as old as his glands," the good doctor
would explain, and this rejuvenating surgery worked because the
goat tissue "blends with and nourishes the human tissue, stimulat¬
ing the human gland to new activity." 2
Although Brinkley's medical credentials were of a dubious sort —
he had a degree from Eclectic Medical University of Kansas City, a
diploma mill —he was a masterful storyteller and something of an
advertising genius. After his first few operations, he told his story to
newspapers in Kansas, and soon they were publishing pictures of
him cradling the first "goat-gland baby," the offspring of an older
man who had undergone the operation. Older men began pouring
284
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
into Milford, each paying $750 for the procedure, and Brinkley
cranked up his publicity machine. He hired three press agents to
write ready-to-print newspaper features, which were then distrib¬
uted to "publications interested in popularizing the latest develop¬
ments from the laboratories of science.” Naturally, these planted
articles included testimonials from satisfied customers, such as J. J.
Tobias, chancellor of Chicago Law School, who —the articles said —
liked to pound his chest and shout: "I'm a new man! It's one of the
great things of the century!” Brinkley established his own "Scien¬
tific Press" and reported a ”90% to 95% success rate” for his sur¬
gery, which, he explained, returned the body to a proper hormonal
"balance.” Once he began broadcasting his story on KFKB in 1923,
he became so famous that three thousand letters arrived at his Mil¬
ford hospital each day, and by the late 1920s, he was perhaps the
wealthiest "doctor" in the United States.
Eventually, Dr. Brinkley earned a place in medical history as one
of the great charlatans of all time, when the American Medical As¬
sociation targeted him as a quack. But when it came to marketing
his goat-gonad surgery, he employed advertising techniques and a
storytelling model that have stood the test of time. He published ar¬
ticles that appeared scientific, courted the press, claimed a very high
success rate, offered a biological rationale for why the surgery
worked, and provided reporters with quotes from satisfied cus¬
tomers. That —as Eli Lilly and other drug manufacturers can attest —
is a tried-and-true formula for turning a psychiatric drug into a
commercial success.
Fibs, Lies, and a Blockbuster Drug
Today, the fraudulent nature of the story told by Eli Lilly and psy¬
chiatry about Prozac when it came to market is fairly well known,
having been documented by Peter Breggin, David Healy, and Joseph
Glenmullen, among others. Breggin and Healy wrote their accounts
after gaining access to Eli Lilly files while serving as expert witnesses
in civil lawsuits, which allowed them to see data and internal
THE STORY THAT WAS
AND WASN'T TOLD
285
memorandums that belied what the public had been told about the
drug. At the risk of going over familiar ground, we need to revisit
that story briefly, for it will help us see, with considerable clarity,
how our societal delusions about the merits of the "second-
generation" psychiatric drugs were formed. Eli Lilly's marketing of
Prozac proved to be a model that other companies followed as they
brought their drugs to market, and it involved telling a false story in
the scientific literature, hyping that story even more to the media,
and hiding risks that could lead to disability and death for those
who used the drugs.
The science of fluoxetine
Drug development begins in the laboratory, with investigation into
a drug's "mechanism of action,” and as we learned earlier, Eli Lilly
scientists determind in the mid-1970s that fluoxetine caused sero¬
tonin to "pile up” in the synapse, which in turn triggered a series of
physiological changes in the brain. Next, in animal studies, the drug
was found to cause stereotyped activity in rats (repetitious sniffing,
licking, etc.) and aggressive behavior in cats and dogs. 3 In 1977, Eli
conducted its first small trial in humans, but "none of the eight pa¬
tients who completed the four-week treatment showed distinct
drug-induced improvement," Eli Lilly's Ray Fuller tolcb his col¬
leagues in 1978. The drug also had caused "a fairly large number of
reports of adverse reactions.” One patient had gone psychotic on
the drug, and others had suffered from "akathisia and restlessness,"
Fuller said. 1
The trials of fluoxetine had barely begun and it was clear that Eli
Lilly had a big problem. Fluoxetine didn't appear to lift depression
and it caused a side effect —akathisia —known to increase the risk of
suicide and violence. After more reports of this kind came in, Eli
Lilly amended its trial protocols. "In future studies, the use of ben¬
zodiazepines to control the agitation will be permitted,” Fuller
wrote on July 23, 1979. 5 The benzodiazepines would help suppress
reports of akathisia, and likely boost efficacy results, as several trials
of benzodiazepines for depression had shown them to be as effective
as a tricyclic. Of course, as Eli Lilly's Dorothy Dobbs later confessed
286
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
in court, the use of benzodiazepines was "scientifically bad,” as it
would "confound the results" and "interfere with the analysis of
both safety and efficacy," but it enabled the company to continue
development of fluoxetine. 6
Still, even with addition of the benzodiazepines, fluoxetine failed
to perform well. During the early 1980s, the company conducted a
phase III trial of the drug in Germany, and in 1985, the German li¬
censing authority, Bundesgesundheitsamt (BGA), concluded that
this drug was "totally unsuitable for the treatment of depression." 7
According to the patients' "self ratings" (as opposed to the doctors'
ratings), the drug produced "little response or no improvement in
the clinical picture of the patients," the BGA noted. 8 At the same
time, it had caused psychosis and hallucinations, and increased
some patients' anxiety, agitation, and insomnia, "which as adverse
effects exceed those which are considered acceptable by medical
standards," the BGA wrote. 9 Most problematic of all, this drug
treatment could prove fatal. "Sixteen suicide attempts were made,
two of these with success," the BGA said. 19 A German Eli Lilly em¬
ployee privately calculated that the incidence rate of suicidal acts
for the fluoxetine patients was ”5.6 times higher than under the
other active medication, imipramine." 11
With Germany having rejected its application, Eli Lilly naturally
worried that it would be unable to gain FDA approval for fluoxe¬
tine.* It needed to hide the suicide data, and'in a 1994 civil lawsuit,
Nancy Lord, an expert in clinical trial design, explained how the
company did it. First, Eli Lilly instructed investigators to record var¬
ious drug-related adverse events as "symptoms of depression." As
such, in the trial results submitted to the FDA, the problems were
attributed to the disease rather than to fluoxetine. Second, when Eli
Lilly scientists tabulated the data from case report forms, they
changed individual reports of "suicidal ideation" to "depression."
Third, Lilly employees went through the German data "and pulled
out [suicide] cases that they didn't think were suicide." 12
All of these shenanigans, Lord told a court in 1994, made the
* At the end of 1989, Eli Lilly obtained approval to market fluoxetine in
Germany, but with a label that warned of the elevated risk of suicide.
THE STORY THAT WAS
AND WASN'T TOLD
287
entire testing process scientifically "worthless.” Yet even with these
statistical manipulations, Eli Lilly still struggled to present a con¬
vincing case for fluoxetine in its application to the FDA. It had
conducted placebo-controlled trials at eight sites, and in four of
them, the fluoxetine patients had fared no better than the placebo
group, and in the others, fluoxetine was only slightly better than a
placebo." Meanwhile, when Peter Breggin reviewed Lilly's docu¬
ments, he discovered that imipramine had proven to be more effec¬
tive than fluoxetine in six of seven trials. 14 The FDA, in its March
28, 1985, review of one large trial, made the same observation:
"Imipramine was clearly more effective than placebo, whereas flu¬
oxetine was less consistently better than placebo." 15 At best, fluoxe¬
tine's efficacy was of a very marginal sort, and FDA reviewer
Richard Kapit also worried about its safety. At least thirty-nine pa¬
tients treated with fluoxetine had gone psychotic in the short trials,
and slightly more than 1 percent had become manic or hypomanic.
Other side effects included insomnia, nervousness, confusion, dizzi¬
ness, memory dysfunction, tremors, and impaired motor coordina¬
tion. Fluoxetine, Kapit concluded, "may negatively affect patients
with depression." 16 The FDA also understood that Eli Lilly had
tried to hide many of these problems, the company having engaged
in "large-scale underreporting” of the harm that fluoxetine could
cause, according to reviewer David Graham. 17
While the trials may have been scientifically worthless, they
nevertheless proved to be an accurate forecast of what happened
after Prozac was brought to market. There were numerous anec¬
dotal accounts of Prozac-treated patients committing horrendous
crimes or killing themselves, and so many adverse-events reports
flowed into the FDA's MedWatch program that Prozac quickly be¬
came America's most complained about drug. By the summer of
1997, the FDA had received thirty-nine thousand such reports
about Prozac, far outstripping the number received by any other
drug for that nine-year period (1988-1997). The MedWatch filings
told of hundreds of suicides, and of a long list of vexing side effects,
which included psychotic depression, mania, abnormal thinking,
hallucinations, hostility, confusion, amnesia, convulsions, tremors,
and sexual dysfunction." The FDA estimates that only 1 percent of
288
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
all adverse events are reported to Med Watch, which suggests that
roughly 4 million Americans during that nine-year period had a bad
or even fatal reaction to Prozac. 19
The story told in the medical journals
Obviously, the record chalked up by fluoxetine in the clinical trials
was not one that would support a successful launch in the market¬
place. The public was not likely to embrace a medication that
German's licensing authority, in its initial review, had deemed "to¬
tally unsuitable" as a treatment for depression. If Prozac was going
to be successful, the psychiatrists that Eli Lilly had paid to run the
trials needed to tell a very different story in the medical journals and
to the public.
The first account of a U.S. trial of fluoxetine appeared in the
journal of Clinical Psychiatry in 1984. This novel agent, wrote
James Bremner, from Northwest Psychopharmacology Research in
Washington, "provides effective antidepressant activity with fewer
and less troublesome side effects than imipramine. . . . None of the
adverse events reported by fluoxetine patients were considered to be
drug related." Fluoxetine, he added, "proved more effective than
the tricyclic antidepressant." 211 Next, John Feigner, from the Univer¬
sity of California at San Diego, reported that fluoxetine was at least
equal in efficacy to imipramine (and probably superior to the tri¬
cyclic) and that "no serious side effects were observed” in his
twenty-two fluoxetine patients during a five-week study. 21 A theme
had been sounded —a very safe and improved antidepressant had
been developed —and Eli Lilly's investigators stuck to it in the years
that followed. "Fluoxetine was better tolerated than imipramine,"
California psychiatrist Jay Cohn reported in 1985. 22 "This drug,"
said Eli Lilly's Joachim Wernicke, in another article in the Journal of
Clinical Psychiatry, "has very few serious side effects." 2 ' Finally, in
the 1985 report on its large phase III trial, Eli Lilly announced that
"fluoxetine produced greater improvement than placebo on all
major efficacy parameters." 21
While these reports did tell of a new drug that was superior to the
old class of antidepressants, this still was not a narrative of a
THE STORY THAT WAS
AND WASN’T TOLD
289
"breakthrough" medication. There was no sense of why this drug
worked better, but as FDA approval for fluoxetine neared, a new
"fact" began to appear in the scientific reports. In a 1987 article in
the British Journal of Psychiatry, Sidney Levine wrote that "studies
have shown that [serotonin] deficiency plays an important role in
the psychobiology of depressive illness." 25 While this was not what
had actually been found —Levine had apparently missed the 1984
NIMH report that "elevations or decrements in the functioning of
serotonergic systems per se are not likely to be associated with de¬
pression”—this article set the stage for fluoxetine to be touted as a
drug that fixed a chemical imbalance. Two years later, University of
Louisville psychiatrists surveyed the fluoxetine literature in order to
provide "prescribing guidelines for the newest antidepressant," and
they wrote that "depressed patients have lower than normal con¬
centrations of [serotonin metabolites] in their cerebrospinal fluid.”
A delusional belief was now spreading through the medical litera¬
ture, and perhaps not surprisingly, the Kentucky psychiatrists con¬
cluded that fluoxetine, which theoretically raised serotonin levels,
was "an ideal drug for the treatment of depression." 26
This trail of reports in medical journals provided Eli Lilly with
the sound bites it needed to advertise its drug to doctors. The com¬
pany flooded medical journals with ads that featured good-looking
people who radiated happiness, the ads touting Prozac as equal in
efficacy to imipramine, and better tolerated. Science had proven
that psychiatry had a new and much improved pill for depression,
which appeared to correct a chemical imbalance in the brain.
The story told to the public
The story that had been told in psychiatric journals was certain to
resonate with the public. However, at this point, the market for
antidepressants was still moderate in size. When Prozac was ap¬
proved, Wall Street analysts predicted that it could generate $135 to
$400 million in annual sales for Eli Lilly. But the drug companies,
the APA, and the leaders of the NIMH were keen on expanding the
market for antidepressants, and the NIMH's DART "public aware¬
ness” campaign turned out to be the perfect vehicle for doing so.
290
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
After the NIMH announced its plans for DART in 1986, it had
studied the public's beliefs about depression. A survey revealed that
only 12 percent of American adults would take a pill to treat it.
Seventy-eight percent said they "would live with it until it passed,”
confident they could handle it on their own. This was an attitude
consistent with what the NIMH had preached only fifteen years ear¬
lier, when Dean Schuyler, head of the depressive section, had told
the public that most depressive episodes "will run their course and
terminate with virtually complete recovery without specific inter¬
vention." There was epidemiological wisdom in the public's belief
that depression would pass, but the NIMH —once Shervert Frazier
and other biological psychiatrists took the helm —was intent on de¬
livering a different message.
The purpose of DART, the NIMH explained in 1988, was "to
change public attitudes so that there is greater acceptance of depres¬
sion as a disorder rather than a weakness." The public needed to
understand that it regularly went "underdiagnosed and under¬
treated," and that it could "be a fatal disease” if left untreated.
There were 31.4 million Americans who suffered from at least a
mild form of depression, the NIMH said, and it was important that
they get diagnosed. The public needed to be made aware that anti¬
depressants produced recovery rates of ”70% to 80% in compari¬
son with 20% to 40% for placebo.” The NIMH vowed to continue
DART indefinitely in order to "inform” the public of these
"facts.
The NIMH officially launched DART in May 1988, five months
after Prozac landed on pharmacy shelves. The NIMH enlisted
"labor, religious, educational groups" and businesses to help it
spread its message, and of course pharmaceutical companies and
NAMI had been on board from the start. The NIMH ran advertise¬
ments in the media, and Eli Lilly helped pay for the printing and dis¬
tribution of 8 million DART brochures titled "Depression: What
You Need to Know.” This pamphlet informed readers, among other
things, of the particular merits of "serotonergic" drugs for the dis¬
ease. "By making these materials on depressive illness available,
accessible in physicians' offices all over the country, important in¬
formation is effectively reaching the public in settings which
THE STORY THAT WAS
AND WASN’T TOLD
291
encourage questions, discussion, treatment, or referral," said NIMH
director Lewis Judd. 28
The remaking of the American mind was under way. This selling
of depression, which was being done under the guise of a "public
education” campaign, turned into one of the most effective market¬
ing efforts ever devised. Newspapers picked up on this story, sales
of Prozac began to soar, and then, on December 18, 1989, the
green-and-white pill officially gained celebrity status when New
York magazine put it on its cover, BYE, BYE BLUES, the headline
screamed, A NEW WONDER DRUG FOR DEPRESSION. In the article,
one "anonymous" user of Prozac said that on a scale of 1 to 100,
he now felt "over 100.” Thanks to this new miracle pill, the maga¬
zine concluded, psychiatrists felt that their "profession has been
buoyed." 29
Other such glowing stories quickly followed. On March 26,
1990, Newsweek's cover featured the green-and-white capsule
floating Nirvana-like over a beautiful landscape, PROZAC: A BREAK¬
THROUGH DRUG FOR DEPRESSION the magazine announced. Physi¬
cians were now writing 650,000 prescriptions for the pill each
month, and "nearly everyone has something nice to say about the
new treatment,” Newsweek said. Patients were loudly exclaiming,
"I never felt better!" 30 Three days later, Natalie Angier of the New
York Times, who arguably was the nation's most popular science
writer, explained that antidepressants "work by restoring the bal¬
ance of neurotransmitter activity in the brain, correcting an abnor¬
mal excess or inhibition of the electrochemical signals that control
mood, thoughts, appetite, pain and other sensations." This new
drug. Dr. Francis Mondimore told Angier, "is not like alcohol or
Valium. It's like antibiotics." 31 Television shows weighed in with a
similar message, and on 60 Minutes, Lesley Stahl told the inspiring
story of a woman, Maria Romero, who, after a decade of horrible
depression, had been reborn on Prozac. "Somebody, something left
my body and another person came in," Romero said. Stahl happily
explained the biological cure that was at work: "Most doctors
believe that chronic depression like Romero's is caused by a chemi¬
cal imbalance in the brain. To correct it, the doctor prescribed
Prozac." 32
292
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Scientology to the Rescue
Fairly early on, there was a moment when this wonder-drug story
threatened to fall apart. The problem, of course, was that fluoxetine
did in fact stir suicidal and violent thoughts in some people, and
during the summer of 1990, the issue of Prozac's safety burst into
the news. And it was then, at that critical moment, that Scientology
proved so useful to Eli Lilly and the psychiatric establishment.
By 1990, so many people had suffered bad reactions to fluoxetine
that a national Prozac Survivors Support Group had formed. Many
harmed by the drug had taken their complaints to lawyers, and two
lawsuits in particular grabbed the public's attention. First, on July
18, newspapers reported that a Long Island woman, Rhoda Hala,
was suing Eli Lilly because, after going on Prozac, she had slashed
her wrists and "other parts of her body hundreds of times." 33 Two
weeks later, newspapers reported on a lawsuit related to a mass
murder committed by a crazed Kentucky man. Five weeks after
starting the drug, Joseph Wesbecker walked into a Louisville print¬
ing plant where he had worked and opened fire with an AK-47
assault rifle, killing eight and wounding twelve. The Citizens Com¬
mission on Human Rights quickly issued a press release urging
Congress to ban this "killer drug,” and that's when Eli Lilly
pounced. These lawsuits, Eli Lilly loudly announced, "are being
drummed up by the Scientology group, which has a history of criti¬
cizing the use of psychiatric drugs." 34
This was the start of Eli Lilly's campaign to save its blockbuster
drug. "Lilly can go down the tubes if we lose Prozac,” wrote chief
medical officer Leigh Thompson, in a harried 1990 memo. 35 The
company quickly honed a four-point message for the media: This
was an issue being raised by Scientologists; extensive clinical trials
had shown that Prozac was a safe drug; the suicidal and homicidal
events were "in the disease, not the drug"; and "people who could
be helped are being scared away from treatment, and that's the real
public menace." 36 The company ran media-training sessions for the
academic psychiatrists it hired as consultants, getting them to prac¬
tice their delivery of this message. "Frankly, I was unimpressed with
THE STORY THAT WAS
AND WASN'T TOLD
293
the performance of our outside professionals," company vice-
president Mitch Daniels complained to Thompson after one such
practice session in April 1991. The company would "mandate” that
the academic psychiatrists perform better "in their future training
sessions," he said.”
An article that appeared in the Wall Street Journal on April 19,
1991, showed that Eli Lilly's training sessions had paid off. "Scien¬
tology," the paper informed its readers, was a "quasi-religious/
business/paramilitary organization" that was "waging war on psy¬
chiatry." The group had attacked Prozac's safety even though "doc¬
tors unaffiliated with Lilly” had found, during the clinical trials,
that there was "a lower tendency toward suicidal thinking with
Prozac than with other antidepressants, or with the starch capsules
given to a control group.” It was, Leigh Thompson said, a "demor¬
alizing revelation to watch twenty years of solid research by doctors
and scientists shouted down in twenty-second sound bites by Scien¬
tologists and lawyers." Indeed, the Wall Street Journal reported, Eli
Lilly, in response to concerns about Prozac's safety, had asked "sui¬
cide experts" to re-scrutinize the trial data, but they had "concluded
that nothing in the clinical trials linked suicidal thinking —common
in depression patients —to Prozac.” It was the disease, not the drug,
and that was the tragedy, explained Jerrold Rosenbaum, a Harvard
psychiatrist at Massachusetts General Hospital. "The public's fear
of Prozac as a result of this campaign has itself become a potentially
serious public-health problem as people stay away from treat¬
ment." 38
Rosenbaum, naturally, was one of Eli Lilly's "outside profession¬
als.” As the Boston Globe later reported, he "sat on a marketing ad¬
visory panel for Lilly before Prozac was launched," his relationship
to Eli Lilly a "cozy” one. 39 But the Wall Street Journal presented
him as an independent expert, one of the nation's top depression
doctors, and so readers could only draw one conclusion: This was
an issue conjured up by noxious Scientologists, rather than a legiti¬
mate concern. Other newspapers and magazines framed the issue in
that way, with Time, in May of that year, publishing a scathing
cover story on Scientology, calling it a "criminal organization" that
attracted "psychopaths." 40
294
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
On September 20, 1991, the FDA did convene a hearing on
whether Prozac elevated the risk of suicide, but the advisory panel,
which was dominated by physicians with ties to pharmaceutical
companies, showed little interest in seriously investigating this ques¬
tion. Although more than two dozen citizens testified on the harm
that the drug could cause, the panel made sure that the scientific
discussion was limited to presentations that supported Eli Lilly's po¬
sition that fluoxetine was perfectly safe. As the Wall Street Journal
reported, the scientific data presented at the hearing proved that
"fluoxetine doesn't lead to increased suicide or suicidal thinking,
and, in fact, show that the drug helps alleviate these conditions.”
The entire controversy, one Lilly supporter told the Journal, was a
"complete fiction" that had been "organized and funded by an anti¬
psychiatric group.” 41
At that moment, Eli Lilly and all of psychiatry had achieved a
public relations victory of lasting importance. The wonder-drug
aura around Prozac had been restored, and the public and the
media had been conditioned to associate criticism of psychiatric
drugs with Scientology. The debate over the merits of these drugs
now seemed to feature the nation's top scientists and doctors on one
side and religious kooks on the other, and if that were so, the public
could be certain where the truth lay. Other SSRIs came to market,
sales of Prozac hit the $1 billion mark in 1992, and then, in 1993,
Brown University psychiatrist Peter Kramer, in his book Listening
to Prozac, pushed the wonder-drug story up a notch. Prozac, he
wrote, was making some patients "better than well.” An era of
"cosmetic psychopharmacology" was dawning, Kramer suggested,
with psychiatry likely to have pills in the near future that could give
normal people whatever personality they wanted. His book spent
twenty-one weeks on the New York Times bestseller list, and soon
Newsweek was warning readers that it was time for society to start
grappling with the ethical questions raised by psychiatry's new
powers. "The same scientific insights into the brain that led to the
development of Prozac are raising the prospect of nothing less than
made-to-order, off-the-shelf personalities," Newsweek explained in
1994. Will those who refuse to "give their brain a makeover," the
magazine asked, be left behind?
THE STORY THAT WAS
AND WASN'T TOLD
295
Gushed neuropsychiatrist Richard Restate: "For the first time in
human history, we will be in a position to design our own brains." 42
America Fooled
As the Prozac story unfolded in the media, surely the ghost of John
Brinkley was smiling somewhere. He had transfixed listeners to his
radio show with tales of the wonders of transplanted goat gonads,
and now here was a storytelling process that had transformed a
drug "totally unsuited” for treating depression into a miracle com¬
pound, with psychiatrists publicly wringing their hands over their
new godlike powers to shape the human mind. Should they worry
about making people "better than well"? Would our society lose
something precious if everybody were happy all the time? The wide¬
spread medicating of the American mind was now under way,
and—as a very quick review will reveal —it was this same story¬
telling process that supported the launch of Xanax as a drug for
panic disorder and the atypical antipsychotics for schizophrenia.
Once those "second-generation" drugs became blockbusters, the
drug companies and academic psychiatrists began touting psychi¬
atric drugs of all kinds for use in children, this storytelling sweeping
millions of American youth into the "mental illness" bin.
Xanax
Xanax (alprazolam) was approved by the FDA as an anti-anxiety
agent in 1981, and then Upjohn set out to get it approved for panic
disorder, which had been newly identified as a discrete condition for
the first time in DSM-III (1980). As a first step, it hired former
NIMH director Gerald Klerman to co-chair its "steering commit¬
tee” for the testing process, and it paid Daniel Freedman, editor of
the Archives of General Psychiatry, to be an assistant to its "divi¬
sion of medical affairs." 43 This was just part of the company's
efforts to co-opt academic psychiatry: "The most senior psychia¬
trists in the world were flooded with offers of consultancies" from
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
296 •
Upjohn, said Isaac Marks, an expert in anxiety disorders at the
Institute of Psychiatry in London.* 4
Klerman and Upjohn designed Upjohn's Cross National Collabo¬
rative Panic Study in a manner that could be expected to produce a
poor placebo response. Patients who had been on benzodiazepines
were allowed into the study, which meant that many in the placebo
group would in fact be going through the horrors of benzodiazepine
withdrawal, and thus could be expected to be extremely anxious
during the first weeks of the trial. Nearly one-fourth of the placebo
patients had traces of benzodiazepines in their blood when the
treatment period began. 45
Benzodiazepines are known to work quickly, and that proved
true in this study. At the end of four weeks, 82 percent of the alpra¬
zolam patients were "moderately improved” or "better," versus 43
percent of the placebo group. However, during the next four weeks,
the placebo patients continued to improve, while the alprazolam
patients did not, and by the end of the eighth week, there "was no
significant difference between the groups" on most of the rating
scales, at least among the patients who remained in the study. The
alprazolam group also experienced a variety of troubling side effects:
sedation, fatigue, slurred speech, amnesia, and poor coordination.
One of every twenty-six alprazolam patients suffered a "serious" re¬
action to the drug, such as mania or aggressive behavior. 46
At the end of eight weeks, the patients were tapered from their
medication for four weeks and then followed while medication-free
for another two weeks. The results were predictable. Thirty-nine
percent of those withdrawn from alprazolam "deteriorated signifi¬
cantly," their panic and anxiety skyrocketing to such an extent they
had to start taking the medication again. Thirty-five percent of the
alprazolam patients suffered "rebound” panic and anxiety symp¬
toms more severe than when the study began, and an equal per¬
centage suffered a host of debilitating new symptoms, including
confusion, heightened sensory perceptions, depression, a feeling that
insects were crawling over them, muscle cramps, blurred vision, di¬
arrhea, decreased appetite, and weight loss. 47
In sum, at the end of fourteen weeks, the drug-exposed patients
were worse off than the placebo group: They were more phobic,
THE STORY THAT WAS
AND WASN'T TOLD
297
The Xanax Study
s
<
u
'c
10
Q.
01
a
E
3
9 -
8 -
7 -
6
5
4
3
2
1
Placebo
Active treatment
Drug taper
Off drug
Baseline Week
1
Week Week Week Week
4 8 9 12
Week Week
13 14
In Upjohn's study of Xanax, patients were treated with the drug or placebo for eight weeks. Then
this treatment was slowly withdrawn (weeks 9 through 12), and during the last two weeks
patients didn't receive any treatment. The Xanax patients fared better during the first four weeks,
which is the result that the Upjohn investigators focused on in their journal articles. However,
once the Xanax patients began withdrawing from the the drug, they suffered many more panic
attacks than the placebo patients, and atthe end ofthe study were much more symptomatic.
Source: Ballenger, C. "Alprazolam in panic disorder and agoraphobia ."Archives of General Psych-
iatry 45 (1988): 41 3-22. Pecknold, C. "Alprazolam in panic disorder and agoraphobia." Archives of
General Psychiatry 45 (1 988): 429-36.
more anxious, more panic stricken, and doing worse on a "global
scale” that assessed overall well-being. Forty-four percent had been
unable to get off the drug, on their way to a lifetime of addiction. In
every way, the results painted a powerful portrait of the benzo trap:
This was a drug that worked for a short time, then its efficacy over
a placebo petered out, and yet when patients tried to go off the
drug, they became quite sick and many couldn't kick the habit. The
first few weeks of relief came at a very high long-term cost, with
those stuck on the drug —as previous benzodiazepine studies had
shown —likely to end up physically, emotionally, and cognitively
impaired.
The Upjohn investigators published three articles in the Archives
of General Psychiatry in May 1988, and anyone who carefully re¬
viewed the data could see the harm caused by alprazolam. But in
order for Xanax to be successfully marketed, Upjohn needed its in¬
vestigators to draw a different sort of conclusion, and so they did,
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
298
particularly in the abstracts of the three articles. First, they focused
their attention on the four-week results (rather than the eight-week
outcomes at the end of the treatment period), announcing that "al¬
prazolam was found to be effective and well-tolerated.” 48 Next,
they noted that 84 percent of the alprazolam patients had finished
the eight-week study, which was evidence that "patient acceptance
of alprazolam was high." Although their alprazolam patients regu¬
larly exhibited such problems as "slurred speech, amnesia” and
other signs of "impaired mentation," they still concluded that the
drug had "few side effects and is well tolerated." 49 Finally, while
they acknowledged that some alprazolam patients fared poorly
when the drug was withdrawn, they reasoned that it had been used
for too short a period and the withdrawal done too abruptly. "We
recommend that patients with panic disorder be treated for a longer
period, at least six months," they said. 50
In London, Isaac Marks and several of his colleagues at the Insti¬
tute of Psychiatry subsequently pointed out how transparently
ridiculous this all was. In a letter to the Archives of General Psychi¬
atry, they observed that since the alprazolam patients "were in a
worse state than patients receiving placebo" at the end of the study,
the finding by the Upjohn investigators that the drug was effective
and well tolerated could only be seen as "biased and arguable." 51
The entire affair, Marks subsequently wrote, "is a classic demon¬
stration of the hazards of research funded by industry." 52
Yet the fact that the alprazolam patients came to such a bad end,
with many on a path to a lifelong addiction, did not deter Upjohn,
Klerman, the APA, and the NIMH from touting Xanax's benefits to
the American public. The same marketing machinery that had made
Prozac a bestseller was rolled out again. Upjohn sponsored a sym¬
posium at the APA's 1988 meeting where the "expert panel” high¬
lighted the four-week results. Robert Pasnau, who had been head of
the APA in 1987, sent a glossy booklet on the Consequences of
Anxiety to APA members, an "educational" effort paid for by Up¬
john. Both Shervert Frazier and Gerald Klerman penned a "Dear
Doctor" letter that Upjohn included in the promotional literature it
sent to doctors about Xanax as a treatment for panic disorder. Up¬
john also gave $1.5 million to the APA so that it could mount a
THE STORY THAT WAS
AND WASN'T TOLD
299
DART-like campaign to "educate” psychiatrists, health-care workers,
and the public about panic disorder, which was said to be "under¬
recognized and undertreated." 53 Finally, the NIMH chipped in too,
identifying panic disorder as a priority concern and sponsoring a
conference in 1991 on it, with its panel of experts designating "high
potency benzodiazepines"— this would be Xanax —as one of the
two "treatments of choice." 54
The FDA approved Xanax as a treatment for panic disorder in
November 1990, and many newspapers and magazines ran the
usual features, IN A PANIC? HELP IS ON THE WAY, a St. Louis Post-
Dispatch headline announced. Treatment, the paper said, helped 70
to 90 percent of those with the debilitating condition, which af¬
flicted "4 million adults in this country." 55 The Associated Press ex¬
plained that "a biochemical malfunctioning in the brain is believed
to be one of the causes of panic attacks. Xanax can block the at¬
tacks by interacting with several different systems in the brain." 56 In
the Chicago Sun-Times, Dr. John Zajecka at Rush Medical College
in Chicago announced that "Xanax is the fastest acting and least
toxic” of medications for the disorder. 57 Once again, a very effec¬
tive, safe drug had arrived on the market, and in 1992, Xanax be¬
came the fifth most frequently prescribed medication in the United
States. 55
Not so atypical
Even as Xanax was on the way to market as a treatment for panic
disorder, Janssen was conducting tests of risperidone, a new drug
for schizophrenia. By this time, the methods that pharmaceutical
firms were employing to create new "blockbuster" psychotropics
were becoming quite well practiced, with nearly everyone employ¬
ing the Prozac model of drug development, and so Janssen, like Eli
Lilly and Upjohn, designed trials that were biased in favor of its
drug. In particular, Janssen compared multiple doses of risperidone
to a high dose of haloperidol (Haldol), as it could then be relatively
certain that one of the risperidone doses would have a good safety
profile in comparison to the old "standard” neuroleptic. As FDA
reviewers noted, these studies were "incapable” of providing any
300
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
meaningful comparison of the two drugs. 59 In the FDA's letter of ap¬
proval to Janssen, Robert Temple, director of the Office of Drug
Evaluation, made this clear:
We would consider any advertisement or promotion labeling
for RISPERDAL false, misleading, or lacking fair balance
under section 502 (a) and 502 (n) of the ACT if there is pres¬
entation of data that conveys the impression that risperidone
is superior to haloperidol or any other marketed antipsy¬
chotic drug product with regard to safety or effectiveness. 60
However, while the FDA could prohibit Janssen from placing
advertisements touting its drug as superior to haloperidol, it did not
have authority over what the academic psychiatrists hired by
Janssen could say. This was the commercial beauty of the "partner¬
ship” that had emerged between psychiatry and the pharmaceutical
industry during the 1980s —the academic doctors could make
claims, both in their medical journals and to the public, that the
FDA considered false in kind. In this case, they published more than
twenty articles in psychiatric journals touting risperidone as equal
or superior to haloperidol in reducing positive symptoms of schizo¬
phrenia (psychosis) and superior to haloperidol in improving nega¬
tive symptoms (lack of emotion). The academic doctors reported
that risperidone reduced hospital stays, improved the patient's abil¬
ity to function socially, and reduced hostility. "Risperidone has im¬
portant advantages compared with haloperidol," they wrote in the
journal of Clinical Psychiatry. "When administered in an effective
dose range, risperidone produced greater improvements on all five
dimensions of schizophrenia." 61
Once again, this was a scientific story of a new and improved
treatment, and in their interviews with the media, Janssen's investi¬
gators told of a wonder drug. This new agent, the Washington Post
reported, "represents a glimmer of hope for a disease that until re¬
cently had been considered hopeless.” Risperidone, it explained, did
not "cause sedation, blurred vision, impaired memory or muscle
stiffness, side effects commonly associated with an earlier genera¬
tion of antipsychotic drugs." 62 The New York Times, quoting
THE STORY THAT WAS
AND WASN'T TOLD
301
Richard Meibach, Janssen's clinical research director, reported that
"no major side effects" had appeared in the two-thousand-plus pa¬
tients treated with risperidone in the clinical trials.* The drug was
thought to "relieve schizophrenia symptoms by blocking excessive
flows of serotonin or dopamine, or both," the paper said.* 3
The atypical revolution was on. Risperdal apparently restored
sanity by balancing multiple neurotransmitters in the brain, and it
seemed to cause no side effects of any note. In 1996, Eli Lilly
brought Zyprexa (olanzapine) to market, and the public story of the
wonders of atypicals got ramped up another notch.
As had become customary, Eli Lilly employed trials that were
"biased by design" against haloperidol, the FDA concluded. As a
result, its large phase III trial, which wasn't placebo controlled, pro¬
vided "little useful efficacy data." As for olanzapine's safety profile,
twenty patients treated with the drug during the trials died, 22
percent suffered a "serious" adverse event (higher than in the
haloperidol patients), and two-thirds failed to complete the studies.
Olanzapine, the data suggested, made patients sleepy and fat, and
caused such problems as Parkinsonian symptoms, akathisia, dysto¬
nia, hypotension, constipation, tachycardia, diabetes, seizures, leak¬
ing breasts, impotence, liver abnormalities, and white blood cell
disorders. Furthermore, warned the FDA's Paul Leber, since olanza¬
pine blocked receptors for many types of neurotransmitters, "no
one should be surprised if, upon marketing, events of all kinds and
severity not previously identified are reported in association with
olanzapine's use." 64
That was the story told by the trial data. The story that Eli Lilly
wanted to appear in the medical journals and newspapers was that
Zyprexa was better than Janssen's Risperdal, and so that's the story
that its hired guns told. Psychiatrists from academic medical schools
announced that olanzapine worked in a more "comprehensive” man¬
ner than either risperidone or haloperidol. It was a well-tolerated
* In fact, eighty-four patients treated with risperidone had suffered a "serious
adverse event," which the FDA defined as a life-threatening event or one that
required hospitalization.
302
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
agent that led to global improvement—it reduced positive symp¬
toms, caused fewer motor side effects than other antipsychotics,
and improved negative symptoms and cognitive function. 65 This
second atypical was better than the first, and the Wall Street Journal
ran with that angle. Zyprexa, it announced, "has substantial advan¬
tages” over other current therapies. "The real world,” explained
John Zajecka, from Rush Medical College, "is finding that Zyprexa
has fewer extrapyramidal side effects than Risperdal." 66 Zyprexa is
"a potential breakthrough of tremendous magnitude," Stanford
University psychiatrist Alan Schatzberg told the New York Times. 67
The only question now seemed to be whether Zyprexa was truly
better than Risperdal, and after AstraZeneca brought a third atypi¬
cal to market, Seroquel, the media settled on the notion that collec¬
tively the new atypicals were a dramatic improvement over the older
drugs. They were, Parade told its readers, "far safer and more effec¬
tive in treating negative symptoms, such as difficulty in reasoning
and speaking in an organized way." 66 The newer drugs, the Chicago
Tribune announced, "are safer and more effective than older ones.
They help people go to work." 69 Wrote the Los Angeles Times, "It
used to be that schizophrenics were given no hope of improving. But
now, thanks to new drugs and commitment, they're moving back
into society like never before.” 70 NAMI chimed in, too, publishing
a book titled Breakthroughs in Antipsychotic Medications, which
helpfully explained that these new drugs "do a better job of balanc¬
ing all of the brain chemicals, including dopamine and serotonin.” 71
On and on it went, and finally NAMI's executive director, Laurie
Flynn, told the press that the promised land had at last been reached:
"These new drugs truly are a breakthrough. They mean we should
finally be able to keep people out of the hospital, and it means that
the long-term disability of schizophrenia can come to an end." 72
Lancet Asks a Question
That was the sequence of storytelling that led to the explosive rise in
the use of psychiatric drugs in the United States. First, American
THE STORY THAT WAS
AND WASN'T TOLD
303
psychiatrists touted Prozac as a wonder drug, next they hailed
Xanax as a safe and effective therapy for panic disorder, and finally
they informed the public that atypical antipsychotics were "break¬
through” medications for schizophrenia. In this way, they rejuve¬
nated the market for psychiatric medications, even though the
clinical studies of the new drugs had not told of any therapeutic
advance.
At least in scientific circles, the "wonder drug” glow around the
second-generation psychotropics has long since disappeared. As we
learned earlier, the SSRIs were reported in 2008 to provide a mean¬
ingful clinical benefit only to severely depressed patients. Xanax is
now understood to be much more addictive than Valium, with vari¬
ous investigators determining that two-thirds of people who take it
for any length of time have trouble getting off it.” As for the top¬
selling atypicals, the hyping of these drugs is now viewed as one of
the more embarrassing episodes in psychiatry's history, as one
government-funded study after another failed to find that they were
any better than the first-generation antipsychotics. In 2005, the
NIMH's "CATIE Trial" determined that there were "no significant
differences” between the atypicals and their predecessors, and even
more troubling, in this study neither the new drugs nor the old ones
could really be said to work. Seventy-four percent of the 1,432 pa¬
tients were unable to stay on the medications, mostly because of
their "inefficacy or intolerable side effects." 74 A study by the U.S.
Department of Veterans Affairs came to a similar conclusion about
the relative merits of atypicals and the older drugs, and then, in
2007, British psychiatrists reported that schizophrenia patients, if
anything, had a better "quality of life” on the old drugs than on the
new ones. 75 All of this led two prominent psychiatrists to write in
the Lancet that the story of the atypicals as breakthrough medica¬
tions could now be "regarded as invention only,” a tale concocted
"by the drug industry for marketing purposes and only now being
exposed.” Yet, they wondered, "how is it that for nearly two
decades we have, as some have put it, 'been beguiled' into thinking
they were superior?" 76
History, as readers of this book can attest, reveals the answer to
that question. The seed for the atypicals story was planted in the
304
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
early 1980s, when the APA embraced "biological psychiatry" as a
story that could be successfully marketed to the public. This was
also a story that the field, as a whole, desperately wanted to believe
in, and soon Nancy Andreasen and others were telling of a revolu¬
tion that was under way, with mental illnesses finally giving up their
biological secrets, even though nobody could precisely explain what
those secrets were. That story gained steam, prepping the public to
believe that therapeutic advances were on the way, and as pharma¬
ceutical companies brought new medications to market, they hired
the top psychiatrists in the country to tell of how these new won¬
drous drugs "balanced” brain chemistry. And it was that co-opting
of academic medicine that gave the story its credibility. This was a
story told by Harvard Medical School psychiatrist Jerrold Rosen¬
baum, by former NIMH director Gerald Klerman, and by Stanford
University psychiatrist Alan Schatzberg.
Of course we, as a society, believed it.
Silencing Dissent
As we have seen, American psychiatry has told the public a false story
over the past thirty years. The field promoted the idea that its drugs
fix chemical imbalances in the brain when they do no such thing,
and it grossly exaggerated the merits of the second-generation
psychotropics. In order to keep that tale of scientific progress afloat
(and to protect its own belief in that tale), it has needed to squelch
talk about the harm that the drugs can cause.
Psychiatry's policing of its own ranks began in earnest in the late
1970s, when Loren Mosher was ousted from the NIMH for having
run his Soteria experiment. The next prominent psychiatrist to end
up on psychiatry's hit list was Peter Breggin. Although he is known
today for his "antipsychiatry” writings, he, too, had once been on
the fast track at the NIMH. After finishing his residency at a Har¬
vard Medical School hospital, Breggin went to the NIMH in 1966
to work on developing community mental health centers. "I was
still the young hotshot guy," he recalled, in an interview. "I thought
THE STORY THAT WAS
AND WASN’T TOLD
305
I would be the youngest professor of psychiatry in the history of
Harvard Medical School. That was the trajectory I was on."” How¬
ever, he saw that the future belonged to biological psychiatry, as op¬
posed to the social psychiatry that interested him, and he left the
NIMH to go into private practice. Soon he began writing about the
hazards of electroshock and psychiatric drugs, which, he argued,
"worked” by disabling the brain. After a number of heated battles
with the APA's leaders, Breggin appeared in 1987 on Oprah Win¬
frey's television show, where he spoke about tardive dyskinesia and
how that dysfunction was evidence that neuroleptics damaged the
brain. His comments so infuriated the APA that it sent a transcript
of the show to NAMI, which in turn filed a complaint with the
Maryland State Commission on Medical Discipline, asking that it
take away Breggin's medical license on the grounds that his state¬
ments had caused schizophrenia patients to stop taking their medi¬
cations (and thus caused harm). Although the commission decided
not to take any action, it did conduct an inquiry (rather than sum¬
marily dismissing NAMI's complaint), and the message to everyone
in the field was, once again, quite clear.
"I think the interesting thing is that Loren [Mosher] and I took
on scientifically the two sides of the issue,” Breggin said. "Loren
took on the issue that there is a better treatment than drugs for
schizophrenia. I took on the treatments —the drugs, electroshock,
and psychosurgery. And what this showed is that it didn't matter
which end you wanted to take, they were willing to destroy your
career. That is the lesson."
The career setback that Irish psychiatrist David Healy experi¬
enced was, in some ways, reminiscent of Mosher's fall from grace.
During the 1990s, he earned a reputation as one of the field's lead¬
ing historians, his writings focusing on the psychopharmacology
era. He had served as secretary of the British Association for Psy¬
chopharmacology, and in early 2000, he accepted an offer from the
University of Toronto's Centre for Addiction and Mental Health to
head up its mood and anxiety program. Up until that moment, he
was very much part of the psychiatric establishment, just as Mosher
had been. However, for several years he had been interested in the
question of whether SSRIs could stir suicide, and he had recently
306
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
completed a "healthy volunteers" study. Two of the twenty volun¬
teers had become suicidal after they were exposed to an SSRI, which
clearly showed that the drug could cause such thoughts. Not long
after he accepted the Toronto job, he presented his results at a meet¬
ing of the British Association for Psychopharmacology. There, one
of the most prominent figures in American psychiatry warned him
to knock it off. "He told me that my career would be destroyed if I
kept on showing results like the ones I'd just shown, that I had no
right to bring out hazards of the pills like these," Healy said.™
In November of 2000, only a few months before he was sched¬
uled to start his new job at the University of Toronto, Healy gave a
talk on the history of psychopharmacology at a colloquium orga¬
nized by the school. In his presentation, Healy spoke about prob¬
lems that had arisen with neuroleptics since their introduction in the
1950s, briefly reviewed the data showing that Prozac and other
SSRIs elevated the risk of suicide, and then observed in passing that
outcomes for affective disorders are worse today than they were a
century ago. This, he observed, shouldn't be happening if "our
drugs really worked." 79
Although the audience subsequently rated his talk as the collo¬
quium's best for content, by the time Healy arrived back in Wales,
the University of Toronto had rescinded the job offer. "While you
are held in high regard as a scholar of the history of modern psychi¬
atry, we do not feel your approach is compatible with the goals for
development of the academic and clinical resources that we have,"
wrote the Centre's head psychiatrist, David Goldbloom, in an
e-mail. 80 Once more, others in the field could draw only one lesson.
"The message is that it is a bad idea to speak out, and that the idea
that treatments might not work or might not be best managed by
being entrusted to doctors is beyond the pale,” Healy said in an in¬
terview. 81
Numerous others can attest to the fact that it is a "bad idea" to
speak out. Nadine Lambert, a psychologist at the University of Cal¬
ifornia at Berkeley, conducted a long-term study of children treated
with Ritalin and found that, as young adults, they had elevated
rates of cocaine abuse and cigarette smoking. After she reported her
results at a 1998 NIH conference, the National Institute on Drug
THE STORY THAT WAS
AND WASN'T TOLD
307
Abuse stopped funding her work. In 2000, when Joseph Glen-
mullen, a clinical instructor in psychiatry at Harvard Medical
School, authored Prozac Backlash, which detailed the many prob¬
lems associated with the use of SSRIs, Eli Lilly mounted a campaign
to discredit him. A public-relations firm gathered critical comments
from several prominent psychiatrists, who derided Glenmullen as a
"nobody" in the field, and then it mailed these "reviews" to various
newspapers. "It's a dishonest book, it's manipulative, it's mischie¬
vous," said Harvard Medical School psychiatrist Jerrold Rosen¬
baum, even though he was a colleague of Glenmullen's. The press
release naturally did not mention that Rosenbaum was an Eli Lilly
consultant. 82 Next up on the chopping block: Gretchen LeFever, a
psychologist at East Virginia Medical School. After she published
research showing that an overly high number of children in Virginia
schools were being diagnosed with ADHD, an anonymous "whistle¬
blower" charged her with scientific misconduct. Her federal research
funds were cut off and her computers were seized, and while she
was subsequently cleared of any misconduct, her career had still
been derailed.
Said Healy: "The thought-control aspect of things in psychiatry
today is like old-style Eastern European social control."
Hiding the Evidence
The third aspect to the storytelling process that has led to our socie¬
tal delusion about the merits of psychiatric drugs is easy to docu¬
ment. Imagine what our beliefs would be today if, over the past
twenty years, we had opened our newspapers and read about the
following findings, which represent but a sampling of the outcome
studies we reviewed earlier in the book:
1990: In a large, national depression study, the eighteen-
month stay-well rate was highest for those treated with
psychotherapy (30 percent) and lowest for those treated
with an antidepressant (19 percent). (NIMH)
308
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
1992: Schizophrenia outcomes are much better in poor
countries like India and Nigeria, where only 16 percent of
patients are regularly maintained on antipsychotics, than
in the United States and other rich countries, where contin¬
ual drug usage is the standard of care. (World Health
Organization)
1995: In a six-year study of 547 depressed patients,
those who were treated for the disorder were nearly seven
times more likely to become incapacitated than those who
weren't, and three times more likely to suffer a "cessation"
of their "principal social role.” (NIMH)
1998: Antipsychotic drugs cause morphological changes
in the brain that are associated with a worsening of schizo¬
phrenia symptoms. (University of Pennsylvania)
1998: In a World Health Organization study of the mer¬
its of screening for depression, those diagnosed and treated
with psychiatric medications fared worse —in terms of
their depressive symptoms and their general health —over
a one-year period than those who weren't exposed to the
drugs. (WHO)
1999: When long-term benzodiazepine users withdraw
from the drugs, they become "more alert, more relaxed,
and less anxious." (University of Pennsylvania)
2000: Epidemiological studies show that long-term out¬
comes for bipolar patients today are dramatically worse
than they were in the pre-drug era, with this deterioration
in modern outcomes likely due to the harmful effects of
antidepressants and antipsychotics. (Eli Lilly; Harvard
Medical School)
2001: In a study of 1,281 Canadians who went on
short-term disability for depression, 19 percent of those
who took an antidepressant ended up on long-term dis¬
ability, versus 9 percent of those who didn't take the med¬
ication. (Canadian investigators)
2001: In the pre-drug era, bipolar patients did not suffer
cognitive decline over the long term, but today they end up
THE STORY THAT WAS
AND WASN’T TOLD
309
almost as cognitively impaired as schizophrenia patients.
(Sheppard Pratt Health System in Baltimore)
2004: Long-term benzodiazepine users suffer cognitive
deficits "moderate to large” in magnitude. (Australian sci¬
entists)
2005: Angel dust, amphetamines, and other drugs that
induce psychosis all increase D 2 HIGH receptors in the
brain; antipsychotics cause this same change in the brain.
(University of Toronto)
2005: In a five-year study of 9,508 depressed patients,
those who took an antidepressant were, on average, symp¬
tomatic nineteen weeks a year, versus eleven weeks for
those who didn't take any medication. (University of Cal¬
gary)
2007: In a fifteen-year study, 40 percent of schizophre¬
nia patients off antipsychotics recovered, versus 5 percent
of the medicated patients. (University of Illinois)
2007: Long-term users of benzodiazepines end up
"markedly ill to extremely ill" and regularly suffer from
symptoms of depression and anxiety. (French scientists)
2007: In a large study of children diagnosed with ADHD,
by the end of the third year "medication use was a signifi¬
cant marker not of beneficial outcome, but of deteriora¬
tion.” The medicated children were also more likely to
engage in delinquent behavior; they ended up slightly
shorter, too. (NIMH)
2008: In a national study of bipolar patients, the major
predictor of a poor outcome was exposure to an anti¬
depressant. Those who took an antidepressant were nearly
four times as likely to become rapid cyclers, which is asso¬
ciated with poor long-term outcome. (NIMH)
A check of newspaper archives reveals that the psychiatric estab¬
lishment has thoroughly succeeded in keeping this information from
the public. I searched for accounts of these studies in the New York
Times archives and in the LexisNexis database, which covers most
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
310
U.S. newspapers, and I couldn't find a single instance where the
results were accurately reported.*
Newspapers, of course, would have been happy to publish these
study results. However, medical news is typically generated in this
way: The scientific journals, the NIH, medical schools, and pharma¬
ceutical companies issue press releases touting certain findings as
important, and reporters then sift through the releases to identify
the ones they deem worthy of writing about. If no press releases are
issued, or there is no other effort by the medical community to pub¬
licize the findings, then no stories appear. We can even document
this blackout process at work in the NIMH's handling of Martin
Harrow's outcomes study. In 2007, the year he published his results
in the Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, the NIMH issued
eighty-nine press releases, many on inconsequential matters. But it
did not issue one on Harrow's findings, even though his was ar¬
guably the best study of the long-term outcomes of schizophrenia
patients that had ever been done in the United States. 83 It's fair to
say that if the results had been the reverse, the NIMH would have
sounded the press-release gong and newspapers across the country
would have touted the findings.
Although reports about most of the studies listed above simply
never appeared in newspapers, there were a couple of instances
when psychiatrists were forced to say something to reporters about
one of the studies, and each time they spun the results. For example,
when the NIMH announced the three-year results from its MTA
study of ADHD treatments, it did not inform the public that stimu¬
lant usage during the third year was a "marker of deterioration."
Instead, it put out a press release with this headline: IMPROVEMENT
* There were newspaper reviews of my book Mad in America that mentioned
the WHO study of better schizophrenia outcomes in poor countries where pa¬
tients were not regularly maintained on the drugs, and since then, this informa¬
tion has become somewhat known. In addition, I mentioned Martin Harrow's
fifteen-year schizophrenia study in a talk I gave at Holy Cross College in Feb¬
ruary 2009, and that led to a February 8, 2009, article in the Worcester
Telegram and Gazette (Mass.) that discussed Harrow's work. That was the
first time that news of his study had appeared in any American newspaper.
THE STORY THAT WAS
AND WASN'T TOLD
• 311
FOLLOWING ADHD TREATMENT SUSTAINED FOR MOST CHILDREN.
That headline told of drugs that had been beneficial, and while the
text of the release did state that "continuing medication was no
longer associated with better outcomes by the third year," it also in¬
cluded a canned quote from lead author Peter Jensen stating that
there was still plenty of reason to keep children on Ritalin. "Our re¬
sults suggest that medication can make a long-term difference for
some children if it's continued with optimal intensity, and not
started or added too late in a child's clinical course." 84
If we want to get another look at this spinning process, we can
turn to a 1998 New York Times article that briefly told of the WHO
study on schizophrenia outcomes in rich and poor countries. After
interviewing psychiatrists about the study, the Times reporter wrote
that "schizophrenics generally responded better to treatment in less
developed countries than in more technologically developed coun¬
tries." 88 Responded better to treatment— readers could only assume
that schizophrenia patients in India and Nigeria responded better to
antipsychotics than patients in the United States and other rich
countries did. They had no way to know that "treatment" for 84
percent of the schizophrenia patients in the poor countries consisted
of being off the drugs.
In July 2009,1 also searched the NIMH and NAMI websites for
some mention of the studies listed above, and I found zilch. For in¬
stance, the NIMH website did not discuss the remarkable decline in
bipolar outcomes in modern times, even though Carlos Zarate, who
coauthored the 2000 article that documented this decline, was head
of the NIMH's mood and anxiety disorders research unit in 2009.
Similarly, NAMI's website didn't provide any information about
Harrow's study, even though it provides reason for parents of schiz¬
ophrenic children to be optimistic. Forty percent of those off med¬
ications recovered over the long term! But that finding directly
contradicted the message that NAMI has promoted to the public for
decades, and NAMI's website is sticking to that message. Antipsy¬
chotics, it informs the public, "correct an imbalance in the chemi¬
cals that enable brain cells to communicate with each other." 86
Finally, the entire outcomes history documented in this book is
missing from the 2008 edition of the APA's Textbook of Psychiatry,
312 • ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
which means that medical students training to be psychiatrists are
kept in the dark about this history." The book does not discuss "su¬
persensitivity psychosis.” It does not mention that antidepressants
may be depressogenic agents over the long term. It does not report
that bipolar outcomes are much worse today than they were forty
years ago. There is no discussion of rising disability rates. There is
no talk about the cognitive impairment that is seen in longtime
users of psychotropic drugs. The textbook authors are clearly famil¬
iar with many of the sixteen studies listed above, but, if they do
mention them, they don't discuss the relevant facts about medica¬
tion usage. The long-running study by Harrow, the textbook states,
reveals that there are some schizophrenia patients who "are able to
function without the benefit of continuous antipsychotic treat¬
ment.” The authors of that sentence didn't mention the stunning
difference in recovery rates for the unmedicated and medicated
groups; instead they crafted a sentence that told of the benefit of
continuous antipsychotic treatment. In a similar vein, while the
textbook briefly discusses the WHO study on the better outcomes
of schizophrenia patients in poor countries like India and Nigeria, it
does not mention that patients in those countries weren't regularly
maintained on antipsychotics. In a section on benzodiazepines, the
authors acknowledge that there are concerns about their addictive
properties, but then state that long-term outcomes for those who
stay on benzodiazepines are generally good, as most patients
"maintain their therapeutic gains."
There is a story that psychiatry doesn't dare tell, which shows
that our societal delusion about the benefits of psychiatric drugs
isn't entirely an innocent one. In order to sell our society on the
soundness of this form of care, psychiatry has had to grossly exag¬
gerate the value of its new drugs, silence critics, and keep the story
of poor long-term outcomes hidden. That is a willful, conscious
process, and the very fact that psychiatry has had to employ such
storytelling methods reveals a great deal about the merits of this
paradigm of care, much more than a single study ever could.
15
Tallying Up the Profits
"Receiving $750 checks for chatting with some
doctors during a lunch break was such easy money
that it left me giddy."
- PSYCHIATRIST DANIEL CARLAT (2007)'
The walk from Jenna's group home in Montpelier, Vermont, to the
town's Main Street is only two blocks long, and yet, on the late
spring morning I visited, it took us twenty minutes to travel that dis¬
tance, for Jenna had to stop every few steps and catch her balance,
with her aide, Chris, constantly putting his hand up behind her in
case she fell.* Jenna had first taken an antidepressant twelve years
earlier, when she was fifteen years old, and now she was on a daily
cocktail of eight drugs, including one for drug-induced Parkinson¬
ian symptoms. As we sat outside a cafe, Jenna told me her story, al¬
though at times —because of her problems with motor control —it
was difficult to understand her. Her tremors are so severe that when
she dunked her pastry, the coffee spilled and she had trouble bring¬
ing the pastry to her lips.
"I'm sooooooo messed up,” she says.
I had gone to the interview thinking that Jenna had been diag¬
nosed with tardive dyskinesia, an antipsychotic side effect that can
disable people. But it wasn't clear whether her motor impairments
* Although Jenna said that I could use her last name, her mother and
stepfather, who have legal guardianship, requested that I use her first name
only.
314
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
were due to that particular type of drug-induced dysfunction or to a
more idiosyncratic drug-related process, and by the time the inter¬
view was over, Jenna had raised a new issue for me to think about.
She told of how psychiatrists and other mental health workers had
always resisted seeing any of her physical or emotional difficulties
as drug-caused, but instead had regularly blamed everything on her
illness, and, from her point of view, that was a thinking process dic¬
tated by monetary interests. If you wanted to understand the care
she'd received, you had to understand that she was valuable to the
pharmaceutical companies as a "consumer" of their medications.
"Nobody," Chris explains, "has addressed the fact that the drugs
may be causing her problems."
The first time that Jenna had been exposed to a psychiatric drug
was when she was in the second grade, and that episode suggested
that she would not be a good responder to psychotropics. Up until
that time Jenna had been a healthy child, a star on a local swim
team; only then she developed seizures, and when she was put on an
anticonvulsive agent, she developed severe motor problems, her
mother said, in a phone interview. But eventually the seizures went
away and once Jenna stopped taking the anticonvulsant, her motor
problems disappeared. Jenna took up horseback riding, excelling in
"show-jumping competition. "She was back to being totally nor¬
mal," her mother recalled.
When Jenna entered ninth grade, her mother and stepfather de¬
cided to send her to an elite boarding school in Massachusetts, as
they didn't trust the public schools in Tennessee, and it was then
that her behavioral and emotional problems began. She was kicked
out of that first school and sent to a second one for troubled teens,
where she "got into all that Gothic stuff" and began "acting out"
sexually, her mother said. Then, on a dare one night, Jenna stole a
package of condoms from a drugstore and "freaked out" when she
was arrested. Now she was sent to a third boarding school and
prescribed Paxil.
"The minute she takes that drug, she starts shaking,” her mother
said. "I tell the doctor, 'Oh my gosh, it is from the medicine.' The
doctor says, 'Oh no, it's not the medicine.' I said, 'Yes it is.' We went
TALLYING UP THE PROFITS
315
from one doctor to another, doing test after test, but they couldn't
find anything and so they kept her on the medications, which made
everything worse. They just wouldn't listen to me.”
In addition to the tremors, Jenna became suicidal while taking
Paxil, and soon her life transformed into a psychiatric nightmare.
She began cutting herself regularly, and at one point, she used an
electric saw to take off the middle finger on her left hand. The Paxil
gave way to cocktails of Klonopin, Depakote, Zyprexa, and other
medications, and during a nearly four-year stay in a psych hospital,
she ended up on a cocktail of fifteen or so drugs, so doped up she
didn't even know where she was. "I don't know the exact date,"
Jenna says, summing up this history, "but slowly my speech and my
walking and my balance and the shaking got really bad at that hos¬
pital. And they just kept on adding drugs. That's how f-f-f-fucked
up they are.”
Today, Jenna's psychiatric problems remain severe. On the day
we met, her wrist was bandaged, as she had recently tried to cut her¬
self, and thus the medications haven't been much help in that re¬
gard, either. But, she says, "I don't see anything different happening.
I have brought up the issue of taking me off the meds billions of
times."
Before we left our sidewalk table, Chris provided me with the de¬
tails of Jenna's daily cocktail: two antidepressants, an antipsychotic,
a benzodiazepine, a Parkinson's medication, and three others for
physical problems likely related to the psychiatric drugs. Later, I cal¬
culated that even if generics were prescribed whenever possible, she
was consuming $800 of medication monthly, or roughly $10,000
annually. She had been on psychiatric medications for twelve years,
which meant that her Rx bill for psychiatric medications might al¬
ready have surpassed $100,000, and given that she will likely
remain on the drugs for the rest of her life, this bill could eventually
end up well north of $200,000.
"They are making a lot of money on me," Jenna says. "But these
drugs have ruined my life. They make me all f-f-f-fucked up.”
316
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
A Business Triumph
Jenna's perspective on her care was not an unusual one. Many of
the people on SSI and SSDI that I interviewed spoke about how they
felt they were caught in the tangles of a business enterprise. "There
is a reason we are called consumers" was a comment I heard several
times. They are right of course that the pharmaceutical companies
want to build a market for their products, and when we view the
psychopharmacology "revolution" through this prism, as a business
enterprise first and a medical enterprise second, we can easily see
why psychiatry and the pharmaceutical companies tell the stories
they do, and why the studies detailing poor long-term outcomes
have been kept from the public. That information would derail a
business enterprise that brings profits to so many.
As we saw earlier, during the late 1970s psychiatry was worried
about its survival. The public viewed its therapies as "low in effi¬
cacy," and sales of psychiatric drugs were in decline. Then, in what
might be called a "rebranding” effort, psychiatry published DSM-
III and began telling the public that mental disorders were "real"
diseases, just like diabetes and cancer, and that their drugs were
chemical antidotes to those diseases, just like "insulin for diabetes."
That story, while it may have been false in kind, created a powerful
conceptual framework for selling psychiatric medications of all
types. Everyone could understand the chemical-imbalance metaphor,
and once the public came to understand that notion, it became
relatively simple for pharmaceutical companies and their story¬
telling allies to build markets for psychiatric drugs of various types.
They ran "educational" campaigns to make the public more
"aware” of the various disorders the drugs were approved to treat,
and, at the same time, they expanded the diagnostic boundaries of
mental disorders.
After Prozac was introduced, NIMH's DART campaign informed
the public that depression regularly went "undiagnosed and un¬
treated.” Upjohn partnered with the APA to tell the public that
"panic disorder" was a common affliction. In 1990, the NIMH
launched its "Decade of the Brain,” telling the public that 20
TALLYING UP THE PROFITS
317
percent of Americans suffered from mental disorders (and thus
might be in need of psychiatric medications). Soon psychiatric groups
and others were promoting "screening programs," which from a
business perspective are best described as customer-recruitment ef¬
forts. NAMI, for its part, understood that its "educational" efforts
served a commercial end, writing in a 2000 document filed with the
government that "providers, health plans, and pharmaceutical com¬
panies want to grow their markets and to increase their share of the
market. . . . NAMI will cooperate with these entities to grow the
market by making persons aware of the issues involving severe
brain disorders." 2
The APA is in charge of defining diagnostic categories in our so¬
ciety, and DSM-IV, an 886-page tome published in 1994, listed 297
disorders, 32 more than DSM-III. New and expanded diagnoses in¬
vite more people into the psychiatric drugstore, and one of the best
examples of this type of market-building occurred in 1998, when
GlaxoSmithKline got the FDA to approve Paxil for "social anxiety
disorder." In the past, this might have been perceived as a character
trait (shyness), but GlaxoSmithKline hired a PR firm, Cohn & Wolfe,
to promote awareness of this newly recognized "disease,” and soon
newspapers and television shows were telling of how SAD afflicted
13 percent of the American population, making it "the third most
common psychiatric disorder in the United States, after depression
and alcoholism." Those afflicted with this illness, the public learned,
were in some ways biologically "allergic to people." 3
Diagnostic changes lay behind the bipolar boom, too. In DSM-III
(1980), bipolar illness was identified for the first time (the old manic-
depressive cohort was splintered into different groups), and then
psychiatry steadily loosened the diagnostic boundaries for this ill¬
ness, such that today the field talks about bipolar I, bipolar II, and a
"bipolarity intermediate between bipolar disorder and normality."
This once rare disease is now said to afflict 1 to 2 percent of the adult
population, and if the "intermediate” bipolar folk are counted, 6
percent. As this diagnostic expansion happened, pharmaceutical
companies and their allies mounted their usual "educational" cam¬
paigns. Abbott Laboratories and NAMI teamed up to promote
a "Bipolar Awareness Day"; in 2002, Eli Lilly joined with the De-
31 8
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
pression and Bipolar Support Alliance to launch a new online desti¬
nation, bipolarawareness.com . Today many websites offer visitors a
quick question-and-answer test to see if they have this illness.
Naturally, pharmaceutical companies want to sell their drugs to
people of all ages, and they built the pediatric market for psy¬
chotropics step by step. First, in the 1980s, the prescribing of stimu¬
lants to "hyperactive” children took off. Next, in the early 1990s,
psychiatrists began regularly prescribing SSRIs to teenagers. But
that meant prepubertal children weren't being prescribed these new
wonder drugs, and in 1997, the Wall Street Journal reported that
the manufacturers of SSRIs were "taking aim at a controversial new
market: children." The drug firms were "preparing their medica¬
tions in easy-to-swallow forms that will be more palatable to even
the youngest tykes," the newspaper said, with Eli Lilly formulating
a "minty liquid” Prozac for the tots to down.* The New York
Times, in its coverage of this initiative, explained quite clearly what
was driving it: "The adult market for [SSRIs] has become satu¬
rated. . . . The companies are looking for expanded markets." 5
Psychiatry quickly provided a medical cover for this marketing ef¬
fort, with the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychia¬
try announcing that 5 percent of all children in the United States
were clinically depressed. "Many of these young patients now are
inadequately treated, experts say, often leading to long-term emo¬
tional and behavioral problems, drug abuse, or even suicide," the
Wall Street Journal reported.*
The creation of the "juvenile bipolar” market was a bit more
complicated. Prior to the 1990s, psychiatry thought that bipolar ill¬
ness simply didn't occur in prepubertal children, or was extremely
rare. But children and teenagers prescribed stimulants and anti¬
depressants often suffered manic episodes, and thus pediatricians
and psychiatrists began to see more youth with "bipolar" symptoms.
At the same time, once Janssen and Eli Lilly brought their atypical
antipsychotics to market, they were looking for a way to sell those
drugs to children, and during the mid-1990s, Joseph Biederman at
Massachusetts General Hospital in Boston provided the diagnostic
framework that made that possible. In 2009, while being deposed in
a legal case, he explained his handiwork.
TALLYING UP THE PROFITS
319
All psychiatric diagnoses, he said, "are subjective in children and
in adults." As such, he and his colleagues decided that children who
in the past had been seen as having pronounced behavioral prob¬
lems should instead be diagnosed with juvenile bipolar illness. "The
conditions that we see in front of us are reconceptualized," Bieder-
man testified. "These children have been called in the past conduct
disorder, oppositional-defiant disorder. It's not that these children
did not exist, they were just under different names." 7 Biederman
and his colleagues decided that "severe irritability" or "affective
storms" would be the telltale signs of juvenile bipolar disorder, and
with this new diagnostic criteria in hand, they announced in 1996
that many children diagnosed with ADHD were in fact "bipolar" or
else "comorbid” for both illnesses. 8 The illness was a "much more
common condition than was previously thought,” often appearing
when children were only four or five years old, Biederman said.*’
Soon parents in the United States were reading newspaper articles
about this newly recognized illness and buying The Bipolar Child, a
book published by Random House in 2000. Child psychiatrists,
meanwhile, began treating it with atypical antipsychotics.
That was the marketing machinery that lured more and more
Americans into the psychiatric drugstore. As new drugs were
brought to market, disease "awareness" campaigns were conducted
and diagnostic categories were expanded. Now, once a business gets
a customer into its store, it wants to keep that customer and get that
customer to buy multiple products, and that's when the psychiatric
"drug trap” kicks in.
The "broken brain" story helps with customer retention, of
course, for if a person suffers a "chemical imbalance," then it makes
sense that he or she will have to take the medication to correct it in¬
definitely, like "insulin for diabetes.” But more important, the drugs
create chemical imbalances in the brain, and this helps turn a first¬
time customer into a long-term user, and often into a buyer of mul¬
tiple drugs. The patient's brain adapts to the first drug, and that
* During Biederman's February 26, 2009, deposition, an attorney asked him
about his rank at Harvard Medical School. "Full professor," he replied.
"What's above that?" the attorney asked. "God," Biederman replied.
3 20 • ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
makes it difficult to go off the medication. The store's exit door is
hard to squeeze through, so to speak. At the same time, since
psychiatric drugs perturb normal function, they regularly cause
physical and psychiatric problems, and this greases the path to
polypharmacy. The hyperactive child is put on a stimulant that
rouses him during the day; at night he needs a sleeping pill to go to
sleep. An atypical causes people to feel depressed and lethargic; psy¬
chiatrists may prescribe an antidepressant to treat that problem.
Conversely, an antidepressant may stir a bout of mania; in that case
an atypical antipsychotic may be prescribed to tamp down the
mania. The first drug triggers a need for a second, and so on.
Eli Lilly even capitalized on this fact when it brought Zyprexa to
market. As it well knew, Prozac and other SSRIs could trigger manic
episodes, and so it instructed its sales representatives to tell psychia¬
trists that Zyprexa "is a great mood stabilizer, especially for pa¬
tients whose symptoms were aggravated by an SSRI." 10 In essence,
Eli Lilly was telling doctors to prescribe its second drug to fix the
psychiatric problems caused by its first one. We can also see this
cascading effect operating at a societal level. The SSRIs came to
market and suddenly bipolar patients were cropping up every¬
where, and then this new group of patients provided a market for
the atypicals.*
All of this has produced a growth industry of impressive dimen¬
sions. In 1985, outpatient sales of antidepressants and antipsychotics
in the United States amounted to $503 million.” Twenty-three years
later, U.S. sales of antidepressants and antipsychotics reached $24.2
billion, nearly a fiftyfold increase. Antipsychotics —a class of drugs
previously seen as extremely problematic in kind, useful only in se¬
verely ill patients —were the top revenue-producing class of drugs in
2008, ahead even of the cholesterol-lowering agents. 12 Total sales of
all psychotropic drugs in 2008 topped $40 billion. Today —and this
* In a similar vein, pharmaceutical companies have pounced on the fact that
many of the drugs initially prescribed for a target symptom don't work very
well. "Two out of three people treated for depression still have symptoms," a
Bristol-Myers Squibb commercial informed television viewers in 2009. The
solution? Add an atypical antipsychotic. Ability, to the mix.
TALLYING UP THE PROFITS
• 321
shows how crowded the drugstore has become —one in every eight
Americans takes a psychiatric drug on a regular basis. 13
The Money Tree
Naturally, this flourishing business enterprise generates great per¬
sonal wealth for executives at pharmaceutical companies, and
money also flows in fairly copious amounts to the academic psychi¬
atrists who tout their drugs. Indeed, the profits from this enterprise
trickle down to nearly all of those who tell the "psychiatric drugs
are good” story to our society. To get a sense of the amounts in¬
volved, we can look at the money that the different players in this
enterprise receive.
We can start with Eli Lilly, as it serves as a good example of the
profits that go to a drug company's shareholders and its executives.
Eli Lilly
In 1987, Eli Lilly's pharmaceutical division generated $2.3 billion in
revenues. The company did not have a central nervous system drug
of any importance, as its three bestselling drugs were an oral anti¬
biotic, a cardiovascular drug, and an insulin product. Eli Lilly began
selling Prozac in 1988, and four years later it became the company's
first billion-dollar drug. In 1996, Eli Lilly brought Zyprexa to mar¬
ket, and it became a billion-dollar drug in 1998. By 2000, these two
drugs accounted for nearly half of the company's revenues of $10.8
billion.
Prozac soon after lost its patent protection, and thus the wealth¬
generating effects of the two drugs can best be assessed across a
thirteen-year period, from 1987 to 2000. During this time, Eli
Lilly's value on Wall Street rose from $10 billion to $90 billion. An
investor who bought $10,000 of Eli Lilly stock in 1987 would have
seen that investment grow to $96,850 in 2000, and along the way
the investor would have received an additional $9,720 in dividends.
At the same time, Eli Lilly's executives and employees, in addition
322
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
to their salaries and bonuses, netted around $3.1 billion from the
stock options they exercised.' 4
Academic psychiatrists
The pharmaceutical companies would not have been able to build a
$40 billion market for psychiatric drugs without the help of psychi¬
atrists at academic medical centers. The public looks to doctors for
information about illnesses and how best to treat them, and so it
was the academic psychiatrists —paid by drug companies to serve as
consultants, on advisory boards, and as speakers —who in essence
acted as the salesmen for this enterprise. The pharmaceutical com¬
panies, in their internal memos, accurately call these psychiatrists
"key opinion leaders," or KOLs for short.
Thanks to a 2008 investigation by Iowa senator Charles Grass-
ley, the public got a glimpse of the amount of money that the phar¬
maceutical companies pay their KOLs. The academic psychiatrists
regularly receive federal NIH grants, and as such, they are required
to inform their institutions how much they receive from pharma¬
ceutical companies, with the medical schools expected to manage
the "conflict of interest” whenever this amount exceeds $10,000
annually. Grassley investigated the records of twenty or so academic
psychiatrists, and he found that not only were many making much
more than $10,000 a year, they were also hiding this fact from their
schools.
Here are a few examples of the money paid to KOLs in psy¬
chiatry.
• From 2000 to 2007, Charles Nemeroff, chair of the psychia¬
try department at Emory Medical School, earned at least
$2.8 million as a speaker and consultant for drug firms, with
GlaxoSmithKline alone paying him $960,000 to promote
Paxil and Wellbutrin. He is a coauthor of the APA's Textbook
of Psychopharmacology, which is the bestselling textbook in
the field. He also wrote a trade book about psychiatric med¬
ications, The Peace of Mind Prescription, for the general
public. He has served on the editorial boards of more than
sixty medical journals and for a time was editor in chief of
TALLYING UP THE PROFITS
323
Neuropsychopharmacology. In December of 2008, he re¬
signed as chair of Emory's psychiatry department, as he had
failed to inform Emory of his drug-company paychecks. 15
Zachary Stowe, also a professor of psychiatry at Emory, re¬
ceived $250,000 from GlaxoSmithKline in 2007 and 2008,
partly to promote the use of Paxil by breast-feeding women.
Emory "reprimanded” him for failing to properly disclose
these payments to the school. 16
Another member of GlaxoSmithKline's speaker bureau was
Frederick Goodwin, a former director of the NIMH. The
company paid him $1.2 million from 2000 to 2008, mostly
to promote the use of mood stabilizers for bipolar illness
(GlaxoSmithKline sells Lamictal, which is a mood stabilizer).
Goodwin is the coauthor of Manic-Depressive Illness, the au¬
thoritative textbook on this disorder, and he also was the
longtime host of a popular radio show, The Infinite Mind,
which was carried on NPR stations nationwide. His show
regularly featured discussions of psychiatric medications,
with Goodwin, in a program broadcast on September 20,
2005, warning that if children with bipolar disorder were not
treated, they could suffer brain damage. Goodwin has been a
speaker or consultant for a number of other pharmaceutical
companies; the $1.2 million was what he received from Glaxo¬
SmithKline alone. In an interview with the New York Times,
Goodwin explained that he was only "doing what every
other expert in the field does." 17
From 2000 to 2005, Karen Wagner, director of child and
adolescent psychiatry at the University of Texas, collected
more than $160,000 from GlaxoSmithKline. She promoted
the use of Paxil in children, and did so in part by coauthoring
an article that falsely reported the results of a pediatric trial
of the drug.
In a confidential document written in October 1998, Glaxo¬
SmithKline concluded that in the study, Paxil "failed to
demonstrate a statistically significant difference from placebo
on the primary efficacy measures." 15 In addition, five of the
ninety-three adolescents treated with Paxil in the study
324
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
suffered "extreme lability," versus one in the placebo group,
which meant that the drug markedly elevated the suicide risk.
The study had shown Paxil to be neither safe nor effective in
adolescents. However, in a 2001 article published in the Jour¬
nal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychi¬
atry, Wagner and twenty-one other leading child psychiatrists
stated that the study proved that Paxil is "generally well tol¬
erated and effective for major depression in adolescents." 19
They did not discuss the sharply elevated suicide risk, writing
instead that only one child treated with Paxil had suffered
a serious adverse event, with that child developing a "head¬
ache.” New York State attorney general Eliot Spitzer sued
GlaxoSmithKline for fraudulently marketing Paxil to adoles¬
cents, a case which was settled out of court.
All told, Wagner has been a consultant or advisor to at
least seventeen pharmaceutical companies. The $160,000
was the amount she received from GlaxoSmithKline alone;
she told her school that she had received $600. 20
From 1999 to 2006, Jeffrey Bostic, a psychiatrist at Massa¬
chusetts General Hospital in Boston, collected more than
$750,000 from Forest Laboratories to promote the prescrib¬
ing of Celexa and Lexapro to children and adolescents. He
gave more than 350 talks in twenty-eight states during this
period, leading one Forest sales rep to boast: "Dr. Bostic is
the man when it comes to child psych!" 21 In March of 2009,
the federal government charged Forest with illegally market¬
ing these drugs to this patient population, alleging that it had
paid "kickbacks, including lavish meals and cash payments
disguised as grants and consulting fees, to induce doctors to
prescribe the drugs.” Dr. Bostic, the federal government said,
served as the company's "star spokesman" in this scheme.
The federal government noted that the company had also
failed to disclose the results of a study of these drugs in
children that had produced "negative” results.
From 2003 to 2007, Melissa DelBello, an associate professor
of psychiatry at the University of Cincinnati, received at least
$418,000 from AstraZeneca. She promoted the prescribing
TALLYING UP THE PROFITS
325
of atypical antipsychotics, including AstraZeneca's Seroquel,
to juvenile bipolar patients. DelBello worked for at least
seven other pharmaceutical companies. "Trust me, I don't
take much” from drug firms, she told the New York Times
prior to Grassley's report. 22
• Joseph Biederman may have been the KOL who did the most
to help the pharmaceutical industry build a market for its
products. To a large extent, juvenile bipolar illness was his
creation, and children and adolescents so diagnosed are often
treated with drug cocktails. Pharmaceutical companies paid
him $1.6 million for his various services from 2000 to 2007,
with much of this money coming from Janssen, the division
of Johnson &c Johnson that sells Risperdal. 23
Biederman also got the company to pay $2 million from
2002 to 2005 to create the Johnson &C Johnson Center for Pe¬
diatric Psychopathology at Massachusetts General Hospi¬
tal. 24 In a 2002 report on the center, he candidly set forth its
aims. The center, he explained, was a "strategic collabora¬
tion" that would "move forward the commercial goals of
J&J.” He and his colleagues would develop screening tests
for juvenile bipolar illness, and then teach CME (continuing
medical education) courses to train pediatricians and psychi¬
atrists to use them. Their research, Biederman wrote, would
"alert physicians to the existence of a large group of children
who might benefit from treatment with Risperdal.” In addi¬
tion, the center would promote the understanding that "pedi¬
atric mania evolves into what some have called mixed or
atypical mania in adulthood, [which] will provide further
support for the chronic use of Risperdal from childhood
through adulthood.'" 1 ' In the past, Biederman noted, he had
successfully led the medical profession to conceive of ADHD
* Biederman here is describing the course of children who are diagnosed with
bipolar illness and then medicated; those children do tend to become chroni¬
cally ill in the way he describes. But there is no medical literature showing that
there is a disease that takes this course in unmedicated children.
326
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
as a ''chronic” illness, and now he would do the same for
bipolar disorder. 25
Biederman has been the Pied Piper of pediatric bipolar
illness in our society, and in this document we can see the
future that he was laying out for the children given this diag¬
nosis. They were being groomed to be lifelong consumers of
psychiatric medications. The child diagnosed with bipolar
disorder would be put on an antipsychotic, and that child
could then be expected to become chronically ill, and that
would require a lifetime of "aggressive treatments such as
Risperdal.” Perhaps there is a file tucked away in a drug com¬
pany cabinet that estimates the expected lifetime consump¬
tion of psychiatric medications by a child diagnosed with
bipolar illness; all we can say, in this book, is that every child
so diagnosed is, from a business standpoint, a new Jenna.
The next tier down
The KOLs are the "stars" of the field, as they are the ones who "in¬
fluence” their peers at a national and international level, but the
pharmaceutical companies also pay physicians to promote their
drugs on a more local basis, with these speakers giving talks at din¬
ners or to other physicians in their offices. Pay typically starts at
$750 per event and rises from there. Two states, Minnesota and
Vermont, have passed "sunshine” laws that disclose these pay¬
ments, and their reports provide insight into the flow of money to
these doctors.
In 2006, pharmaceutical firms gave $2.1 million to Minnesota
psychiatrists, up from $1.4 million in 2005. From 2002 to 2006, the
recipients of drug-company money included seven past presidents of
the Minnesota Psychiatric Society and seventeen faculty psychia¬
trists at the University of Minnesota. John Simon, who was a mem¬
ber of the state's Medicaid formulary committee, which guides the
state's spending on drugs, was the top-paid psychiatrist, earning
$570,000 for his services to drug companies. All told, 187 of 571
psychiatrists in Minnesota received pharmaceutical money for some
reason or other during this period, a percentage that was "much
TALLYING UP THE PROFITS
327
higher” than for any other specialty. Their collective take was $7.4
million. 26
Vermont's reports tell much the same story. Of all the medical
specialties, psychiatry received the most money from the drug
companies.
The community psychiatrist
The pharmaceutical companies also provide freebies to community
psychiatrists. They invite them to free dinners where the KOLs and
the local experts give their talks, and their sales representatives reg¬
ularly come to their offices bearing small gifts. "Gave Dr. Child a
cupcake sized peanut butter cup,” wrote an Eli Lilly sales represen¬
tative, in a 2002 report to her boss. "He was kind of tickled.” Or as
she said after another sales call: "Doc and staff loved the goodie
box I brought in, filled with useful items for their new clinic." 22
These are very small bribes, but even a small gift helps build a social
bond. A California group surveyed the drug firms and found that
they do set a limit on the freebies that are offered to a psychiatrist
each year; GlaxoSmithKline's was $2,500 per physician, while Eli
Lilly's was $3,000. There are many companies that sell psychiatric
drugs, and thus any psychiatrist who welcomes sales reps can enjoy
a regular supply of goodies.
NAMI and all the rest
Eli Lilly now posts on the Web a list of the "educational" and "phil¬
anthropy" grants it makes, and this provides a peek at the money
going to patient advocacy groups and various educational organiza¬
tions. In the first quarter of 2009 alone, Eli Lilly gave $551,000 to
NAMI and its local chapters, $465,000 to the National Mental
Health Association, $130,000 to CHADD (an ADHD patient-
advocacy group), and $69,250 to the American Foundation for Sui¬
cide Prevention. The company gave more than $1 million to various
educational organizations, including $279,533 to the Antidote Ed¬
ucation Company, which runs a "continuing medical education"
course. Those are the amounts from one pharmaceutical company
328
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
for three months; any full accounting of the flow of money to
patient advocacy groups and educational organizations would re¬
quire adding up the grants from all of the makers of psychiatric
drugs. 28
We All Pay the Tab
According to a 2009 report by the federal Agency for Healthcare
Research and Quality, spending on mental health services is now
rising at a faster rate than for any other medical category. 28 In 2008,
the United States spent about $170 billion on mental health ser¬
vices, which is twice the amount it spent in 2001, and this spending
is projected to increase to $280 billion in 2015. The public,
primarily through its Medicaid and Medicare programs, picks up
close to 60 percent of the nation's spending on mental health
services. 30
Such is the story of the psychiatric drug business. The industry
has excelled at expanding the market for its drugs, and this gener¬
ates a great deal of wealth for many. However, this enterprise has
depended on the telling of a false story to the American public, and
the hiding of results that reveal the poor long-term outcomes with
this paradigm of care. It also is exacting a horrible toll on our soci¬
ety. The number of people disabled by mental illness during the past
twenty years has soared, and now this epidemic has spread to our
children. Indeed, millions of children and adolescents are being
groomed to be lifelong users of these drugs.
From a societal and moral point of view, that is a bottom-line
that cries out for change.
Part Five
Solutions
16
Blueprints for Reform
I think it is time for another hunger strike.
- VINCE BOEHM, 2009
On July 28, 2003, six "psychiatric survivors" associated with
MindFreedom International, a patients' rights organization, an¬
nounced a "fast for freedom.” David Oaks, Vince Boehm, and four
others sent a letter to the American Psychiatric Association, NAMI,
and the U.S. Office of the Surgeon General stating that they would
begin a hunger strike unless one of the organizations provided "sci¬
entifically valid evidence" that the various stories they told to the
public about mental disorders were true. Among other things, the
MindFreedom group asked for evidence proving that major mental
illness are "biologically-based brain diseases," and for evidence that
"any psychiatric drug can correct a chemical imbalance" in the
brain. The MindFreedom Six had put together a scientific panel to
review the organizations' replies, an advisory group that included
Loren Mosher, and they demanded that if the APA and the others
couldn't provide such scientific evidence, "you publicly admit to
media, government officials, and the general public that you are un¬
able to do so.” 1
Here's how the APA responded: "The answers to your questions
are widely available in the scientific literature, and have been for
years,” wrote medical director James Scully. He suggested that they
read the U.S. Surgeon General's 1999 Mental Health report, or an
332
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
APA textbook coedited by Nancy Andreasen. "This is a 'user-
friendly' textbook for persons just being introduced to the field of
psychiatry," he explained. 2
Only the uneducated, it seemed, asked such dumb questions. But
Scully had failed to list any citations, and so the six "psychiatric sur¬
vivors" began their hunger strike, and when their scientific advisors
reviewed the texts that Scully had referred them to, they found no
citations there, either. Instead, the texts all grudgingly acknowl¬
edged the same bottom line. "The precise causes [etiology] of men¬
tal disorders are not known," U.S. surgeon general Satcher
confessed in his 1999 report. MindFreedom's scientific panel, in its
August 22 reply to Scully, observed that the strikers had asked
"clear questions about the science of psychiatry," and yet the APA
had brushed them off. "By not giving specific answers to the specific
questions posed by the hunger strikers, you appear to be affirming
the very reason for the hunger strike." 3
The APA never answered that letter. Instead, after the Mind-
Freedom group broke their fast (several started to have health prob¬
lems), it issued a press release, stating that the APA, NAMI, and the
rest of the psychiatric community "will not be distracted by those
who would deny that serious mental disorders are real medical con¬
ditions that can be diagnosed accurately and treated effectively." 4
But it was clear to all observers who had won this battle. The strik¬
ers had called the APA's bluff, and the APA had come up empty. It
hadn't come up with a single citation that supported the "brain dis¬
ease” story it told to the public. The MindFreedom Six, along with
their scientific panel, then issued a clarion call for help:
We urge members of the public, journalists, advocates, and of¬
ficials reading this exchange to ask for straightforward an¬
swers to our questions from the APA. We also ask Congress to
investigate the mass deception that the "diagnosis and treat¬
ment of mental disorders,” as promoted by bodies such as the
APA and its powerful allies, represents in America today. 5
The strike, noted MindFreedom executive director David Oaks,
stirred articles in the Washington Post and the Los Angeles Times.
BLUEPRINTS FOR REFORM
333
"The purpose of the strike was to educate the public. It was about
empowering the public and getting them to talk about these issues,
which affect everyone. It was about challenging the corporate bully¬
ing of the [public] mind." 6
Lessons from a Hunger Strike
When I first thought about writing a "solutions" chapter, I figured
that I would simply report on programs, both in the United States
and abroad, that involve using psychiatric medications in a selec¬
tive, cautious manner (or not at all), and are producing good re¬
sults. But then I thought of the hunger strike, and I realized that the
MindFreedom group had precisely identified the bigger issue at
hand.
The real question regarding psychiatric medications is this: When
and how should they be used? The drugs may alleviate symptoms
over the short term, and there are some people who may stabilize
well over the long term on them, and so clearly there is a place for
the drugs in psychiatry's toolbox. However, a "best" use paradigm
of care would require psychiatry, NAMI, and the rest of the psychi¬
atric establishment to think about the medications in a scientifically
honest way and to speak honestly about them to the public. Psychi¬
atry would have to acknowledge that the biological causes of men¬
tal disorders remain unknown. It would have to admit that the
drugs, rather than fix chemical imbalances in the brain, perturb the
normal functioning of neurotransmitter pathways. It would have to
stop hiding the results of long-term studies that reveal that the med¬
ications are worsening long-term outcomes. If psychiatry did that, it
could figure out how to use the medications judiciously and wisely,
and everyone in our society would understand the need for alterna¬
tive therapies that don't rely on the medications or at least minimize
their use.
In his 1992 book How to Become a Schizophrenic, John
Modrow —who had been so diagnosed —wrote the following:
"How then are we to help 'schizophrenics'? The answer is simple:
334
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Stop the lies!" 7 In essence, that’s what the MindFreedom Six were
demanding, and as their advisory panel observed, this is a perfectly
rational request. And that, I think, sums up the challenge that we, as
a society, now face. How do we break up the psychiatry-and-drug-
company partnership that, as we have seen, regularly does lie to us?
How can we insist that our society's mental health system be driven
by honest science rather than by a partnership that is constantly
seeking to expand the market for psychiatric drugs?
There is no easy answer to that question. But clearly our society
needs to have a conversation about it, and so I thought that the rest
of this "solutions" chapter should be devoted to interviews and in¬
vestigations of alternative programs that could help make that
conversation a fruitful one.
An Artful Form of Care
David Healy is a professor of psychiatry at Cardiff University and
tends to psychiatric patients at the District General Hospital in
North Wales, where he has been since 1990. His office is located a
few feet from a closed ward, and naturally, he regularly prescribes
psychiatric medications. Indeed, although he has come to be per¬
ceived by many in psychiatry as a "maverick,” he recoils at that
word. In the 1980s, he notes, he researched serotonin reuptake in
depressed patients. He participated as a clinical investigator in a
trial of Paxil. He has authored more than a dozen books and pub¬
lished more than 120 articles, with much of his writing focusing on
the history of psychiatry and the psychopharmacology era. His CV
speaks of a psychiatrist and historian who, until he began writing
about problems with the SSRIs, was embraced by the psychiatric es¬
tablishment. "I don't think I've changed much at all," he said. "I
think the mainstream has left me." 8
His thoughts on how psychiatric drugs should be used (and what
they really do) have been deeply influenced both by his writings on
the history of psychiatry and by a study he has conducted that
BLUEPRINTS FOR REFORM
335
compares outcomes of the mentally ill in North Wales a century ago
with outcomes in the region today. The population hasn't changed
in this period, with around 240,000 in the area, and whereas all the
seriously mentally were treated at the North Wales Asylum in Den¬
bigh a century ago, today all psychiatric patients needing to be hos¬
pitalized are treated at the District General Hospital in Bangor. By
poring over records of the two institutions, Healy and his assistants
have been able to determine the number of people who were treated
back then and the number treated today, as well as the frequency of
their hospitalizations.
The common belief, Healy notes, is that the old asylums were
bulging with lunatics. Yet from 1894 to 1896, there were only
forty-five people per year admitted to the North Wales Asylum (for
mental problems). Furthermore, as long as the patients didn't suc¬
cumb to tuberculosis or some other infectious disease, they regu¬
larly got better over the course of three months to a year and went
home. Fifty percent were discharged as "recovered” and another 30
percent as "relieved.” In addition, the overwhelming majority of
patients admitted for a first episode of illness were discharged and
never again rehospitalized, and that was true even for psychotic pa¬
tients. This latter group averaged only 1.23 hospitalizations in a
ten-year period (that number includes the initial hospitalization).
Today, the assumption is that patients fare much better than they
used to thanks to psychiatric medications. However, in 1996, there
were 522 people admitted to the psychiatric ward at the District
General Hospital in Bangor —nearly twelve times the number ad¬
mitted to the Denbigh asylum a century earlier. Seventy-six percent
of the 522 patients had been there before, part of a large group of
patients in North Wales that cycle regularly through the hospital.
Although the patients spent a shorter time in the hospital than they
did in 1896, only 36 percent were discharged as recovered. Finally,
the patients admitted for a first episode of psychosis in the 1990s
averaged 3.96 hospitalizations over the course of ten years —more
than three times the number a century earlier. Patients today are
clearly more chronically ill than they were a century ago, with mod¬
ern treatments apparently having set up a "revolving door." 9
336
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
"We have been surprised by how poor the five-year outcomes are
today,” Healy said. "Each time we look at the current data, at the
first batch of five-year outcomes [for a particular diagnostic group],
we think, 'God, that can't be the case.' "
Their study sends a fairly clear message about how and when
psychiatric medications should be used. "A bunch of people used to
recover,” Healy explained, but if you immediately put all patients
on medications, you run the risk of "giving them a chronic problem
they wouldn't have had in the old days.” Healy now tries to "watch
and wait" before giving psychiatric drugs to first-episode patients,
as he wants to see if this type of natural recovery can take hold. "I
try to use the drugs cautiously in reasonably low doses, and I tell the
patient, 'If the drug isn't doing what we want it to do, we are going
to halt it,' " he said. If psychiatrists listened to their patients about
how the drugs were affecting them, he concluded, "we would have
only a few patients on them long-term.”
There it is: a simple prescription for using the medications judi¬
ciously. Once a physician realizes that many people who experience
a bout of psychosis or a deep depression can recover naturally, and
that long-term use of psychotropics is associated with increased
chronicity, then it becomes apparent that the drugs need to be used
in a selective, limited manner. Healy has seen this approach work
with his patients, many of whom initially insist that they need the
drugs. "I say to them, 'We can do more harm than good,' " he said.
"They don't realize just how much harm we can do."
Healing the "In-Between"
For a long time, western Lapland in Finland had one of the highest
rates of schizophrenia in Europe. There are about 70,000 people
who live there, and during the 1970s and early 1980s, twenty-five
or so new cases of schizophrenia appeared each year —an incidence
rate double and even triple the norm for other parts of Finland and
the rest of Europe. Furthermore, those patients regularly became
BLUEPRINTS FOR REFORM
337
chronically ill. But today the long-term outcomes of psychotic pa¬
tients in western Lapland are the best in the Western world, and this
region now sees very few new cases of schizophrenia.
This is a medical success that has been decades in the making,
and it began in 1969 when Yrjo Alanen, a Finnish psychiatrist who
had psychoanalytic training, arrived at the psychiatric hospital in
Turku, a port city in southwest Finland. At that time, few psychia¬
trists in the country thought that psychotherapy could help schizo¬
phrenics. However, Alanen believed that the hallucinations and
paranoid utterances of schizophrenic patients, when carefully
parsed, told meaningful stories. Hospital psychiatrists, nurses, and
staff needed to listen to the patients. "It's almost impossible for any¬
one meeting with these patients' families to not understand that
they have difficulties in life," Alanen explained in an interview at
the psychiatric hospital in Turku. They are "not ready" to be adults,
and "we can help with this development." 10
Over the next fifteen years, Alanen and a handful of other Turku
psychiatrists, most notably Jukka Aaltonen and Viljo Rakkolainen,
created what they called the "need-adapted” treatment of psychotic
patients. Since psychotic patients are a very heterogeneous group,
they decided that treatment needed to be "case specific.” Some first-
episode patients would need to be hospitalized, and others would
not. Some would benefit from low doses of psychiatric medications
(either benzos or neuroleptics), and others would not. Most impor¬
tant, the Turku psychiatrists settled on group family therapy —of a
particularly collaborative type —as the core treatment. Psychiatrists,
psychologists, nurses, and others trained in family therapy all
served on two- and three-member "psychosis teams," which would
meet regularly with the patient and his or her family. Decisions
about the patient's treatment were made jointly at those meetings.
In those sessions, the therapists did not worry about getting the
patient's psychotic symptoms to abate. Instead, they focused the
conversation on the patient’s past successes and achievements, with
the thought that this would help strengthen his or her "grip on life.”
The hope, said Rakkolainen, "is that they haven't lost the idea that
they can be like others." The patient might also receive individual
338 •
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
psychotherapy to help this process along, and eventually the patient
would be encouraged to construct a new "self-narrative” for going
forward, the patient imagining a future where he or she was inte¬
grated into society, rather than isolated from it. "With the biological
conception of psychosis, you can't see the past achievements" or the
future possibilities, Aaltonen said.
During the 1970s and 1980s, the outcomes for psychotic patients
in the Turku system steadily improved. Many chronic patients
were discharged from the hospital, and a study of first-episode
schizophrenic-type patients treated from 1983 to 1984 found that
61 percent were asymptomatic at the end of five years and only 18
percent were on disability. This was a very good result, and from
1981 to 1987, Alanen coordinated the Finnish National Schizo¬
phrenia Project, which determined that the need-adapted model of
care developed in Turku could be successfully introduced into other
cities. Two decades after Alanen and the others had initiated their
Turku project, Finland had decided that psychotherapy could in¬
deed help psychotic patients.
However, the question of the best use of antipsychotics remained,
and in 1992, Finland mounted a study of first-episode patients to
answer it. All six sites in the study provided the newly diagnosed
patients with need-adapted treatment, but in three of the centers,
the patients were not put on antipsychotics during the first three
weeks (benzos could be used), with drug therapy initiated only if the
patient hadn't improved during this period. At the end of two years,
43 percent of the patients from the three "experimental” sites had
never been exposed to neuroleptics, and overall outcomes at the ex¬
perimental sites were "somewhat better" than they were at the
centers where nearly all of the patients had been exposed to the
drugs. Furthermore, among the patients at the three experimental
sites, those who had never been exposed to neuroleptics had the
best outcomes. 11
"I would advise case-specific use [of the drugs],” Rakkolainen
said. "Try without antipsychotics. You can treat them better with¬
out medication. They become more interactive. They become them¬
selves." Added Aaltonen: "If you can postpone medication, that's
important."
BLUEPRINTS FOR REFORM
339
It might seem that Finnish psychiatry, given the outcomes of the
study, would have then embraced —on a national level —this "no
immediate use of neuroleptics" model of care. Instead, Alanen and
the other creators of need-adapted treatment retired, and during the
1990s, Finland’s treatment of psychosis became much more "bio¬
logically" oriented. Even in Turku, first-episode patients are regu¬
larly treated with antipsychotics today, and Finnish guidelines now
call for the patients to be kept on the drugs for at least five years
after a first episode. "I am a bit disappointed,” Alanen confessed at
the end of our interview.
Fortunately, one of the three "experimental" sites in the 1992-
1993 study did take the results to heart. And that site was Tornio, in
western Lapland.
On my way north to Tornio, I stopped to interview Jaakko Seikkula,
a professor of psychotherapy at the University of Jyvaskyla. In
addition to working at Keropudas Hospital in Tornio for nearly
twenty years, he has been the lead author on several studies docu¬
menting the extraordinary outcomes of psychotic patients in west¬
ern Lapland.
The transformation of care at Keropudas Hospital, from a sys¬
tem in which patients were regularly hospitalized and medicated to
one in which patients are infrequently hospitalized and only occa¬
sionally medicated, began in 1984, when Rakkolainen visited and
spoke about need-adapted treatment. The Keropudas staff, Seikkula
recalled, immediately sensed that holding "open meetings," where
every participant freely shared his or her thoughts, would provide
psychotic patients with a very different experience from conven¬
tional psychotherapy. "The language we use when the patient is sit¬
ting with us is so different from the language we use when we
(therapists] are by ourselves and discussing the patient,” he said.
"We do not use the same words, and we have to listen more to the
patient's ideas about what is going on, and listen more to the
family."
Eventually, Seikkula and others in Tornio developed what they
called open-dialogue therapy, which was a subtle variation of
340
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Turku's need-adapted model. As was the case in Turku, patient
outcomes in western Lapland improved during the 1980s, and then
Tornio was selected to be one of the three experimental sites in
Finland's 1992-93 first-episode study. Tornio enrolled thirty-four
patients, and at the end of two years, twenty-five had never been
exposed to neuroleptics. Nearly all of the never-medicated patients
in the national study (twenty-five of twenty-nine) had actually
come from this one site, and thus it was only here that hospital
staff observed the longer-term course of unmedicated psychosis.
And they found that while recovery from psychosis often pro¬
ceeds at a fairly slow pace, it regularly happens. The patients,
Seikkula said, "went back to their work, to their studies, to their
families." 12
Encouraged by the results, Keropudas Hospital immediately
started a new study, charting the long-term outcomes of all first-
episode psychotic patients in western Lapland from 1992 through
1997. At the end of five years, 79 percent of the patients were
asymptomatic and 80 percent were working, in school, or looking
for work. Only 20 percent were on government disability. Two-thirds
Five-Year Outcomes for First-Episode Psychotic Patients in Finnish
Western Lapland Treated with Open-Dialogue Therapy
Patients (N=75)
Schizophrenia (N=30)
Other psychotic disorders (N=45)
Antipsychotic use
Never exposed to antipsychotics
67 %
Occasional use during five years
33% /
Ongoing use at end of five years
20%
Psychotic symptoms
Never relapsed during five years
67%
Asymptomatic at five-year follow-up
79%
Functional outcomes at five years
Working or in school
73%
Unemployed
7%
On disability
20%
Source: Seikkula, J. "Five-year experience of first-episode nonaffective psychosis in o pe n-d i al og u e
approach." Psychotherapy Research 16 (2006): 21 4-28.
BLUEPRINTS FOR REFORM
341
of the patients had never been exposed to antipsychotic medication,
and only 20 percent took the drugs regularly. 13 Western Lapland
had discovered a successful formula for helping psychotic patients
recover, with its policy of no immediate use of neuroleptics in first-
episode patients critical to that success, as it provided an "escape
valve” for those who could recover naturally.
"I am confident of this idea," Seikkula said. "There are patients
who may be living in a quite peculiar way, and they may have psy¬
chotic ideas, but they still can hang on to an active life. But if they
are medicated, because of the sedative action of the drugs, they lose
this 'grip on life,' and that is so important. They become passive,
and they no longer take care of themselves."
Today, the psychiatric facilities in western Lapland consist of the
fifty-five-bed Keropudas Hospital, which is located on the outskirts
of Tornio, and five mental-health outpatient clinics. There are around
one hundred mental-health professionals in the district (psychia¬
trists, psychologists, nurses, and social workers), and most have
completed a nine-hundred-hour, three-year course in family ther¬
apy. Many of the staff—including psychiatrist Birgitta Alakare and
psychologists Tapio Salo and Kauko Haarakangas —have been there
for decades, and today open-dialogue therapy is a well-polished
form of care.
Their conception of psychosis is quite distinct in kind, as it
doesn't really fit into either the biological or psychological category.
Instead, they believe that psychosis arises from severely frayed so¬
cial relationships. "Psychosis does not live in the head. It lives in the
in-between of family members, and the in-between of people," Salo
explained. "It is in the relationship, and the one who is psychotic
makes the bad condition visible. He or she 'wears the symptoms'
and has the burden to carry them."“
With most of the staff in the district trained in family therapy, the
system is able to respond quickly to a psychotic crisis. Whoever is
first contacted—by a parent, a patient seeking help, or perhaps a
school administrator —is responsible for organizing a meeting
within twenty-four hours, with the family and patient deciding
342
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
where the meeting should be held. The patient’s home is the
preferred place. There must be at least two staff members present at
the meeting, and preferably three, and this becomes a "team” that
ideally will stay together during the patient's treatment. Everyone
goes to that first meeting aware that they "know nothing," said
nurse Mia Kurtti. Their job is to promote an "open dialogue” in
which everybody's thoughts can become known, with the family
members (and friends) viewed as coworkers. "We are specialists in
saying that we are not specialists," Birgitta Alakare said.
The therapists consider themselves guests in the patient's home,
and if an agitated patient runs off to his or her room, they simply
ask the patient to leave the door open, so that he or she can listen to
the conversation. "They hear voices, we meet them, and we try to
reassure them," Salo said. "They are psychotic, but they are not
violent at all." Indeed, most patients want to tell their story, and
when they speak of hallucinations and paranoid thoughts, the ther¬
apists simply listen and reflect upon what they've heard. "I think
[psychotic symptoms] are very interesting,” Kurtti said. "What's
the difference between voices and thoughts? We are having a con¬
versation."
No mention is made of antipsychotics in the first few meetings. If
the patient begins sleeping better and bathing regularly, and in other
ways begins to reestablish societal connections, the therapists know
that the patient's "grip on life" is strengthening, and that medica¬
tion will not be needed. Now and then, Alakare may prescribe a
benzodiazepine to help a person sleep or to dampen the patient's
anxiety, and eventually she may prescribe a neuroleptic at a low
dose. "Usually I suggest that the patient use it for some months,”
Alakare said. "But when the problems go away, after six months or
a year, or maybe even after three years, we try to stop the medica¬
tion."
From the outset, the therapists strive to give both the patient and
family a sense of hope. "The message that we give is that we can
manage this crisis. We have experience that people can get better,
and we have trust in this kind of possibility,” Alakare said. They
have found that it can take a long time —two, three, or even five
years —for a patient to recover. Although a patient's psychotic
BLUEPRINTS FOR REFORM
343
symptoms may abate fairly quickly, they are focused on the patient's
"grip on life" and repairing his or her relationship to society, and
that is a much bigger task. The team continues to meet with the pa¬
tient and family, and as this process unfolds, teachers and prospec¬
tive employers are asked to attend too. "It's about restoring social
connections," Salo said. "The 'in-between' starts working again,
with family and with friends.”
Over the past seventeen years, open-dialogue therapy has trans¬
formed "the picture of the psychotic population" in western Lap-
land. Since the 1992-93 study, not a single first-episode psychotic
patient has ended up chronically hospitalized. Spending on psychi¬
atric services in the region dropped 33 percent from the 1980s to
the 1990s, and today the district's per-capita spending on mental-
health services is the lowest among all health districts in Finland.
Recovery rates have stayed high: From 2002 to 2006, Tornio par¬
ticipated in a multinational study by Nordic countries of first-
episode psychosis, and at the end of two years, 84 percent of the
patients had returned to work or school, and only 20 percent were
taking antipsychotics. Most remarkable of all, schizophrenia is now
disappearing from the region. Families in western Lapland have be¬
come so comfortable with this gentle form of care that they call the
hospital (or one of the outpatient clinics) at the first sign of psy¬
chosis in a loved one, with the result being that today first-episode
patients typically have had psychotic symptoms for less than a
month and, with treatment initiated at this early stage, very few go
on to develop schizophrenia (the diagnosis is made after a patient
has been psychotic for longer than six months). Only two or three
new cases of schizophrenia appear each year in western Lapland, a
90 percent drop since the early 1980s.”
Tornio's success has drawn the attention of mental-health-care
providers in other European countries, and during the past twenty
years, two or three other groups in Europe have reported that the
combination of psychosocial care and limited use of neuroleptics
has produced good outcomes. 16 "This really happened," Seikkula
said. "It's not just a theory."
344
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
On my way back to Helsinki, I kept puzzling over this one thought:
Why are the group meetings in Tornio so therapeutic? Given the
outcomes literature for neuroleptics, I could understand why selec¬
tive use of the drugs had proven to be so helpful. But why did open-
dialogue therapy help psychotic patients heal?
During my two days in Tornio, I sat in on three group sessions,
and although I don't speak Finnish, it was nevertheless possible to
gain a sense of the meetings' emotional tenor and to observe how
the conversation flowed. Everyone sat in a circle, in a very relaxed
and calm manner, and before anyone spoke, there often was a split-
second moment of silence, as if whoever was going to speak next
was gathering his or her thoughts. Now and then someone laughed,
and I couldn't identify a time when anyone was interrupted, and yet
no individual seemed to go on speaking too long, either. The con¬
versation seemed graced by gentility and humility, and both family
members and patients listened with rapt attention whenever the
therapists turned and spoke to each other. "We like to know what
they really think, rather than just have them give us advice," said
the parents in one of the meetings.
But that was the sum of it. It was all a bit mystifying, and even
the staff at Keropudas Hospital hadn't really been able to explain
why these conversations were so therapeutic. "The severe symp¬
toms begin to pass,” Salo said with a shrug. "We don't know how it
happens, but [open-dialogue therapy] must be doing something,
because it works.”
A Natural Antidepressant
In the early 1800s, Americans regularly turned to a book written by
Scottish physician William Buchan for medical advice. In Domestic
Medicine, Buchan prescribed this pithy remedy for melancholy:
The patient ought to take as much exercise in the open air as
he can bear ... A plan of this kind, with a strict attention to
BLUEPRINTS FOR REFORM
345
diet, is a much more rational method of cure, than confining
the patient within doors, and plying him with medicines. 17
Two centuries later, British medical authorities rediscovered the
wisdom of Buchan's advice. In 2004, the National Institute for
Health and Clinical Excellence, which acts as an advisory panel to
the country's National Health Service, decided that "antidepres¬
sants are not recommended for the initial treament of mild depres¬
sion, because the risk-benefit ratio is poor." Instead, physicians
should try non-drug alternatives and advise "patients of all ages
with mild depression of the benefits of following a structured and
supervised exercise programme." 18
Today, general practitioners in the UK may write a prescription
for exercise. "The evidence base for exercise as a treatment for de¬
pression is quite good," said Andrew McCulloch, executive director
of the Mental Health Foundation, a London-based charity that has
been promoting this alternative. "It also reduces anxiety. It's good
for self-esteem, control of obesity, et cetera. It has a broad-spectrum
effect." 19
In terms of its short-term efficacy as an antidepressant, studies
have shown that exercise produces a "substantial improvement"
within six weeks, that its effect size is "large," and that 70 percent
of all depressed patients respond to an exercise program. "These
success rates are quite remarkable," German investigators wrote in
2008. = “ In addition, over time, exercise produces a multitude of
"side benefits.” It enhances cardiovascular function, increases mus¬
cle strength, lowers blood pressure, and improves cognitive func¬
tion. People sleep better, they function better sexually, and they also
tend to become more socially engaged.
A 2000 study by James Blumenthal at Duke University also
revealed that it is unwise to combine exercise with drug therapy.
He randomized 156 older depressed patients into three groups —
exercise, Zoloft, and Zoloft plus exercise —and at the end of sixteen
weeks, those treated with exercise alone were doing as well as those
in the other two groups. 21 Blumenthal then tracked the patients for
another six months, with the patients free to choose whatever
346
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
The Long-Term Benefit of Exercise for Depression
Treatment During
First Four Months
Percentage of Patients
in Remission at End of
Four Months
Percentage of Remitted
Patients Who Relapsed
in Six-Month Follow-up
Percentage of
Patients Depressed
at End of Ten
Months
Zoloft alone
6 9%
3 8%
5 2 %
Zoloft plus
6 6 %
3 1 %
5 5 %
exercise therapy
Exercise therapy
6 0%
8%
3 0%
alone
In this study by Duke researchers, older patients with depression were treated for 16 weeks in
one ofthree ways, and then followed for another six months. Patients treated with exercise alone
had the lowest rates of relapse during the foMowing six months, and as a group, they were much
less likely to be suffering from depressive symptoms at the end often months. Source: Babyak,
M. "Exercise treatment for major depression." Psychosomatic Medicine 62 (2000): 6S S -3 8.1 00- 1 1 .
treatment they wanted during this period, and at the end the pa¬
tients treated initially with exercise alone were doing the best. Only
8 percent of those who had been well at the end of sixteen weeks
had relapsed during the follow-up, and by the end of ten months 70
percent of the exercise-only group were asymptomatic. In the two
Zoloft-exposed groups, more than 30 percent of the patients who
had been well at the end of sixteen weeks relapsed, and fewer than
50 percent were asymptomatic by the study's end. The "Zoloft plus
exercise" group had fared no better than the "Zoloft alone” pa¬
tients, which suggested that exposure to Zoloft negated the benefits
of exercise. "This was an unexpected finding, because it was as¬
sumed that combining exercise with medication would have, if
anything, an additive effect,” Blumenthal wrote. 22
In 2003, when Britain's Mental Health Foundation launched its
exercise-for-depression campaign, it took advantage of the fact that
general practitioners in Britain were already "prescribing” exercise
to patients with diabetes, hypertension, osteoporosis, and other
physical conditions. The delivery of this medical care requires
physicians to collaborate with local YMCAs, gyms, and recre¬
ational facilities, with these collaborations known as "exercise-
referral schemes," and thus the foundation simply needed to get the
GPs to start prescribing exercise to their depressed patients too.
BLUEPRINTS FOR REFORM
347
Today, more than 20 percent of the GPs in the UK prescribe exercise
to depressed patients with some frequency, which is four times the
percentage who did in 2004.
A "prescription" for exercise typically provides the patient with
twenty-four weeks of treatment. An exercise professional assesses
the patient's fitness and develops an appropriate "activity plan,"
with the patient then given discounted or free access to the collabo¬
rating YMCA or gym. Patients work out on exercise machines,
swim, and take various exercise classes. In addition, many exercise-
referral schemes provide access to "green gyms.” The outdoor pro¬
grams may involve group walks, outdoor stretching classes, and
volunteer environmental work (managing local woodlands, improv¬
ing footpaths, creating community gardens, etc.). Throughout the
six months of treatment, the exercise professional monitors the
patient’s health and progress.
As might be expected, patients have found ”exercise-on-
prescription" treatment to be quite helpful. They told the Mental
Health Foundation that exercise allowed them to "take control of
their recovery" and to stop thinking of themselves as "victims" of a
disease. Their confidence and self-esteem increased; they felt calmer
and more energetic. Treatment was now focused on their "health,"
rather than on their "illness."
"The fathers of medicine wouldn't be surprised about what we
are doing," McCulloch said. "They would say, 'Hasn't science gone
any further? Diet and exercise? This is what is new?' If they could
travel in a time machine, they would think we were mad, because
people have been saying these things for thousands of years."
These Kids Are Awesome
The children who end up living at Seneca Center in San Leandro,
California, have come to the last stop for severely disturbed youth
in the northern part of the state. The children, five to thirteen years
old, have usually cycled through several foster homes and have had
multiple hospitalizations, and their behavior has been so difficult
348
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
that there are no foster homes or hospitals left that want to see them
again. In bureaucratic terms, they are "level-14" kids, which is the
designation given to the most troubled kids in California, but since
these children have flunked out of other level-14 facilities, they are
better described as ”level-14-plus-plus” youth. Counties pay Seneca
Center $15,000 a month to shelter a child and, not surprisingly,
when the children arrive at the center, most are on heavy-duty drug
cocktails. "They are so drugged up that they are asleep most of the
day," said Kim Wayne, director of the residence program. 23
And then their lives begin to change dramatically.
I visited one of Seneca Center's two residences for younger chil¬
dren in the summer of 2009, and when I entered, here is what I saw:
a young African American girl wearing headphones singing along to
a Jordin Sparks song; a second slightly older African American girl
sitting at the kitchen table, leafing through photos of their recent
group trip to Disneyland; and two African American boys at the
table goofing around with each other and racing to see who could
drink a glass of water the fastest. A Caucasian girl sat on the couch,
and the sixth resident of the house, I later learned, was off at a
swimming lesson. Within a short while, the girl with the head¬
phones was singing a cappella (and quite well), and the girl huddled
over the photo album had started calling me Bob Marley, appar¬
ently because I knew who Jordin Sparks was. Now and then, one of
the children erupted into laughter.
"The kids are so grateful to be off the drugs," said therapist Kari
Sundstrom. "Their personalities come back. They are people again."
The two Seneca Center homes may be the last residential facilities
in the United States where severely troubled children under county
or state control are treated without psychiatric drugs. Indeed, in
most child-psychiatry circles, this would be considered unethical.
'T've been told, 'If your child had a disease, would you deny your
child medication that helped him get better?' " said Seneca Center
founder and CEO Ken Berrick. And even within the agency, which
has a staff of around seven hundred and provides a variety of ser¬
vices to two thousand troubled children and youth in northern
California, the residence program is an anomaly.
When the center opened in 1985, Berrick and others sought to
BLUEPRINTS FOR REFORM
349
hire consulting psychiatrists who would use psychiatric medications
in a "conservative” fashion and never for purposes of "behavioral
control.” Some used the drugs more than others, and then there was
Tony Stanton, whom the agency hired in 1987 to oversee the chil¬
dren's residential program. In the 1960s, he had trained at Langley
Porter Hospital in San Francisco, which at the time emphasized the
"importance of environment" to a child's mental health. Stanton's
own "attachment theory" convinced him of the importance of emo¬
tional relationships to a child's well-being. Then, in the late 1970s,
while he was in charge of a psychiatric ward for children at a
county hospital, he assigned a "mentor” to every child. The chil¬
dren weren't medicated, and he saw a number of them become at¬
tached to their mentors and "blossom.”
"That experience allowed me to see this therapeutic principle in
action," Stanton said. "You just can't organize yourself without a
connection to another human being, and you can't make that con¬
nection if you embalm yourself with drugs.”
When a child enters Seneca Center's residential program, Stanton
does not ask "what's wrong” with the child, but rather "what hap¬
pened to them.” He gets the department of social services, schools,
and other agencies to send him all of the records they have on the
child, and then he spends eight to ten hours constructing a "life
chart.” As might be expected, the charts regularly tell of children
who have been sexually abused, physically abused, and horribly
neglected. But Stanton also tracks their medication history and how
their behavior may have changed after they were put on a particular
drug, and given that the children who arrive at Seneca Center are se¬
riously disturbed, these medical histories regularly tell of psychiatric
care that has worsened their behavior. "I'll have people say, 'We
want to try the child on Risperdal now,’ and I'll say, 'Let’s take a
look at the chart and see what happened before. I don't think it will
be helpful,' " Stanton said.
The children regularly arrive at the center on drug cocktails, and
thus it can take a month or two to withdraw the medications. Often
the children, having been repeatedly told that they need the drugs,
are nervous about this process —"One kid told me 'What do you
mean you are taking me off my meds? I'll destroy your program,' "
350
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
Stanton said—and often they do become more aggressive for a time.
Staff may have to use "physical restraints" more frequently (they
have been trained to hold the kids in "safe” ways). However, these
behavioral problems usually begin to abate and by the end of the
withdrawal process, the child has "come alive.”
"It's wonderful," Kim Wayne said. "Most times when the kids
come in, they can't keep their heads up, they are lethargic, they are
just a blank and there is minimal engagement. You just can't get
through to them. But when they come off their meds, you can en¬
gage them and you get to see who they are. You get a sense of their
personality, their sense of humor, and what kinds of things they like
to do. You may have to use physical restraints for a time, but to me,
it's worth it."
Once they are off meds, the children begin to think of themselves
in a new way. They see that they can control their own behavior,
and this gives them a sense of "agency," Stanton said. The Seneca
Center uses behavior-modification techniques to promote this self-
control, with the children constantly having to abide by a well-
defined set of rules. They have to ask permission to go to the
bathroom and enter bedrooms, and if they don't comply with
the rules, they may be sent to a "time-out" or lose a privilege. But
the staff tries to focus on reinforcing positive behaviors, offering
words of praise and rewarding the kids in various ways. The chil¬
dren are required to keep their rooms clean and perform a daily
chore, and at times they will help prepare the evening meal.
"The question of feeling in charge of yourself and being responsi¬
ble for yourself is the central issue in their lives," Stanton said.
"They may only partially get there while they are with us, but when
we are really successful, we see them develop this sense of 'Oh, I can
do this; I want to be in control of myself and my own life.' They see
themselves as having that power.”
Even more important, once the children are off the medications
they are better able to form emotional bonds with the staff, and the
staff with them. They have known rejection all their lives, and they
need to form relationships that nurture a belief that they are worthy
of being loved, and when that happens, their "internal narrative”
can switch from "I'm a bad kid” to "I'm a good kid.”
BLUEPRINTS FOR REFORM
351
"They come in thinking, T'm crazy, you are going to hate me,
you are going to get rid of me, I'm going to be the worst kid you
have ever seen,” said therapist Julie Kim. "But then they become
willing to form [emotional] attachments, and that’s such an amaz¬
ing thing. You can see the power of a relationship to change a kid,
and even the kids who seem the toughest when they come in here,
who don't make any progress at first, eventually do."
Although Kim and others can tell anecdotal stories of children
discharged from the residence program who have returned to ordi¬
nary schools and done well, the center has not done a long-term
follow-up of the children that have gone through their residence
program. The only statistical information the center has to show
that its residence program works is this: 225 children lived at its res¬
idences from 1995 to 2006, and nearly all were discharged to lower-
level group homes or to a foster home or to their biological families.
Their time at Seneca Center at least turned their lives in a new di¬
rection. And yet, it is difficult to be optimistic that their lives con¬
tinue down that path. Their emotional and behavioral problems do
not completely go away, and so many of the discharged children —
and perhaps most —are remedicated. They return to a world where
that is the norm. Their time at Seneca Center may primarily provide
them with a temporary oasis from a society prone to asking "what's
wrong with them," and thus, if we want to assess whether the no¬
medication policy of the center's residence program is providing the
children with a "benefit," instead of looking to the future, perhaps
we should focus on the present and look at what it is like for the
children to have this opportunity to "come alive” for a time and
fully feel the world.
I spent two days at the center, and there were three children in
particular I had a chance to interact with. One was a twelve-year-
old boy I'll call Steve. When he'd arrived at Seneca Center a year
earlier, he'd been so filled with suicidal and self-destructive habits
that doctors thought he had suffered brain damage from all of his
head-banging episodes. Since then he’d become very attached to
Stacy, one of the male staff at his house, and during our interview,
he flopped down into a chair, grinned, and immediately took over
the conversation. "I hate taking medicine. It is real boring being on
352
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
drugs,” he said, and then he began telling us about migratory tur¬
tles, a raccoon that had been poking around their house, a trip to
McDonald’s with Stacy, and what people needed to do to prepare
for an earthquake. All of that was prelude to a story about a comic
book he wanted to write, titled The Adventures of Sam Dune and
Rock, which featured numerous "good and evil" characters, includ¬
ing one who needed to take drugs to keep from going mad. Steve
held center stage for at least an hour, and afterward he happily in¬
formed Stacy that the interview had been "cold, real cold,” which
of course meant that he had enjoyed himself immensely.
I'll call the two African American girls I met in the Los Reyes
house Layla (the a cappella singer) and Takeesha. Their "life charts"
both told of nightmarish pasts, and that was particularly true
for Takeesha. When she'd arrived at the Seneca Center in 2006, at
age seven, she was described as delusional, guarded, suspicious, un¬
cooperative, and very sedated. After we spent thirty minutes or so at
the kitchen table, talking about American Idol and the trip they had
taken to Disneyland, Takeesha asked if we could go outside and
play catch with a football. We did that for a while, and then Takee¬
sha got permission to ride her bike in the street, but only if she
promised to go only a few houses away in either direction, and sud¬
denly she came to a screeching halt in the driveway. "I'm going to
Burger King. What do you want?” she announced. Seconds later she
proudly returned holding an imaginary bag filled with a Whopper,
French fries, and a Coke, which I paid for with an equally imagi¬
nary five-dollar bill, asking if she would please make change. When
it came time to say good-bye, Layla asked for a hug, and then
Takeesha —having scurried into her bedroom to find something-
held out what appeared to be a package of gum, except for the fact
that the piece sticking out was clearly metallic in kind.
"It’s just gum!" she squealed when I felt the slight buzz.
The next day I sat in on their class. I spoke briefly with the teacher
and several of the aides, and they all said the same thing. "These kids
are awesome! We could drug the kids into submission, but for what
purpose? I love this place!” I was there with Tony Stanton, and after
a while it became evident that our presence was causing a dilemma
for both Layla and Takeesha. They were supposed to be paying at-
BLUEPRINTS FOR REFORM
353
tention to the teacher, and they knew that if they didn't, they would
be sent to time-out (there was a steady march of children to the time¬
out corner), and yet both were clearly intent on making contact with
us. We were sitting by the sink, and at last both girls decided they
just had to wash their hands. As Layla went back to her seat, she
couldn't resist giving us a high-five, even though this was a breach of
class protocol. Meanwhile, as Takeesha passed by my chair, she
whispered, "Bob Marley, what are you doing here?”
At that moment, I couldn't imagine any outcome data of a more
powerful sort.
On the Drawing Board
Psychiatry and the rest of medicine regularly proclaim that treat¬
ments should be "evidence-based." The solutions we've reviewed in
this chapter all meet that standard. David Healy's belief that the
psychiatric medications should be used in a cautious manner, the
open-dialogue program in Tornio, and the prescribing of exercise as
a first-line therapy for mild-to-moderate depression are all rooted in
good science. The same can be said of Tony Stanton's medication-
withdrawal policy. Earlier in the book, we saw that children put on
stimulants, antidepressants, and antipsychotics often worsen over
the long term, and that those who end up on drug cocktails can be
said to be suffering from an iatrogenic illness. The medications can
be viewed as pathological agents, and thus when Tony Stanton
takes the Seneca Center children off the drugs, he is —in essence-
providing treatment for a "disease.” The proof that the treatment
works can be found in the staff's observation that the children
"come alive.”
Given this perspective, it would be helpful if we could identify a
mainstream medication-withdrawal program in adults, one that
arises from research into this process. How quickly should the
drugs be withdrawn? After the drugs are withdrawn, how long does
it take for the brain to "renormalize?" Or does it? Do neuronal
feedback mechanisms reset? Do presynaptic neurons begin releasing
354
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
normal amounts of the neurotransmitter? Do receptor densities re¬
turn to normal? Psychiatry has been using psychotropic medications
for more than fifty years, yet all of these questions basically remain
unanswered. Indeed, people who want to stop taking the drugs have
been mostly left to fend for themselves, sharing information on the
Internet and through their various peer networks.
However, in the fall of 2 009, a major provider of mental-health
services in eastern and central Massachusetts, Advocates, drew up a
plan for a medication-withdrawal study. Advocates provides ser¬
vices to several thousand people with psychiatric difficulties, and in
2008, when it asked its clients for "new ideas,” many put this at the
top of their wish list, said Keith Scott, director of recovery and peer
support services. "A number said, 'Geez, it would be great if there
would be a place where I could try to stop taking my medication
without being threatened with losing my housing or my services and
the relationships that are important to me.' That seemed extremely
reasonable to me." 24
The medical director of Advocates, Chris Gordon, who is an as¬
sistant clinical professor of psychiatry at Harvard Medical School,
said that he hoped to obtain funding from either the state Depart¬
ment of Mental Health or a federal agency. Advocates plans to
provide both medical and social support to patients in the "drug
reduction/elimination" study, and Gordon said that if patients begin
to struggle during the withdrawal process, he'd like to see if they
can be helped through that crisis without restarting the medica¬
tions. He'd like to follow the patients in the program for five years,
so Advocates can get a sense of their long-term outcomes.
This initiative, Gordon said, is being driven in part by the fact
that the mentally ill are now dying twenty-five years earlier than
their peers, and that it is clear that the atypical antipsychotics,
which regularly cause metabolic dysfunction, are contributing to
that early death problem. “We see it all the time. We could name a
terrible list of people we know personally and care about who died
way too young," he said. 25
BLUEPRINTS FOR REFORM
355
The Alaska Project
If I had to identify one person in the United States who was doing
the most to "change the system," I would pick Alaska attorney Jim
Gottstein. A 1978 graduate of Harvard Law School, Gottstein was
hospitalized twice in the 1980s because of bouts of mania, and that
personal experience has inspired a lifelong career of fighting to im¬
prove the plight of the mentally ill in our society.
During the 1980s and 1990s, Gottstein joined other attorneys in
an epic lawsuit by the Alaska Mental Health Association against the
state. In 1956, Congress allowed Alaska's territorial administrators
to set aside one million acres of prime federal land as an asset that
would fund mental-health programs, but in 1978 the state legisla¬
ture redesignated the acreage as "general grant lands," leaving the
mentally ill out in the cold. The state basically "stole” the land,
Gottstein said, and eventually he and other attorneys negotiated a
$1.1 billion settlement. 26 The state gave $200 million and nearly a
million acres of land to a newly created Mental Health Trust Au¬
thority, with the trust allowed to spend this money as it sees fit,
without the legislature's approval.
In 2002, Gottstein founded a non-profit organization, Psych-
Rights, and the first thing that it did was mount a "public informa¬
tion" campaign. PsychRights brought various people to Anchorage
to speak to judges, lawyers, psychiatrists, and the general public
about the outcomes literature for antipsychotics.* Gottstein be¬
lieved that this would provide a foundation for a lawsuit challenging
the state's right to medicate patients forcibly, and for lobbying the
Mental Health Trust Authority to fund a Soteria-like home, where
psychotic patients who didn't want to take neuroleptics could get
help.
"The public opinion is that the meds work, and that if people
weren't crazy, they would know that the drugs are good for them,"
Gottstein said. "But if we can get judges and lawyers to understand
* In the interest of full disclosure, I was one of the speakers at several of those
events.
356
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
that it's not necessarily good for the person and potentially very
harmful, they would tend to honor a person's legal right to refuse
treatment. In the same vein, if the public knew that there are other
non-drug approaches like Soteria that work better, they would sup¬
port alternatives, right?"
State case laws governing the forced treatment of psychiatric pa¬
tients date back to the late 1970s. Although state supreme courts
typically ruled that patients have a right to refuse treatment (in non¬
emergency situations), they nevertheless noted that antipsychotics
were understood to be "a medically sound treatment of mental dis¬
ease," and thus hospitals could apply to a court to sanction forced
treatment. At such hearings, hospitals regularly argue that no com¬
petent person would refuse "medically sound treatment," and thus
courts consistently order patients to be medicated. 27 But in 2003,
Gottstein initiated a forced-drugging lawsuit on behalf of a woman
named Faith Myers, and he put the medication on trial, arguing that
the state could not show that it was in her best medical interest to
take an antipsychotic. He got Loren Mosher and a second psychia¬
trist who knows the outcomes literature well, Grace Jackson, to
serve as his expert witnesses, and he also filed copies of the many re¬
search studies that tell of how neuroleptics can worsen long-term
outcomes.
Having become versed in the scientific literature, the Alaska
Supreme Court gave PsychRights a stunning legal victory in 2006.
"Psychotropic medication can have profound and lasting negative
effects on a patient's mind and body," the court wrote. These drugs
"are known to cause a number of potentially devastating side ef¬
fects.” As such, it ruled in Myers v. Alaska Psychiatric Institute that
a psychiatric patient could be forcibly medicated only if a court "ex¬
pressly finds by clear and convincing evidence that the proposed
treatment is in the patient’s best interest and that no less intrusive
alternative is available." 28 In Alaska case law, antipsychotics are no
longer viewed as treatment that will necessarily help psychotic
people.
In 2004, Gottstein launched an effort to get the Mental Health
Trust Authority to fund a Soteria home in Anchorage, which would
offer psychotic patients the type of care that Loren Mosher's Soteria
BLUEPRINTS FOR REFORM
357
Project did in the 1970s. Once again, he relied on the persuasive
powers of the scientific literature to carry his argument, and in the
summer of 2009, a seven-bedroom Soteria home opened a few miles
south of downtown. The director of the project, Susan Musante,
formerly led a psychiatric rehabilitation program at the University
of New Mexico Mental Health Center; the consulting psychiatrist,
Aron Wolf, is a well-respected figure in Alaskan psychiatry.
"We want to work with younger people who have been on psy¬
chiatric medications for only a short time, and by getting them off
the meds and helping them get better, we hope to keep them from
going down the path of chronic illness," Musante said. "Our expec¬
tation is that people will recover. We expect them to go to work or
to school, to return to age-appropriate behavior. We are here to help
them to dream again and to pursue those dreams. We are not set up
to funnel them onto SSI or SSDI." 29
Gottstein now has his sights set on a legal challenge national in
scope. He has been filing lawsuits that challenge the medicating of
foster children and poor children in Alaska (the poor are covered by
Medicaid), and ultimately he hopes to take one of these cases to the
U.S. Supreme Court. He sees this as a 14th Amendment issue, with
the children being deprived of their liberty without due process of
law. At the heart of any such case would be a scientific question: Are
the foster children being treated with medications that help, or are
they being treated with tranquilizing drugs that cause long-term
harm?
"I analogize it to Broum v. Board of Education," Gottstein said.
"Before that decision, there was widespread acceptance in the
United States that segregation is OK. The Supreme Court had previ¬
ously said that segregation was OK. But then in Brown v. Board of
Education, the court said it wasn't OK, and that really changed
public opinion. Today you can't get anyone to say segregation is
OK. And that’s how I visualize this whole effort."
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
358
We the People
As a society, we put our trust in the medical profession to develop
the best possible clinical care for diseases and ailments of all types.
We expect that the profession will be honest with us as it goes about
this task. And yet, as we look for ways to stem the epidemic of dis¬
abling mental illness that has erupted in this country, we cannot
trust psychiatry, as a profession, to fulfill that responsibility.
For the past twenty-five years, the psychiatric establishment has
told us a false story. It told us that schizophrenia, depression, and
bipolar illness are known to be brain diseases, even though —as the
MindFreedom hunger strike revealed—it can't direct us to any sci¬
entific studies that document this claim. It told us that psychiatric
medications fix chemical imbalances in the brain, even though
decades of research failed to find this to be so. It told us that Prozac
and the other second-generation psychotropics were much better
and safer than the first-generation drugs, even though the clinical
studies had shown no such thing. Most important of all, the psychi¬
atric establishment failed to tell us that the drugs worsen long-term
outcomes.
If psychiatry had been honest with us, the epidemic could have
been curbed long ago. The long-term outcomes would have been
publicized and discussed, and that would have set off societal
alarms. Instead, psychiatry told stories that protected the image of
its drugs, and that storytelling has led to harm done on a grand and
terrible scale. Four million American adults under sixty-five years
old are on SSI or SSDI today because they are disabled by mental ill¬
ness. One in every fifteen young adults (eighteen to twenty-six years
old) is "functionally impaired” by mental illness. Some 250 children
and adolescents are added to the SSI rolls daily because of mental
illness. The numbers are staggering, and still the epidemic-making
machinery rolls on, with two-year-olds in our country now being
"treated" for bipolar illness.
As I noted earlier in this chapter, I believe the MindFreedom Six
showed what must be done if we are going to halt this epidemic. We
need to become informed about the long-term outcomes literature
BLUEPRINTS FOR REFORM
359
reviewed in this book, and then we need to ask the NIMH, NAMI,
the APA, and all those who prescribe the medications to address the
many questions raised by that literature. In other words, we need to
have an honest scientific discussion. We need to talk about what is
truly known about the biology of mental disorders, about what the
drugs actually do, and about how the drugs increase the risk that
people will become chronically ill. If we could have that discussion,
then change surely would follow. Our society would embrace and
promote alternative forms of non-drug care. Physicians would pre¬
scribe the medications in a much more limited, cautious manner. We
would stop putting foster children on heavy-duty cocktails and pre¬
tending that it was medical care. In short, our societal delusion
about a "psychopharmacology" revolution could at last fade away,
and good science could illuminate the path to a much better future.
Epilogue
"Few dare to announce unwelcome truth.
- EDWIN PERCY WHIPPLE (1 866)'
This book tells a history of science that leads readers to a socially
awkward place. Our society believes that psychiatric medications
have led to a "revolutionary” advance in the treatment of mental
disorders, and yet these pages tell of a drug-induced epidemic of dis¬
abling mental illness. Society sees the beautiful woman, and this
book directs the reader's gaze to the old woman. It's never easy to
hold a belief that is out of sync with what the rest of society be¬
lieves, and in this instance, it's particularly difficult because the
story of progress is told by figures of scientific authority —the APA,
the NIMH, and psychiatrists at prestigious universities such as Har¬
vard Medical School. Disagree with the common wisdom on this
topic, and it seems that you must be a card-carrying member of the
flat-Earth society.
But for those readers still wondering about the history told here,
I offer one last story. You can read it and decide for yourself
whether you are now, metaphorically speaking, in the flat-Earth
camp.
After I interviewed Jaakko Seikkula at the University of
Jyvaskyla, he asked me to give a short talk on the history of an-
tipsychotics to a few of his colleagues. Now, Seikkula and others at
Keropudas Hospital in Tornio did not decide to use antipsychotics
362
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
in a selective manner because they thought that the drugs worsened
psychotic symptoms over the long term. Instead, they observed that
many people did better when off them. Thus, when I spoke to
Seikkula's colleagues at the University of Jyvaskyla, this notion that
antipsychotics can make people chronically ill was something they
hadn't thought much about before, and at the end of my talk, one of
the members of our circle asked if this could be true of antidepress¬
ants, too. He and others had been researching the long-term out¬
comes of depressed patients in Finland, and charting too whether
they had used the drugs, and they had been startled by their results.
So, dear readers, ask yourself this: What do you think they
found? And are you surprised?
NOTES
To read many of the source documents listed here, go to madinamerica.com or
robertwhitaker.org
Chapter 1: A Modern Plague
1. J. Bronowski, The Ascent of Man (New York: Little, Brown & Co., 1973), 153.
2. IMS Health, "2007 top therapeutic classes by U.S. sales."
3. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Mental Health: A Report of the
Surgeon General (1999), 3, 68, 78.
4. E. Shorter, A History of Psychiatry (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1997), 255.
5. R. Friedman, "On the Horizon, Personalized Depression Drugs," New York Times ,
June 19, 2007.
6. Boston Globe editorial, "When Kids Need Meds," June 22, 2007.
7. Address by Carolyn Robinowitz, APA Annual Conference, Washington, D.C., May
4, 2008.
8. C. Silverman, The Epidemiology of Depression (Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press,
1968), 139.
9. Social Security Administration, annual statistical reports on the SSDI and SSI pro¬
grams, 1987-2008. To calculate a total disability number for 1987 and 2007, I
added the number of recipients under age sixty-five receiving an SSI payment that
year and the number receiving an SSDI payment due to mental illness, and then I ad¬
justed the total to reflect the fact that one in every six SSDI recipients also receives
an SSI payment. Thus, mathematically speaking: SSI recipients + (.833 x SSDI recip¬
ients) = total number of disabled mentally ill.
10. Silverman, The Epidemiology of Depression, 139.
11. The annual Social Security Administration reports don't provide data on the specific
364
NOTES
diagnoses of SSI and SSDI recipients disabled by mental illness. However, various re¬
searchers have reported that affective disorders now make up 37 percent (or more)
of the disabled mentally ill. See, for instance, J. Cook, "Results of a multi-site clini¬
cal trials study of employment models for mental health consumers," available at:
psvch.uic.edu/EIDP/eidp-3-20-03.pdf .
12. U.S. Government Accountability Office, "Young adults with serious mental illness"
(June 2008).
13. Social Security Administration, annual statistical reports on the SSI program, 1996-
2008; and Social Security Bulletin, Annual Statistical Supplement, 1988-1992.
Chapter 2: Anecdotal Thoughts
1. Adlai Stevenson, speech at University of Wisconsin, October 8, 1952. As cited by L.
Frank, Quotationary (New York: Random House, 2001), 430.
Chapter 3: The Roots of an Epidemic
1. J. Young, The Medical Messiahs (Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press, 1967),
281.
2. Chemical Heritage Foundation, "Paul Ehrlich, Pharmaceutical Achiever," accessed
at chemheritage.org .
3. P. de Kruif, Dr. Ehrlich's Magic Bullet (New York: Pocket Books, 1940), 387.
4. L. Sutherland, Magic Bullets (Boston: Little, Brown and Company, 1956), 127.
5. L. Garrett, The Coming Plague (New York: Penguin, 1995), 49.
6. T. Mahoney, The Merchants of Life (New York: Harper &c Brothers, 1959), 14.
7. "Mind Is Mapped in Cure of Insane," New York Times, May 15, 1937.
8. "Surgery Used on the Soul-Sick," New York Times, June 7, 1937.
9. A. Deutsch, The Shame of the States (New York: Harcourt Brace, 1948), 41.
10. E. Torrey, The Invisible Plague (New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press,
2001), 295.
11. G. Grob, The Mad Among Us (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1994),
189.
12. "Need for Public Education on Psychiatry Is Stressed," New York Times, November
16, 1947.
Chapter 4: Psychiatry's Magic Bullets
1. E. Valenstein, Blaming the Brain (New York: The Free Press, 1998), 38.
2. J. Swazey, Chlorpromazine in Psychiatry (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1974), 78.
3. Ibid, 79.
4. Ibid, 105.
5. Ibid, 134-35.
6. F. Ayd Jr., Discoveries in Biological Psychiatry (Philadelphia: Lippincott, 1970), 160.
7. Symposium proceedings, Chlorpromazine and Mental Health (Philadelphia: Lea
and Fabiger, 1955), 132.
8. Ayd, Discoveries in Biological Psychiatry, 121.
9. M. Smith, Small Comfort (New York: Praeger, 1985), 23.
NOTES
3 65
10. Ibid, 26.
11. Ibid, 72.
12. "TB and Hope," Time, March B, 1952.
13. Valenstein, Blaming the Brain, 38.
14. "TB Drug Is Tried in Mental Cases," New York Times, April 7, 1957.
15. M. Mintz, The Therapeutic Nightmare (Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1965), 166.
16. Ibid, 488.
17. Ibid, 481.
18. Ibid, 59, 62.
19. T. Mahoney, The Merchants of Life (New York: Harper 8c Brothers, 1959), 4, 16.
20. Mintz, The Therapeutic Nightmare, 83.
21. Swazey, Chlorpromazine in Psychiatry, 190.
22. "Wonder Drug of 1954?" Time, June 14, 1954.
23. "Pills for the Mind," Time, March 7, 1955.
24. "Wonder Drugs: New Cures for Mental Ills?" U.S. News and World Report, June
17 , 1955 .
25. "Pills for the Mind," Time, March 7, 1955.
26. "Don't-Give-a-Damn Pills," Time, February 27, 1956.
27. Smith, Small Comfort, 67-69.
28. "To Nirvana with Miltown," Time, July 7, 1958.
29. "Wonder Drug of 1 954?" Time, June 14, 1954.
30. "TB Drug Is Tried in Mental Cases," New York Times, April 7, 1957.
31. Smith, Small Comfort, 70.
32. "Science Notes: Mental Drug Shows Promise," New York Times, April 7, 1957.
33. "Drugs and Depression," New York Times, September 6, 1959.
34. H. Himwich, "Psychopharmacologic drugs," Science 127 (1958): 59-72.
35. Smith, Small Comfort, 110.
36. Ibid, 104.
37. The NIMH Psychopharmacology Service Center Collaborative Study Group, "Phe-
nothiazine treatment in acute schizophrenia," Archives of General Psychiatry 10
( 1964 ): 246 - 61 .
38. Valenstein, Blaming the Brain, 70-79. Also see David Healy, The Creation of
Psychopharmacology (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2002), 106, 205-
206 .
39. J. Schildkraut, "The catecholamine hypothesis of affective disorders," American
Journal of Psychiatry 111 (1965): 509-22.
40. Valenstein, Blaming the Brain, 82.
41. A. Baumeister, "Historical development of the dopamine hypothesis of schizophre¬
nia," Journal of the History of the Neurosciences 1 1 (2002): 265-77.
42. Swazey, Chlorpromazine in Psychiatry, 4.
43. Ibid, 8.
44. Ayd, Discoveries in Biological Psychiatry, 215-16.
45. Ibid, 127.
46. Ibid, 195.
366
NOTES
Chapter 5: The Hunt for Chemical Imbalances
1. T. H. Huxley, Critiques and Addresses (London: Macmillan &c Co., 1873), 229.
2. E. Azmitia, "Awakening the sleeping giant," Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 52
(1991), suppl. 12:4-16.
3. M. Bowers, "Cerebrospinal fluid 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid and homovanillic acid
in psychiatric patients," International Journal of Neuropharmacology 8 (1969):
255-62.
4. R. Papeschi, "Homovanillic and 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid in cerebrospinal fluid of
depressed patients," Archives of General Psychiatry 25 (1971): 354-58.
5. M. Bowers, "Lumbar CSF 5-hydroxyindoleacetic acid and homovanillic acid in af¬
fective syndromes," Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 158 (1974): 325-30.
6. D. L. Davies, "Reserpine in the treatment of anxious and depressed patients,"
Lancet 2 (1955): 117-20.
7. J. Mendels, "Brain biogenic amine depletion and mood," Archives of General Psy¬
chiatry 30 (1974): 447-51.
8. M. Asberg, "Serotonin depression: A biochemical subgroup within the affective dis¬
orders?" Science 191 (1976): 478-80; M. Asberg, "5-HIAA in the cerebrospinal
fluid," Archives of General Psychiatry 33 (1976): 1193-97.
9. H. Nagayama, "Postsynaptic action by four antidepressive drugs in an animal
model of depression," Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 15 (1981): 125-
30. Also see H. Nagayama, "Action of chronically administered antidepressants on
the serotonergic postsynapse in a model of depression," Pharmacology Biochem¬
istry and Behavior 25 (1986): 805-11.
10. J. Maas, "Pretreatment neurotransmitter metabolite levels and response to tricyclic
antidepressant drugs," American Journal of Psychiatry 141 (1984): 1159-71.
11. J. Lacasse, "Serotonin and depression: a disconnect between the advertisements and
the scientific literature," PloS Medicine 2 (2005): 1211-16.
12. C. Ross, Pseudoscience in Biological Psychiatry (New York: John Wiley &c Sons,
1995), 111.
13. Lacasse, "Serotonin and depression."
14. D. Healy, "Ads for SSRI antidepressants are misleading," PloS Medicine news
release, November 2005.
15. I. Creese, "Dopamine receptor binding predicts clinical and pharmacological poten¬
cies of antischizophrenic drugs," Science 192 (1976): 481-83; P. Seeman, "Antipsy¬
chotic drug doses and neuroleptic/dopamine receptors," Nature 261 (1976J: 177-79.
16. "Schizophrenia: Vast effort focuses on four areas," New York Times, November 13,
1979.
17. M. Bowers, "Central dopamine turnover in schizophrenic syndromes," Archives of
General Psychiatry 31 (1974): 50-54.
18. R. Post, "Cerebrospinal fluid amine metabolites in acute schizophrenia," Archives
of General Psychiatry 32 (1975): 1063-68.
19. J. Haracz, "The dopamine hypothesis: an overview of studies with schizophrenic
patients," Schizophrenia Bulletin 8 (1982): 438-58.
NOTES
367
20. T. Lee, "Binding of 3 H-neuroleptics and 3 H-apomorphine in schizophrenic brains,"
Nature 374 (1978): 897-900.
21. D. Burt, "Antischizophrenic drugs: chronic treatment elevates dopamine receptor
binding in brain," Science 196 (1977): 326-27.
22. M. Porceddu, "[ 3 H]SCH 23390 binding sites increase after chronic blockade of d-1
dopamine receptors," European Journal of Pharmacology 118 (1985): 367-70.
23. A. MacKay, "Increased brain dopamine and dopamine receptors in schizophrenia,"
Archives of General Psychiatry 39 (1982): 991-97.
24. J. Kornhuber, " 3 H-spiperone binding sites in post-mortem brains from schizo¬
phrenic patients," Journal of Neural Transmission 75 (19 8 9): 1-10.
25. J. Martinot, "Striatal D 2 dopaminergic receptors assessed with positron emission
tomography and bromospiperone in untreated schizophrenic patients," Ameri¬
can Journal of Psychiatry 147 (1990): 44-50; L. Farde, "D 2 dopamine receptors in
neuroleptic-na'ive schizophrenic patients," Archives of General Psychiatry 47 (1990):
213-19; J. Hietala, "Striatal D 2 dopamine receptor characteristics in neuroleptic-
nai've schizophrenic patients studied with positron emission tomography," Archives
of General Psychiatry 51 (1994): 116-23.
26. P. Deniker, "The neuroleptics: a historical survey," Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica
82, suppl. 358 (1990): 83-87. Also: "From chlorpromazine to tardive dyskinesia,"
Psychiatric Journal of the University of Ottawa 14 (1989): 253-59.
27. J. Kane, "Towards more effective antipsychotic treatment," British Journal of Psy¬
chiatry 165, suppl. 25 (1994): 22-31.
28. E. Nestler and S. Hyman, Molecular Neuropharmacology (New York: McGraw
Hill, 2002), 392.
29. J. Mendels, "Brain biogenic amine depletion and mood," Archives of General Psy¬
chiatry 30 (1974): 447-51.
30. P. Deniker, "The neuroleptics: a historical survey," Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica
82, suppl. 358 (1990): 83-87. Also: "From chlorpromazine to tardive dyskinesia,"
Psychiatric Journal of the University of Ottawa 14 (1989): 253-59.
31. D. Healy, The Creation of Psychopharmacology (Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univer¬
sity Press, 2002), 217.
32. E. Valenstein, Blaming the Brain (New York: The Free Press, 1998), 96.
33. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Mental Health: A Report of the
Surgeon General (1999), 3, 68, 78.
34. J. Glenmullen, Prozac Backlash (New York: Simon & Schuster, 2000), 196.
35. Lacasse, "Serotonin and depression."
36. R. Fuller, "Effect of an uptake inhibitor on serotonin metabolism in rat brain," Life
Sciences 15 (1974): 1161-71.
37. D. Wong, "Subsensitivity of serotonin receptors after long-term treatment of rats
with fluoxetine," Research Communications in Chemical Pathology and Pharma¬
cology 32 (1981): 41-51.
38. J. Wamsley, "Receptor alterations associated with serotonergic agents," Journal of
Clinical Psychiatry 48, suppl. (1987): 19-25.
368
NOTES
39. A. Schatzberg, Textbook of Psychopharmacology (Washington, DC: American
Psychiatric Press, 1995), 8.
40. C. Montigny, "Modification of serotonergic neuron properties by long-term treat¬
ment with serotonin reuptake blockers," Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 51, suppl. B
(1990): 4-8.
41. D. Wong, "Subsensitivity of serotonin receptors after long-term treatment of rats
with fluoxetine," Research Communications in Chemical Pathology and Pharma¬
cology 32 (1981): 41-51.
42. C. Montigny, "Modification of serotonergic neuron properties by long-term treat¬
ment with serotonin reuptake blockers," Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 51, suppl. B
(1990): 4-8.
43. R. Fuller, "Inhibition of serotonin reuptake," Federation Proceedings 36 (1977):
2154-58.
44. B. Jacobs, "Serotonin and behavior," Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 52, suppl.
(1991): 151-62.
45. Schatzberg, Textbook of Psychopharmacology, 619.
46. S. Hyman, "Initiation and adaptation: A paradigm for understanding psychotropic
drug action," American Journal of Psychiatry 153 (1996): 151-61.
Chapter 6: A Paradox Revealed
1. E. Stip, "Happy birthday neuroleptics!" European Psychiatry 17 (2002): 115-19.
2. M. Boyle, "Is schizophrenia what it was?" Journal of the History of Behavioral
Science 26 (1990): 323-33; M. Boyle, Schizophrenia: A Scientific Delusion? (New
York: Routledge, 1990).
3. P. Popenoe, "In the melting pot," Journal of Heredity 14 (1923): 223.
4. J. Cole, editor. Psychopharmacology (Washington, DC: National Academy of Sci¬
ences, 1959), 142.
5. Ibid, 386-87.
6. N. Lehrman, "Follow-up of brief and prolonged psychiatric hospitalization," Com¬
prehensive Psychiatry 2 (1961): 227-40.
7. R. Warner, Recovery from Schizophrenia (Boston: Routledge & Kegan Paul, 1985),
74.
8. L. Epstein, "An approach to the effect of ataraxic drugs on hospital release rates,"
American Journal of Psychiatry 119 (1962): 246-61.
9. C. Silverman, The Epidemiology of Depression (Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press,
1968), 139.
10. J. Swazey, Chlorpromazine in Psychiatry (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1974), 247.
11. Cole, Psychopharmacology, 144,285.
12. Ibid, 285.
13. Ibid, 347.
14. R. Baldessarini, Chemotherapy in Psychiatry (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University
Press, 1977), 29.
15. A. Schatzberg, editor. Textbook of Psychopharmacology (Washington, DC:
American Psychiatric Press, 1995), 624.
NOTES
369
16. P. Gilbert, "Neuroleptic withdrawal in schizophrenic patients," Archives of General
Psychiatry 52 (1995): 173-88.
17. J. Geddes, "Prevention of relapse," New England Journal of Medicine 346 (2002):
56-58.
18. L. Dixon, "Conventional antipsychotic medications for schizophrenia." Schizophre¬
nia Bulletin 21 (1995): 567-77.
19. Stip, "Happy birthday, neuroleptics!"
20. N. Schooler, "One year after discharge," American journal of Psychiatry 123
(1967): 986-95.
21. R. Prien, "Discontinuation of chemotherapy for chronic schizophrenics," Hospital
and Community Psychiatry 22 (1971): 20-23.
22. G. Gardos and J. Cole, "Maintenance antipsychotic therapy: is the cure worse than
the disease?" American journal of Psychiatry 133 (1977): 32-36.
23. G. Gardos and J. Cole, "Withdrawal syndromes associated with antipsychotic
drugs," American Journal of Psychiatry 135 (1978): 1321-24. Also see Gardos and
Cole, "Maintenance antipsychotic therapy."
24. J. Bockoven, "Comparison of two five-year follow-up studies," American Journal of
Psychiatry 132 (1975): 796-801.
25. W. Carpenter, "The treatment of acute schizophrenia without drugs," American
Journal of Psychiatry 134 (1977): 14-20.
26. M. Rappaport, "Are there schizophrenics for whom drugs may be unnecessary or
contraindicated?" International Pharmacopsychiatry 13 (1978): 100-11.
27. S. Mathews, "A non-neuroleptic treatment for schizophrenia," Schizophrenia Bul¬
letin 5 (1979): 322-32.
28. J. Bola, "Treatment of acute psychosis without neuroleptics," Journal of Nervous
and Mental Disease 191 (2003): 219-29.
29. Carpenter, "The treatment of acute schizophrenia."
30. G. Paul, "Maintenance psychotropic drugs in the presence of active treatment pro¬
grams," Archives of General Psychiatry 27 (1972): 106-14.
31. T. Van Putten, "The board and care home: does it deserve a bad press?" Hospital
and Community Psychiatry 30 (1979): 461-64.
32. Gardos and Cole, "Maintenance antipsychotic therapy."
33. P. Deniker, "Are the antipsychotic drugs to be withdrawn?" in C. Shagass, editor.
Biological Psychiatry (New York: Elsevier, 1986), 1-9.
34. G. Chouinard, "Neuroleptic-induced supersensitivity psychosis," American Journal
of Psychiatry 135 (1978): 1409-10.
35. G. Chouinard, "Neuroleptic-induced supersensitivity psychosis: Clinical and phar¬
macologic characteristics," American Journal of Psychiatry 137 (1980): 16-20.
36. G. Chouinard, "Neuroleptic-induced supersensitivity psychosis, the 'Hump Course,'
and tardive dyskinesia," Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology 2 (1982): 143-44.
37. G. Chouinard, "Severe cases of neuroleptic-induced supersensitivity psychosis,"
Schizophrenia Research 5 (1991): 21-33.
38. P. Muller, "Dopaminergic supersensitivity after neuroleptics," Psychopharmacology
60 (1978): 1-11.
370
NOTES
39. L. Martensson, "Should neuroleptic drugs be banned?" Proceedings of the World
Federation of Mental Health Conference in Copenhagen, 1984, accessed via
www.larsmartensson.com, 10/30/08.
40. P. Breggin, Brain Disabling Treatments in Psychiatry (New York: Springer Publish¬
ing Company, 1997), 60.
41. S. Snyder, Drugs and the Brain (New York: Scientific American Library, 1986), 88.
42. C. Harding, "The Vermont longitudinal study of persons with severe mental ill¬
ness," American journal of Psychiatry 144 (1987): 727-34; C. Harding, "The
Vermont longitudinal study of persons with severe mental illness, II," American
Journal of Psychiatry 144 (1987): 727-35.
43. P. McGuire, "New hope for people with schizophrenia," APA Monitor 31 (February
2000 ).
44. C. Harding, "Empirical correction of seven myths about schizophrenia with
implications for treatment," Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica 384, suppl. (1994):
14-16.
45. A. Jablensky, "Schizophrenia: manifestations, incidence and course in different cul¬
tures," Psychological Medicine 20, monograph (1992): 1-95.
46. Ibid. See tables on page 60 for medication usage by individual centers; see table on
page 64 for medication usage by developing and developed countries.
47. K. Hopper, "Revisiting the developed versus developing country distinction in
course and outcome in schizophrenia," Schizophrenia Bulletin 26 (2000): 835-46.
48. J. Wade, "Tardive dyskinesia and cognitive impairment," Biological Psychiatry 22
(1987): 393-95.
49. M. Myslobodsky, "Central determinants of attention and mood disorder in tardive
dyskinesia," Brain and Cognition 23 (1993): 56-70.
50. H. Wisniewski, "Neurofibrillary pathology in brains of elderly schizophrenics
treated with neuroleptics," Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders 8 (1994):
211-27.
51. M. Chakos, "Increase in caudate nuclei volumes of first-episode schizophrenic pa¬
tients taking antipsychotic drugs," American Journal of Psychiatry 151 (1994):
1430-36; A. Madsen, "Neuroleptics in progressive structural brain abnormalities in
psychiatric illness," Lancet 352 (1998): 784-85; R. Gur, "A follow-up of magnetic
resonance imaging study of schizophrenia," Archives of General Psychiatry 55
(1998): 145-52.
52. R. Gur, "Subcortical MRI volumes in neuroleptic-naive and treated patients with
schizophrenia," American Journal of Psychiatry 155 (1998): 1711-17.
53. P. Seeman, "Dopamine supersensitivity correlates with Dz HIGH states, implying
many paths to psychosis," Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 102
(2005): 3513-18.
54. B. Ho, "Progressive structural brain abnormalities and their relationship to clinical
outcome," Archives of General Psychiatry 60 (2003): 585-94.
55. N. Andreasen, "Longitudinal changes in neurocognition during the first decade of
schizophrenia illness," International Congress on Schizophrenia Research (2005):
348.
NOTES
371
56. C. Dreifus, "Using imaging to look at changes in the brain," New York Times,
September 16, 2008.
57. T. McGlashan, "Rationale and parameters for medication-free research in psy¬
chosis," Schizophrenia Bulletin 32 (2006): 300-302.
58. M. Harrow, "Factors involved in outcome and recovery in schizophrenia patients
not on antipsychotic medications," Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 195
(2007): 406-14.
59. National Institute of Mental Health, "The Numbers Count," accessed at
www.nimh.nih.gov on 3/7/2008.
Chapter 7: The Benzo Trap
1. S. Garfield, "Valium's 40th Birthday," Observer, February 2, 2003.
2. E. Shorter, A History of Psychiatry (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1997), 161,
181.
3. A. Tone, The Age of Anxiety (New York: Basic Books, 2009), 15.
4. American Psychiatry Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders (1952), 31.
5. C. Silverman, The Epidemiology of Depression (Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press,
1968), 139.
6. L. Hollister, "Drugs for emotional disorders," Journal of the American Medical As¬
sociation 234 (1975): 942-47.
7. F. Ayd Jr., Discoveries in Biological Psychiatry (Philadelphia: Lippincott, 1970), 127.
8. D. Greenblatt, "Meprobamate: a study of irrational drug use," American Journal of
Psychiatry 127 (1971): 33-39.
9. C. Essig, "Addiction to nonbarbiturate sedative and tranquillizing drugs," Clinical
Pharmacology & Therapeutics 5 (1964): 334-43.
10. "Letdown for Miltown," Time, April 30, 1965.
11. Tone, The Age of Anxiety, 171.
12. M. Smith, Small Comfort (New York: Praeger, 1985), 78.
13. Tone, The Age of Anxiety, 172.
14. G. Cant, "Valiumania," New York Times, February 1, 1976.
15. R. Hughes, The Tranquilizing of America (New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich,
1979), 8.
16. Tone, The Age of Anxiety, 176.
17. Committee on the Review of Medicines, "Systematic review of the benzodi¬
azepines," British Medical Journal 280 (1980): 910-12.
18. Editorial, "Benzodiazepines on trial," British Medical Journal 288 (1984): 1101-12.
19. Smith, Small Comfort, 32.
20. S. Stahl, "Don't ask, don't tell, but benzodiazepines are still the leading treatments
for anxiety disorder," Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 63 (2002): 756-67.
21. IMS Health, "Top therapeutic classes by U.S. dispensed prescriptions," 2006 and
2007 reports.
22. K. Solomon, "Pitfalls and prospects in clinical research on antianxiety drugs,"
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 39 (1978): 823-31.
372
NOTES
23. A. Shapiro, "Diazepam: how much better than placebo?" Journal of Psychiatric Re¬
search 17 (1983): 51-73.
24. C. Gudex, "Adverse effects of benzodiazepines," Social Science & Medicine 33
(1991): 587-96.
25. J. Martin, "Benzodiazepines in generalized anxiety disorder," Journal of Psy¬
chopharmacology 21 (2007): 774-82.
26. Malcolm Lader interview, January 12, 2009.
27. B. Maletzky, "Addiction to diazepam," International Journal of Addictions 11
(1976): 95-115.
28. A. Kales, "Rebound insomnia," Science 201 (1978): 1039-40.
29. H. Petursson, "Withdrawal from long-term benzodiazepine treatment," British
Medical Journal 283 (1981): 643-35.
30. H. Ashton, "Benzodiazepine withdrawal," British Medical Journal 288 (1984):
1135-40.
31. H. Ashton, "Protracted withdrawal syndromes from benzodiazepines," Journal of
Substance Abuse Treatment 9 (1991): 19-28.
32. P. Cowen, "Abstinence symptoms after withdrawal of tranquillising drugs," Lancet
2, 8294(1982): 360-62.
33. H. Ashton, "Benzodiazepine withdrawal," British Medical Journal 288 (1984):
1135-40.
34. H. Ashton, Benzodiazepines: How They Work and How to Withdraw (Newcastle
upon Tyne: University of Newcastle, 2000), 42.
35. H. Ashton, "Protracted withdrawal syndromes from benzodiazepines," Journal of
Substance Abuse Treatment 9 (1991): 19-28.
36. K. Rickels, "Long-term benzodiazepine users 3 years after participation in a discon¬
tinuation program," American Journal of Psychiatry 148 (1991): 757-61.
37. K. Rickels, "Psychomotor performance of long-term benzodiazepine users before,
during, and after benzodiazepine discontinuation," Journal of Clinical Psychophar¬
macology 19 (1999): 107-13.
38. S. Patten, "Self-reported depressive symptoms following treatment with cortico¬
steroids and sedative-hypnotics," International Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine 26
(1995): 15-24.
39. Ashton, Benzodiazepines, 8.
40. A. Pelissolo, "Anxiety and depressive disorders in 4,425 long term benzodiazepine
users in general practice," Encephale 33 (2007): 32-38.
41. Hughes, The Tranquilizing of America, 17.
42. S. Golombok, "Cognitive impairment in long-term benzodiazepine users," Psycho¬
logical Medicine 18 (1988): 365-74.
43. M. Barker, "Cognitive effects of long-term benzodiazepine use," CNS Drugs 18
(2004): 37-48.
44. WHO Review Group, "Use and abuse of benzodiazepines," Bulletin of the World
Health Organization 61 (1983): 551-62.
45. Maletzky, "Addiction to diazepam."
NOTES
373
46. R. Caplan, "Social effects of diazepam use/' Social Science & Medicine 21 (1985):
887-98.
47. H. Ashton, "Tranquillisers," British Journal of Addiction 84 (1989): 541-46.
48. Ashton, Benzodiazepines , 12.
49. Stevan Gressitt interview, January 9, 2009.
50. U.S. Department of Health 6c Human Services, SAMHSA, Mental Health , United
States (2002).
51. Government Accountability Office, Young Adults with Serious Mental Illness , June
2008.
52. R. Vasile, "Results of a naturalistic longitudinal study of benzodiazepine and SSRI
use in the treatment of generalized anxiety disorder and social phobia," Depression
and Anxiety 22 (2005): 59-67.
53. Malcolm Lader interview, January 12, 2009.
Chapter 8: An Episodic Illness Turns Chronic
1. C. Dewa, "Depression in the workplace," A Report to the Ontario Roundtable on
Appropriate Prescribing, November 2001.
2. A. Solomon, The Noonday Demon (New York: Simon & Schuster, 2001), 289.
3. C. Goshen, editor. Documentary History of Psychiatry (New York: Philosophical
Library, 1967), 118-20.
4. Solomon, The Noonday Demon , 286.
5. E. Wolpert, editor, Manic-Depressive Illness (New York: International Universities
Press, 1997), 34.
6. C. Silverman, The Epidemiology of Depression (Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press,
1968), 44, 139. The first-admission and residence data in Silverman's book is for all
manic-depressive patients; the unipolar patients comprised about 75 percent of that
total.
7. Ibid, 79, 142.
8. E Ayd, Recognizing the Depressed Patient (New York: Grune & Stratton, 1961),
13.
9. A. Zis, "Major affective disorder as a recurrent illness," Archives of General Psy¬
chiatry 36 (1979): 835-39.
10. G. Winokur, Manic Depressive Illness (St. Louis: The C.V. Mosby Company, 1969),
19-20.
11. T. Rennie, "Prognosis in manic-depressive psychoses," American Journal of Psychi¬
atry 98 (1941): 801-14. See table on page 811.
12. G. Lundquist, "Prognosis and course in manic-depressive psychoses," Acta Psychi-
atrica Scandinavica , suppl. 35 (1945): 7-93.
13. D. Schuyler, The Depressive Spectrum (New York: Jason Aronson, 1974), 49.
14. J. Cole, "Therapeutic efficacy of antidepressant drugs," Journal of the American
Medical Association 190 (1964): 448-55.
15. N. Kline, "The practical management of depression," Journal of the American
Medical Association 190 (1964): 122-30.
374
NOTES
16. Winokur, Manic Depressive Illness, 19.
17. Schuyler, The Depressive Spectrum, 47.
18. Medical Research Council, "Clinical trial of the treatment of depressive illness,"
British Medical Journal 1 (1965): 881-86.
19. A. Smith, "Studies on the effectiveness of antidepressant drugs," Psychopharmacol¬
ogy Bulletin 5 (1969): 1-53.
20. A. Raskin, "Differential response to chlorpromazine, imipramine, and placebo,"
Archives of General Psychiatry 23 (1970): 164-73.
21. R. Thomson, "Side effects and placebo amplification," British Journal of Psychiatry
140(1982): 64-68.
22. I. Elkin, "NIMH treatment of depression collaborative research program," Archives
of General Psychiatry 47 (1990): 682-88.
23. A. Khan, "Symptom reduction and suicide risk in patients treated with placebo in
antidepressant clinical trials," Archives of General Psychiatry 57 (2000): 311-17.
24. E. Turner, "Selective publication of antidepressant trials and its influence on appar¬
ent efficacy," New England Journal of Medicine 358 (2008): 252-60.
25. I. Kirsch, "Initial severity and antidepressant benefits," PLoS Medicine 5 (2008):
260-68.
26. G. Parker, "Antidepressants on trial," British Journal of Psychiatry 194 (2009): 1-3.
27. C. Barbui, "Effectiveness of paroxetine in the treatment of acute major depression in
adults," Canadian Medical Association Journal 178 (2008): 296-305.
28. J. Ioannidis, "Effectiveness of antidepressants," Philosophy, Ethics, and Humanities
in Medicine 3 (2008): 14.
29. Hypericum Trial Study Group, "Effect of Hypericum perforatum in major depres¬
sive disorder," Journal of the American Medical Association 287 (2002): 1807-14.
30. J.D. Van Scheyen, "Recurrent vital depressions," Psychiatria, Neurologia, Neuro-
chirurgia76 (1973): 93-112.
31. Ibid.
32. R. Mindham, "An evaluation of continuation therapy with tricyclic antidepressants
in depressive illness," Psychological Medicine 3 (1973): 5-17.
33. M. Stein, "Maintenance therapy with amitriptyline," American Journal of Psychia¬
try 137(1980): 370-71.
34. R. Prien, "Drug therapy in the prevention of recurrences in unipolar and bipolar af¬
fective disorders," Archives of General Psychiatry 41 (1984): 1096-1104. See table
6 and figure 2.
35. M. Shea, "Course of depressive symptoms over follow-up," Archives of General
Psychiatry 49 (1992): 782-87.
36. A. Viguera, "Discontinuing antidepressant treatment in major depression," Harvard
Review of Psychiatry 5 (1998): 293-305.
37. P. Haddad, "Antidepressant discontinuation reactions," British Medical Journal 316
(1998): 1105-6.
38. G. Fava, "Do antidepressant and antianxiety drugs increase chronicity in affective
disorders?" Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 61 (1994): 125-31.
NOTES
375
39. G. Fava, "Can long-term treatment with antidepressant drugs worsen the course of
depression?" Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 64 (2003): 123-33.
40. Ibid.
41. G. Fava, "Holding on: depression, sensitization by antidepressant drugs, and the
prodigal experts," Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 64 (1995): 57-61; G. Fava,
"Potential sensitizing effects of antidepressant drugs on depression," CNS Drugs 12
(1999): 247-56.
42. R. Baldessarini, "Risks and implications of interrupting maintenance psychotropic
drug therapy," Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 63 (1995): 137-41.
43. R. El-Mallakh, "Can long-term antidepressant use be depressogenic?" Journal of
Clinical Psychiatry 60 (1999): 263.
44. "Editorial sparks debate on effects of psychoactive drugs," Psychiatric News, May
20, 1994.
45. Consensus Development Panel, "Mood disorders," American Journal of Psychiatry
142 (1985): 469-76.
46. R. Hales, editor. Textbook of Psychiatry (Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
Press, 1999), 525.
47. J. Geddes, "Relapse prevention with antidepressant drug treatment in depressive
disorders," Lancet 361 (2003): 653-61.
48. L. Judd, "Does incomplete recovery from first lifetime major depressive episode her¬
ald a chronic course of illness?" American Journal of Psychiatry 157 (2000): 1501-4.
49. R. Tranter, "Prevalence and outcome of partial remission in depression," Journal of
Psychiatry and Neuroscience 27 (2002): 241-47.
50. Hales, Textbook of Psychiatry, 547.
51. J. Rush, "One-year clinical outcomes of depressed public sector outpatients," Bio¬
logical Psychiatry 56 (2004): 46-53.
52. Ibid.
53. D. Warden, "The star*d project results," Current Psychiatry Reports 9 (2007):
449-59.
54. NIMH, Depression (2007): 3. (NIH Publication 07-3561.)
55. D. Deshauer, "Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors for unipolar depression,"
Canadian Medical Association Journal 178 (2008): 1293-1301.
56. C. Ronalds, "Outcome of anxiety and depressive disorders in primary care," British
Journal of Psychiatry 171 (1997): 427-33.
57. E. Weel-Baumgarten, "Treatment of depression related to recurrence," Journal of
Clinical Pharmacy and Therapeutics 25 (2000): 61-66.
58. S. Patten, "The impact of antidepressant treatment on population health," Popula¬
tion Health Metrics 2 (2004): 9.
59. D. Goldberg, "The effect of detection and treatment on the outcome of major de¬
pression in primary care," British Journal of General Practice 48 (1998): 1840-44.
60. Dewa, "Depression in the workplace."
61. W. Coryell, "Characteristics and significance of untreated major depressive disor¬
der," American Journal of Psychiatry 152 (1995): 1124-29.
376
NOTES
62. J. Moncrieff, "Trends in sickness benefits in Great Britain and the contribution of
mental disorders," Journal of Public Health Medicine 22 (2000): 59-67.
63. T. Helgason, "Antidepressants and public health in Iceland," British Journal of Psy¬
chiatry 184 (2004): 157-62.
64. R. Rosenheck, "The growth of psychopharmacology in the 1990s," International
Journal of Law and Psychiatry 28 (2005): 467-83.
65. M. Posternak, "The naturalistic course of unipolar major depression in the absence
of somatic therapy," Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 194 (2006): 324-49.
66. Ibid. Also see M. Posternak, "Untreated short-term course of major depression,"
Journal of Affective Disorders 66 (2001): 139-46.
67. J. Cole, editor. Psychopharmacology (Washington, DC: National Academy of Sci¬
ences, 1959), 347.
68. NIMH, "The numbers count," accessed at www.nimh.nih.gov on 3/7/2008; W. Eaton,
"The burden of mental disorders," Epidemiologic Reviews 30 (2008): 1-14.
69. M. Fava, "A cross-sectional study of the prevalence of cognitive and physical symp¬
toms during long-term antidepressant treatment," Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 67
(2006): 1754-59.
70. M. Kalia, "Comparative study of fluoxetine, sibutramine, sertraline and defenflu¬
ramine on the morphology of serotonergic nerve terminals using serotonin immuno-
histochemistry," Brain Research 858 (2000): 92-105. Also see press release by
Thomas Jefferson University Hospital, "Jefferson scientists show several serotonin¬
boosting drugs cause changes in some brain cells," 2/29/2000.
Chapter 9: The Bipolar Boom
1. D. Healy, Mania (Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press, 2008), 16, 41, 43.
2. I calculated these estimates by applying the 25 percent figure to the 1955 data on
patients in state and county mental hospitals with a diagnosis of manic-depressive
illness.
3. C. Silverman, The Epidemiology of Depression (Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Univer¬
sity Press, 1968), 139.
4. G. Winokur, Manic Depressive Illness (St. Louis: The C.V. Mosby Company, 1969),
19.
5. F. Wertham, "A group of benign chronic psychoses," American Journal of Psychia¬
try 9 (1929): 17-78.
6. G. Lundquist, "Prognosis and course in manic-depressive psychoses," Acta Psychi-
atrica Scandinavica, suppl. 35 (1945): 7-93.
7. M. Tsuang, "Long-term outcome of major psychoses," Archives of General Psychi¬
atry 36 (1979): 1295-1301.
8. Winokur, Manic Depressive Illness, 21.
9. NIMH, The Numbers Count: Mental Disorders in America, accessed at
www.nimh.nih.gov on 3/7/2008.
10. C. Baethge, "Substance abuse in first-episode bipolar I disorder," American Journal
of Psychiatry 162 (2005): 1008-10; E. Frank, "Association between illicit drug and
NOTES
377
alcohol use and first manic episode," Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior 86
(2007): 395-400.
11. S. Strakowski, "The effects of antecedent substance abuse on the development of
first-episode psychotic mania," Journal of Psychiatric Research 30 (1996): 59-68.
12. J. Goldberg, "Overdiagnosis of bipolar disorder among substance use disorder inpa¬
tients with mood instability," Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 69 (2008): 1751-57.
13. M. Van Laar, "Does cannabis use predict the first incidence of mood and anxiety
disorders in the adult population?" Addiction 102 (2007): 1251-60.
14. G. Crane, "The psychiatric side effects of iproniazid," American Journal of Psychia¬
try 112 (1956): 494-501.
15. J. Angst, "Switch from depression to mania," Psych op athology 18 (1985): 140-54.
16. American Psychiatric Association, Practice Guidelines for Major Depressive Disor¬
der in Adults (Washington, DC: APA, 1993), 22.
17. A. Martin, "Age effects on antidepressant-induced manic conversion," Archives of
Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine 158 (2004): 773-80.
18. J. Goldberg, "Risk for bipolar illness in patients initially hospitalized for unipolar
depression," American Journal of Psychiatry 158 (2001): 1265-70.
19. R. El-Mallakh, "Use of antidepressants to treat depression in bipolar disorder," Psy¬
chiatric Services 53 (2002): 58-84.
20. Interview with Fred Goodwin, "Advances in the diagnosis and treatment of bipolar
disorder," Primary Psychiatry, accessed via Internet on 3/6/09 at primarypsychia-
try.com .
21. G. Fava, "Can long-term treatment with antidepressant drugs worsen the course of
depression?" Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 64 (2003): 123-33.
22. L. Judd, "The prevalence and disability of bipolar spectrum disorders in the US pop¬
ulation," Journal of Affective Disorders 73 (2003): 123-31.
23. J. Angst, "Toward a re-definition of subthreshold bipolarity," Journal of Affective
Disorders 73 (2003): 133-46.
24. Ibid; Judd, "The prevalence and disability."
25. R. Fieve, Moodswing (New York: William Morrow and Company, 1975), 13.
26. For a history of lithium, see Healy, Mania, and J. Moncrieff, The Myth of the Chem¬
ical Cure (New York: Palgrave MacMillan, 2008).
27. S. Tyrer, "Lithium in the treatment of mania," Journal of Affective Disorders 8
(1985): 251-57.
28. J. Baker, "Outcomes of lithium discontinuation," Lithium 5 (1994): 187-92.
29. R. Baldessarini, "Discontinuing lithium maintenance treatment in bipolar disor¬
ders," Bipolar Disorders 1 (1999): 17-24.
30. G. Faedda, "Outcome after rapid v. gradual discontinuation of lithium treatment in
bipolar disorders," Archives of General Psychiatry 50 (1993): 448-55.
31. J. Himmelhoch, "On the failure to recognize lithium failure," Psychiatric Annals 24
(1994): 241-50.
32. J. Moncrieff, The Myth of the Chemical Cure (London: Palgrave Macmillan, 2008),
199.
378
NOTES
33. G. Goodwin, "Recurrence of mania after lithium withdrawal," British Journal of
Psychiatry 164 (1994): 149-52.
34. H. Markar, "Efficacy of lithium prophylaxis in clinical practice," British Journal of
Psychiatry 155 (1989): 496-500; J. Moncrieff, "Lithium revisited," British Journal
of Psychiatry 167 (1995): 569-74.
35. J. Goldberg, "Lithium treatment of bipolar affective disorders under naturalistic fol¬
lowup conditions," Psychopharmacology Bulletin 32 (1996): 47-54.
36. M. Gitlin, "Relapse and impairment in bipolar disorder," American Journal ofPsy-
.chiatry 152 (1995): 1635-40.
37. J. Moncrieff, "Lithium: evidence reconsidered," British Journal of Psychiatry 171
(1997): 113-19.
38. F. Goodwin, Manic-Depressive Illness (New York: Oxford University Press, 1990),
647.
39. A. Zis, "Major affective disorder as a recurrent illness," Archives of General
Psychiatry 36 (1979): 835-39.
40. A. Koukopoulos, "Rapid cyclers, temperament, and antidepressants," Comprehen¬
sive Psychiatry 24 (1983): 249-58.
41. N. Ghaemi, "Diagnosing bipolar disorder and the effect of antidepressants," Jour¬
nal of Clinical Psychiatry 61 (2000): 804-809.
42. N. Ghaemi, "Antidepressants in bipolar disorder," Bipolar Disorders 5 (2003):
421-33.
43. R. El-Mallakh, "Use of antidepressants to treat depression in bipolar disorder," Psy¬
chiatric Services 53 (2002): 580-84.
44. A. Koukopoulos, "Duration and stability of the rapid-cycling course," Journal of
Affective Disorders 72 (2003): 75-85.
45. R. El-Mallakh, "Antidepressant-associated chronic irritable dysphoria in bipolar
disorder," Journal of Affective Disorders 84 (2005): 267-72.
46. N. Ghaemi, "Treatment of rapid-cycling bipolar disorder," American Journal of
Psychiatry 165 (2008): 300-301.
47. C. Schneck, "The prospective course of rapid-cycling bipolar disorder," American
Journal of Psychiatry 165 (2008): 370-77.
48. L. Judd, "The long-term natural history of the weekly symptomatic status of bipolar
I disorder," Archives of General Psychiatry 59 (2002): 530-37.
49. L. Judd, "A prospective investigation of the natural history of the long-term weekly
symptomatic status of bipolar II disorder," Archives of General Psychiatry 60
(2003): 261-69.
50. R. Joffe, "A prospective, longitudinal study of percentage of time spent ill in patients
with bipolar I or bipolar II disorders," Bipolar Disorders 6 (2004): 62-66.
51. R. Post, "Morbidity in 258 bipolar outpatients followed for 1 year with daily
prospective ratings on the NIMH life chart method," Journal of Clinical Psychiatry
64 (2003): 680-90.
52. L. Judd, "Residual symptom recovery from major affective episodes in bipolar dis¬
orders and rapid episode relapse/recurrence," Archives of General Psychiatry 65
(2008): 386-94.
NOTES
379
53. C. Zarate, "Functional impairment and cognition in bipolar disorder," Psychiatric
Quarterly 71 (2000): 309-29.
54. Gitlin, "Relapse and impairment."
55. R Keck, "12-month outcome of patients with bipolar disorder following hospital¬
ization for a manic or a mixed episode," American journal of Psychiatry 155
(1998): 646-52.
56. D. Kupfer, "Demographic and clinical characteristics of individuals in a bipolar
disorder case registry," journal of Clinical Psychiatry 63 (2002): 120-25.
57. N. Huxley, "Disability and its treatment in bipolar disorder patients," Bipolar Dis¬
orders 9 (2007): 183-96.
58. T. Goldberg, "Contrasts between patients with affective disorders and patients with
schizophrenia on a neuropsychological test battery," American journal of Psychia¬
try 150 (1993): 1355-62.
59. J. Zihl, "Cognitive deficits in schizophrenia and affective disorders," Acta Psychi-
atrica Scandinavica 97 (1998): 351-57.
60. F. Dickerson, "Outpatients with schizophrenia and bipolar I disorder," Psychiatry
Research 102 (2001): 21-27.
61. G. Malhi, "Neuropsychological deficits and functional impairment in bipolar de¬
pression, hypomania and euthymia," Bipolar Disorders 9 (2007): 114-25.
62. V. Balanza-Martinez, "Persistent cognitive dysfunctions in bipolar I disorder and
schizophrenic patients," Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics 74 (2005): 113-19;
A Martinez-Aran, "Functional outcome in bipolar disorder," Bipolar Disorders 9
(2007): 103-13.
63. M. Pope, "Determinants of social functioning in bipolar disorder," Bipolar Disor¬
ders 9 (2007): 38-44.
64. C. Zarate, "Antipsychotic drug side effect issues in bipolar manic patients," Journal
of Clinical Psychiatry 61, suppl. 8 (2000): 52-61.
65. C. Zarate, "Functional impairment and cognition in bipolar disorder," Psychiatric
Quarterly 71 (2000): 309-29.
66. D. Kupfer, "The increasing medical burden in bipolar disorder," journal of the
American Medical Association 293 (2005): 2528-30.
67. L. Citrome, "Toward convergence in the medication treatment of bipolar disorder
and schizophrenia," Harvard Review of Psychiatry 13 (2005): 28-42.
68. Huxley, "Disability and its treatment."
69. M. Harrow, "Factors involved in outcome and recovery in schizophrenia patients
not on antipsychotic medications," Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders 195
(2007): 406-14.
70. W. Eaton, "The burden of mental disorders," Epidemiology Review 30 (2008):
1-14.
Chapter 10: An Epidemic Explained
1. Interview with Amy Upham, June 14, 2009.
2. M. Morgan, "Prospective analysis of premature mortality in schizophrenia in rela¬
tion to health service engagement," Psychiatry Research 117 (2003): 127-35;
3 8 o
NOTES
C. Colton, "Congruencies in increased mortality rates, years of potential life lost,
and causes of death among public mental health clients in eight states," Preventing
Chronic Disease 3 (April 2006).
3. S. Saha, "A systematic review of mortality in schizophrenia," Archives of General
Psychiatry 64 (2007): 1123-31; L. Appleby, "Sudden unexplained death in psychi¬
atric in-patients," British Journal of Psychiatry 176 (2000): 405-406; M. Jouka-
maa, "Schizophrenia, neuroleptic medication, and mortality," British Journal of
Psychiatry 188 (2006): 122-27.
Chapter 11: The Epidemic Spreads to Children
1. B. Carey, "What's wrong with a child? Psychiatrists often disagree," New York
Times , November 11, 2006.
2. R. Kessler, "Mood disorders in children and adolescents," Biological Psychiatry 49
(2001): 1002-14.
3. J. O'Neal, Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology Made Simple (Oakland, CA:
New Harbinger Publications, 2006), 6.
4. R. Mayes, Medicating Children (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2009),
46.
5. G. Jackson, "Postmodern psychiatry," unpublished paper, September 2, 2002.
6. Mayes, Medicating Children, 54.
7. Ibid, 61.
8. R. Mayes, "ADHD and the rise in stimulant use among children," Harvard Review
of Psychiatry 16 (2008): 151-66.
9. G. Golden, "Role of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in learning disabilities,"
Seminars in Neurology 11 (1991): 35-41.
10. NIH Consensus Development Conference statement, "Diagnosis and treatment of
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder," November 16-18, 1998.
11. P. Breggin, Talking Back to Ritalin (Cambridge, MA: Perseus Publishing, 2001),
180.
12. S. Hyman, "Initiation and adaptation: a paradigm for understanding psychotropic
drug action," American Journal of Psychiatry 153 (1996): 151-61.
13. Breggin, Talking Back to Ritalin , 83.
14. H. Rie, "Effects of methylphenidate on underachieving children," Journal of Con¬
sulting and Clinical Psychology 44 (1976): 250-60.
15. C. Cunningham, "The effects of methylphenidate on the mother-child interactions
of hyperactive identical twins," Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology 20
(1978): 634-42.
16. N. Fiedler, "The effects of stimulant drugs on curiosity behaviors of hyperactive
boys," Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 11 (1983): 193-206.
17. T. Davy, "Stimulant medication and short attention span," Journal of Developmen¬
tal & Behavioral Pediatrics 10 (1989): 313-18.
18. D. Granger, "Perceptions of methylphenidate effects on hyperactive children's peer
interactions," Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 21 (1993): 535-49.
NOTES
381
19. J. Swanson, "Effects of stimulant medication on learning in children with ADHD,"
Journal of Learning Disabilities 24 (1991): 219-30.
20. Breggin, Talking Back to Ritalin, 92.
21. J. Richters, "NIMH Collaborative Multisite Multimodal Treatment Study of Chil¬
dren with ADHD," Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psy¬
chiatry 34 (1995): 987-1000.
22. T. Spencer, "Pharmacotherapy of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder across the
life cycle," Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 35
(1996): 409-32.
23. E. Sleator, "How do hyperactive children feel about taking stimulants and will they
tell the doctor?" Clinical Pediatrics 21 (1982): 474-79.
24. D. Jacobvitz, "Treatment of attentional and hyperactivity problems in children with
sympathomimetic drugs," Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent
Psychiatry 29 (1990): 677-88.
25. A. Sroufe, "Treating problem children with stimulant drugs," New England Journal
of Medicine 289 (1973): 407-13.
26. Ibid.
27. Rie, "Effects of methylphenidate."
28. R. Barkley, "Do stimulant drugs improve the academic performance of hyperkinetic
children?" Clinical Pediatrics 8 (1978): 137-46.
29. Swanson, "Effects of stimulant medication."
30. C. Whalen, "Stimulant pharmacotherapy for attention-deficit hyperactivity disor¬
ders," in S. Fishberg and R. Greenberg, eds.. From Placebo to Panacea (New York:
John Wiley & Sons, 1997), 329.
31. R. Schachar, "Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder," Canadian Journal of Psychi¬
atry 47 (2002): 337-48.
32. Whalen, "Stimulant pharmacotherapy," 327.
33. P. Breggin, "Psychostimulants in the treatment of children diagnosed with ADHD,"
International Journal of Risk & Safety in Medicine 12 (1993): 3-35.
34. Ibid.
35. Richters, "NIMH Collaborative Multisite."
36. P. Jensen, "3-year follow-up of the NIMH MTA study," Journal of the American
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 46 (2007): 989-1002. See chart on page
997 for medication use.
37. The MTA Cooperative Group, "A 14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment
strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder," Archives of General Psychia¬
try 56 (1999): 1073-86.
38. Jensen, "3-year follow-up."
39. B. Molina, "Delinquent behavior and emerging substance use in the MTA at 36
months," Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 46
(2007): 1028-39.
40. B. Molina, "MTA at 8 years," Journal of the American Academy of Child & Ado¬
lescent Psychiatry 48 (2009): 484-500.
382
NOTES
41. C. Miranda, "ADHD drugs could stunt growth," Daily Telegraph (UK), November
12, 2007.
42. Breggin, Talking Back to Ritalin; K. Bolla, "The neuropsychiatry of chronic cocaine
abuse," Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 10 (1998): 280-89.
43. S. Castner, "Long-lasting psychotomimetic consequences of repeated low-dose am¬
phetamine exposure in rhesus monkeys," Neuropsychopharmacology 20 (1999):
10-28.
44. W. Carlezon, "Enduring behavioral effects of early exposure to methylphenidate in
rats," Biological Psychiatry 54 (2003): 1330-37.
45. C. Bolanos, "Methylphenidate treatment during pre- and periadolescence alters be¬
havioral responses to emotional stimuli at adulthood," Biological Psychiatry 54
(2003): 1317-29.
46. J. Zito, "Rising prevalence of antidepressants among US youths," Pediatrics 109
(2002): 721-27.
47. R. Fisher, Prom Placebo to Panacea (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1997), 309.
48. T. Delate, "Trends in the use of antidepressants in a national sample of commercially
insured pediatric patients, 1998 to 2002," Psychiatric Services 55 (2004): 387-91.
49. Editorial, "Depressing research," Lancet 363 (2004): 1335.
50. T. Laughren, Memorandum, "Background comments for Feb. 2, 2004 meeting of
psychopharmacological drugs advisory committee," January 5, 2004. Accessed at
fda.gov .
51. J. Leo, "The SSRI trials in children," Ethical Human Psychology and Psychiatry 8
(2006): 29-41.
52. C. Whittington, "Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors in childhood depression,"
Lancet 363 (2004): 1341-45.
53. Editorial, "Depressing research," Lancet 363 (2004): 1335.
54. J. Jureidini, "Efficacy and safety of antidepressants for children and adolescents,"
British Medical Journal (2004): 879-83.
55. T. Wilens, "A systematic chart review of the nature of psychiatric adverse events in
children and adolescents treated with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors," Jour¬
nal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology 13 (2003): 143-52.
56. T. Gualtieri, "Antidepressant side effects in children and adolescents," Journal of
Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology 16 (2006): 147-57.
57. P. Breggin, Brain-Disabling Treatments in Psychiatry (New York: Springer Publish¬
ing Company, 2008), 153.
58. D. Papolos, The Bipolar Child (New York: Broadway Books, 2000), xiv.
59. C. Moreno, "National trends in the outpatient diagnosis and treatment of bipolar
disorder in youth," Archives of General Psychiatry 64 (2007): 1032-39.
60. J. Kluger, "Young and Bipolar," Time , August 19, 2002.
61. L. Lurie, "Psychoses in children," Journal of Pediatrics 36 (1950): 801-9.
62. Ibid.
63. B. Hall, "Our present knowledge about manic-depressive states in childhood," Ner¬
vous Child 9 (1952): 319-25.
NOTES
383
64. J. Anthony, "Manic-depressive psychosis in childhood," Journal of Child Psychol¬
ogy and Psychiatry 1 (1960): 53-72.
65. W. Weinberg, "Mania in childhood," American Journal of Diseases of Childhood
130(1976): 380-85.
66. R. DeLong, "Lithium carbonate treatment of select behavior disorders in children
suggesting manic-depressive illness," Journal of Pediatrics 93 (1978): 689-94.
67. M. Strober, "Bipolar illness in adolescents with major depression," Archives of
General Psychiatry 39 (1982): 549-55.
68. R Lewinsohn, "Bipolar disorders in a community sample of older adolescents," Jour¬
nal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 34 (1995): 454-63.
69. G. Carlson, "Manic symptoms in psychiatrically hospitalized children —what do
they mean?" Journal of Affective Disorders 51 (1998): 123-35.
70. J. Kluger, "Young and Bipolar."
71. D. Janowsky, "Proceedings: effect of intravenous d-amphetamine, 1-amphetamine
and methylphenidate in schizophrenics," Psychopharmacology Bulletin 19 (1974):
15-24.
72. E. Cherland, "Psychotic side effects of psychostimulants," Canadian Journal of
Psychiatry 44 (1999): 811-13.
73. K. Gelperin, "Psychiatric adverse events associated with drug treatment of ADHD,"
FDA, Center for Drug Evaluation and Research, March 3, 2006.
74. D. Papolos, "Bipolar disorder, co-occuring conditions, and the need for extreme
caution before initiating drug treatment," Bipolar Child Newsletter 1 (November
1999).
75. M. DelBello, "Prior stimulant treatment in adolescents with bipolar disorder," Bi¬
polar Disorders 3 (2001): 53-57.
76. J. Biederman, "Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and juvenile mania," Journal
of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 35 (1996): 997-1008.
77. J. Jain, "Fluoxetine in children and adolescents with mood disorders," Journal of
Child & Adolescent Psychopharmacology 2 (1992): 259-65.
78. G. Emslie, "A double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial of fluoxetine in
children and adolescents with depression," Archives of General Psychiatry 54
(1997): 1031-37.
79. P. Breggin, The Anti-Depressant Fact Book (Cambridge, MA: Perseus Publishing,
2001), 116.
80. A. Martin, "Age effects on antidepressant-induced manic conversion," Archives of
Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine 158 (2004): 773-80.
81. G. Faedda, "Pediatric onset bipolar disorder," Harvard Review of Psychiatry 3
(1995): 171-95.
82. B. Geller, "Bipolar disorder at prospective follow-up of adults who had prepubertal
major depressive disorder," American Journal of Psychiatry 158 (2001): 125-27.
83. D. Cicero, "Antidepressant exposure in bipolar children," Psychiatry 66 (2003):
317-22.
84. D. Papolos. "Antidepressant-induced adverse effects in juvenile-onset bipolar
384
NOTES
disorder/' paper presented at the Fifth International Conference on Bipolar Disor¬
der, June 12-14, 2003, Pittsburgh, PA.
85. G. Faedda, "Pediatric bipolar disorder," Bipolar Disorders 6 (2004): 305-13.
86. M. Hellander, "Children with bipolar disorder," Journal of the American Academy
of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 38 (1999): 495.
87. H. Marano, "Crisis on the campus," Psychology Today, May 2, 2002.
88. C. Reichart, "Earlier onset of bipolar disorder in children by antidepressants or
stimulants," Journal of Affective Disorders 78 (2004): 81-84. Also see abstracts
presented at the Fourth International Conference on Bipolar Disorder in Pittsburgh,
June 2001.
89. B. Geller, "Child and adolescent bipolar disorder," Journal of the American Acad¬
emy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 36 (1997): 1168-76.
90. Papolos, "Antidepressant-induced adverse effects."
91. G. Faedda, "Treatment-emergent mania in pediatric bipolar disorder," Journal of
Affective Disorders (82): 149-58.
92. R. Pedis, "Long-term implications of early onset in bipolar disorder," Biological
Psychiatry 55 (2004): 875-81.
93. B. Birmaher, "Course and outcome of bipolar spectrum disorder in children and
adolescents," Development and Psychopathology 18 (2006): 1023-35.
94. M. DelBello, "Twelve-month outcome of adolescents with bipolar disorder
following first hospitalization for a manic or mixed episode," American Journal of
Psychiatry 164 (2007): 582-90.
95. T. Goldstein, "Psychosocial functioning among bipolar youth," Journal of Affective
Disorders 114 (2009): 174-83.
96. B. Geller, "Two-year prospective follow-up of children with a prepubertal and early
adolescent bipolar disorder phenotype," American Journal of Psychiatry 159
(2002): 927-33.
97. "Hayes says new treatments for pediatric bipolar disorder not ready for prime time"
(December 3, 2008 press release), accessed at hayesinc.com, August 2, 2009.
98. Social Security Administration, annual statistical reports on the SSI program, 1996-
2008; Social Security Bulletin, Annual Statistical Supplement, 1988-1992.
99. Pediatric Academic Societies, "Pediatric psychiatry admissions on the rise," May 16,
2000, press release.
100. D. Satcher, Report of Surgeon General's Conference on Children's Mental Health
(U.S. Dept, of Health and Human Services, 2001).
101. B. Whitford, "Depression, eating disorders and other mental illnesses are on the
rise," Newsweek, August 27, 2008.
102. U.S. Government Accountability Office, "Young adults with serious mental illness"
(June 2008).
Chapter 12: Suffer the Children
1. J. Zito, "Psychotropic medication patterns among youth in foster care," Pediatrics
121 (2008): 157-63.
NOTES
385
Chapter 13: The Rise of an Ideology
1. C. Ross, Pseudoscience in Psychiatry (New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1995).
2. G. Klerman, "A debate on DSM-III," American journal of Psychiatry 141 (1984):
539-42.
3. M. Sabshin, "Report of the medical director," American journal of Psychiatry 137
(1980): 1308.
4. See blurbs for second edition of The Myth of Mental Illness, published by Harper &c
Row in 1974.
5. B. Nelson, "Psychiatry's anxious years," New York Times, November 2, 1982.
6. D. Adler, "The medical model and psychiatry's tasks," Hospital and Community
Psychiatry 32 (1981): 387-92.
7. Sabshin, "Report of the medical director."
8. Nelson, "Psychiatry's anxious years."
9. Copy from a Smith Kline and French advertisement that ran monthly in Mental
Hospitals in 1962.
10. L. Thorne, "Inside Russia's psychiatric jails," New York Times Magazine, June 12,
1977.
11. U.S. Senate, Committee on the Judiciary, Subcommittee to Investigate Juvenile
Delinquency, Drugs in Institutions, 94th Cong., 1st sess., 1975.
12. A. Tone, The Age of Anxiety (New York: Basic Books, 2009), 176.
13. M. Smith, Small Comfort (New York: Praeger, 1985), 32.
14. Interview with Arthur Piatt, June 8, 2009.
15. M. Sabshin, "On remedicalization and holism in psychiatry," Psychosomatics 18
(1977): 7-8.
16. A. Ludwig, "The medical basis of psychiatry," American journal of Psychiatry 134
(1977): 1087-92.
17. P. Blaney, "Implications of the medical model and its alternatives," American Jour¬
nal of Psychiatry 132 (1975): 911-14.
18. S. Guze, "Nature of psychiatric illness," Comprehensive Psychiatry 19 (1978):
295-307.
19. Adler, "The medical model."
20. M. Wilson, "DSM-III and the transformation of American psychiatry," American
journal of Psychiatry 150 (1993): 399-410.
21. S. Kirk, The Selling ofDSM (New York: Aldine de Gruyter, 1992), 114.
22. Ibid, 134.
23. M. Sabshin, "Turning points in twentieth-century American psychiatry," American
Journal of Psychiatry (1990): 1267-74.
24. Klerman, "A debate on DSM-III."
25. J. Maxmen, The New Psychiatrists (New York: New American Library, 1985),
35,31.
26. H. Kutchins, Making Us Crazy (New York: The Free Press, 1997), 248.
27. Kirk, The Selling ofDSM, 115.
28. M. Sabshin, "Report of the medical director" (1980), 1308.
386
NOTES
29. L. Havens, "Twentieth-century psychiatry," American journal of Psychiatry 138
(1981): 1279-87.
30. B. Bursten, "Rallying 'round the medical model," Hospital and Community Psychi¬
atry 32(1981): 371.
31. Sources for this political battle include reviews by NIMH's "Clinical Programs Proj¬
ects Research Review Committee" on April 27, 1970; April 1-2, 1973; April 1974;
April 21, 1975; June 27, 1977; December 1, 1977; February 17-18, 1978; and June
26-27, 1978.
32. Interview with Loren Mosher, December 1, 2000.
33. M. Sabshin, "Report of the medical director," American Journal of Psychiatry 138
(1981): 1418-21.
34. P. Breggin, Toxic Psychiatry (New York: St. Martin's Press, 1991), 360.
35. Sabshin, "Report of the medical director" (1981).
36. M. Sabshin, "Report of the medical director," American Journal of Psychiatry 140
(1983): 1398-1403.
37. R. Peele, "Report of the speaker-elect," American Journal of Psychiatry 143 (1986):
1348-50.
38. M. Sabshin, "Report of the medical director," American Journal of Psychiatry 143
(1986): 1342-46.
39. M. Sabshin, "Report of the medical director," American Journal of Psychiatry 145
(1988): 1338 A 12.
40. Sabshin, "Report of the medical director" (1981).
41. M. Sabshin, Changing American Psychiatry (Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
Publishing, Inc., 2008), 78.
42. Sabshin, "Report of the medical director" (1983).
43. Sabshin, "Report of the medical director" (1986).
44. New York Times , November 26, 1981; September 7, 1982; July 29, 1984.
45. J. Franklin, "The Mind-Fixers," Baltimore Evening Sun, July 1984.
46. M. Gold, The Good News About Depression (New York: Villard Books, 1987),
xi-xiii.
47. N. Andreasen, The Broken Brain (New York: Harper & Row, 1984), 29-30.
48. Ibid, 138.
49. Franklin, "The Mind-Fixers."
50. Sabshin, Changing American Psychiatry, 194.
51. M. Dumont, "In bed together at the market," American Journal of Orthopsychiatry
60 (1990): 484-85.
52. F. Gottlieb, "Report of the speaker," American Journal of Psychiatry 142 (1985):
1246-49.
53. Breggin, Toxic Psychiatry, 46, 357.
54. P. Breggin, Medication Madness (New York: St. Martin's Press, 2008), 150.
55. S. Boseley, "Scandal of scientists who take money for papers ghostwritten by drug
companies," Guardian, February 7, 2002.
56. M. Angel, "Is academic medicine for sale?" New England Journal of Medicine 342
(2000): 1516-18.
NOTES
387
57. D. Regier, "The NIMH depression awareness, recognition, and treatment pro¬
gram," American Journal of Psychiatry 145 (1988): 1351-57.
58. Breggin, Toxic Psychiatry, 14.
59. E. Foulks, "Advocating for persons who are mentally ill," Administration and
Policy in Mental Health and Mental Health Services Research 27 (2000): 353-67.
60. A. Hatfield, "The National Alliance for the Mentally 111," Community Mental
Health Journal 27 (1991): 95-103.
61. E. Benedek, "Report of the secretary," American Journal of Psychiatry 144 (1987):
1381-88.
62. Breggin, Toxic Psychiatry, 363.
63. Foulks, "Advocating for persons."
64. K. Silverstein, " Prozac.org ," Mother Jones, November/December 1999.
65. R. Behar, "The thriving cult of greed and power," Time, May 6, 1991.
Chapter 14: The Story That Was... and Wasn't Told
1. D. Healy, Mania (Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press, 2008), 132.
2. G. Carson, The Roguish World of Doctor Brinkley (New York: Rinehart & Co.,
1960).
3. P. Breggin, Brain-Disabling Treatments in Psychiatry (New York: Springer Publish¬
ing Co., 2008), 390.
4. "Fluoxetine project team meeting," July 31, 1978, accessed at healyprozac.com .
5. "Fluoxetine project team meeting," July 23, 1979, accessed at healyprozac.com .
6. J. Cornwell, The Power to Harm (New York: Viking, 1996), 147-48.
7. D. Healy, Let Them Eat Prozac (New York: New York University Press, 2004), 39.
8. Ibid, 128.
9. Ibid, 249.
10. BGA letter to Eli Lilly, May 25, 1984, Forsyth v. Eli Lilly trial documents, exhibit
42. See baumhedlundlaw.com/media/timeline .
11. Forsyth v. Eli Lilly trial documents, exhibit 58.
12. Cornwell, The Power to Harm, 198.
13. Healy, Let Them Eat Prozac, 35.
14. P. Breggin, Talking Back to Prozac (New York: St. Martin's Press, 1994), 41.
15. Ibid, 46.
16. Ibid, 90. Also see P. Breggin, Brain-Disabling Treatments in Psychiatry, 79,
86,91.
17. D. Graham, "Sponsor's ADR submission on fluoxetine dated July 17, 1990," FDA
document, September 1990.
18. T. Moore, "Hard to Swallow," Washingtonian, December 1997.
19. D. Kessler, "Introducing MEDWatch," Journal of the American Medical Association
269(1993): 2765-68.
20. J. Bremner, "Fluoxetine in depressed patients," Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 45
(1984): 414-19.
21. J. Feigner, "A comparative trial of fluoxetine and amitriptyline in patients with
major depressive disorder," Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 46 (1985): 369-72.
388
NOTES
22. J. Cohn, "A comparison of fluoxetine, imipramine, and placebo in patients with
major depressive disorder," Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 46 (1985): 26-31.
23. J. Wernicke, "The side effect profile and safety of fluoxetine," Journal of Clinical
Psychiatry 46 (1985): 59-67.
24. P. Stark, "A review of multicenter controlled studies of fluoxetine vs. imipramine
and placebo in outpatients with major depressive disorder," Journal of Clinical Psy¬
chiatry 46 (1985): 53-58.
25. S. Levine, "A comparative trial of a new antidepressant, fluoxetine," British Journal
of Psychiatry 150 (1987): 653-55.
26. R. Pary, "Fluoxetine: prescribing guidelines for the newest antidepressant," South¬
ern Medical Journal 82 (1989): 1005-9.
27. D. Regier, "The NIMH depression awareness, recognition and treatment program,"
American Journal of Psychiatry 145 (1988): 1351-57.
28. Healy, Let Them Eat Prozac, 9.
29. F. Schumer, "Bye-Bye, Blues," New York, December 18, 1989.
30. G. Cowley, "Prozac: A Breakthrough Drug for Depression," Newsweek, March 26,
1990.
31. N. Angier, "New antidepressant is acclaimed but not perfect," New York Times,
March 29, 1990.
32. B. Duncan, "Exposing the mythmakers," Psychotherapy Networker, March/April
2000 .
33. M. Waldholz, "Prozac said to spur idea of suicide," Wall Street Journal, July 18,
1990.
34. Ibid. Also see S. Shellenbarger, "Eli Lilly stock plunges $4,375 on news of another
lawsuit over Prozac drug," Wall Street Journal, July 27, 1990.
35. Memo from Leigh Thompson to Allan Weinstein, February 7, 1990, accessed at
healyprozac.com
36. Memo from Mitch Daniels to Leigh Thompson, "Upcoming TV appearance," April
15, 1991, accessed at healyprozac.com .
37. Ibid.
38. T. Burton, "Medical flap: Anti-depression drug of Eli Lilly loses sales after attack by
sect," Wall Street Journal, April 19, 1991.
39. L. Garnett, "Prozac revisited," Boston Globe, May 7, 2000.
40. R. Behar, "The Thriving Cult of Greed and Power," Time, May 6, 1991.
41. T. Burton, "Panel finds no credible evidence to tie Prozac to suicides and violent be¬
havior," Wall Street Journal, September 23, 1991.
42. S. Begley, "Beyond Prozac," Newsweek, February 7, 1994.
43. P. Breggin, Toxic Psychiatry (New York: St. Martin's Press, 1991), 348-50. In this
book, Breggin detailed the bad science involved in the Xanax trials, the co-opting of
academic psychiatry, and the involvement of the APA in marketing the drug.
44. "High Anxiety," Consumer Reports, January 1993.
45. C. Ballenger, "Alprazolam in panic disorder and agoraphobia," Archives of General
Psychiatry 45 (1988): 413-22.
NOTES
389
46. R. Noyes, "Alprazolam in panic disorder and agoraphobia," Archives of General
Psychiatry 45 (1988): 423-28.
47. J. Pecknold, "Alprazolam in panic disorder and agoraphobia," Archives of General
Psychiatry 45 (1988): 429-36.
48. Ballenger, "Alprazolam in panic disorder."
49. Noyes, "Alprazolam in panic disorder."
50. Pecknold, "Alprazolam in panic disorder."
51. I. Marks, "The 'efficacy' of alprazolam in panic disorder and agoraphobia,"
Archives of General Psychiatry 46 (1989): 668-72.
52. I. Marks, "Reply to comment on the London/Toronto study," British Journal of Psy¬
chiatry 162 (1993): 790-94.
53. Breggin, Toxic Psychiatry, 344-53.
54. F. Pollner, "Don't overlook panic disorder," Medical World News, October 1, 1991.
55. J. Randal, "In a panic?" St. Louis Post-Dispatch, October 7, 1990.
56. H. Brown, "Panic attacks keeps thousands from malls, off roads," Associated Press,
November 19, 1990.
57. R. Davis, "When panic is disabling," Chicago Sun-Times, June 29, 1992.
58. "High Anxiety," Consumer Reports.
59. FDA reviews of risperidone data included the following written commentaries:
reviews by Andrew Mosholder, May 11, 1993, and November 7, 1993; David
Hoberman, April 20, 1993; and Thomas Laughren, December 20, 1993.
60. Approval letter from Robert Temple to Janssen Research Foundation, December 29,
1993.
61. S. Marder, "The effects of risperidone on the five dimensions of schizophrenia
derived by factor analysis," Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 58 (1997): 538-46.
62. "New hope for schizophrenia," Washington Post, February 16, 1993.
63. "Seeking safer treatments for schizophrenia," New York Times, January 15, 1992.
64. FDA reviews of olanzapine data included the following written commentaries:
reviews by Thomas Laughren on September 27, 1996; by Paul Andreason on July
29 and September 26, 1996; and by Paul Leber on August 18 and August 30,
1996.
65. C. Beasley, "Efficacy of olanzapine," Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 58, suppl. 10
(1997): 7-12.
66. "Psychosis drug from Eli Lilly racks up gains," Wall Street Journal, April 14, 1998.
67. "A new drug for schizophrenia wins approval from the FDA," New York Times,
October 2, 1996.
68. "Schizophrenia, close-up of the troubled brain," Parade, November 21, 1999.
69. "Mental illness aid," Chicago Tribune, June 4, 1999.
70. "Lives recovered," Los Angeles Times, January 30, 1996.
71. P. Weiden, Breakthroughs in Antipsychotic Medications (New York: WW Norton,
1999), 26
72. Wall Street Journal, "Psychosis drug from Eli Lilly."
73. "High Anxiety," Consumer Reports.
390
NOTES
74. J. Lieberman, "Effectiveness of antipsychotic drugs in patients with schizophrenia,"
New England Journal of Medicine (2005): 1209-33.
75. L. Davies, "Cost-effectiveness of first- v. second-generation antipsychotic drugs."
British Journal of Psychiatry 191 (2007): 14-22.
76. P. Tyrer, "The spurious advance of antipsychotic drug therapy," Lancet 373 (2009):
4-5.
77. Interview with Peter Breggin, October 10, 2008.
78. Healy interview on CBS News and Current AJfairs, June 12, 2001.
79. D. Healy, "Psychopharmacology and the government of the self," talk given
November 30, 2000, at the University of Toronto.
80. E-mail from David Goldbloom to David Healy, December 7, 2000.
81. Interview with Healy by e-mail, July 4, 2009.
82. Memo from Larry Carpman to Steve Kurkjian, April 11, 2000.
83. "Science News from 2007," NIMH website, accessed on July 2, 2009.
84. NIMH press release, July 20, 2007.
85. J. Sharkey, "Delusions; paranoia is universal," New York Times, August 2, 1998.
86. Search of NAMI website on July 7, 2009.
87. R. Hales, The American Psychiatric Publishing Textbook of Psychiatry (Arlington,
VA: American Psychiatric Publishing, 2008).
Chapter 15: Tallying Up the Profits
1. D. Carlat, "Dr. Drug Rep," New York Times, November 25, 2007.
2. NAMI IRS 990 Form, 2000.
3. B. Koerner, "First you market the disesase, then you push the pills to treat it,"
Guardian, July 30, 2002.
4. E. Tanouye, "Antidepressant makers study kids' market," Wall Street Journal, April
4, 1997.
5. B. Strauch, "Use of antidepression medicine for young patients has soared," New
York Times, August 10, 1997.
6. Tanouye, "Antidepressant makers."
7. Deposition of Joseph Biederman in legal case of Avila v. Johnson & Johnson Co.,
February 26, 2009, pages 139, 231, 232, 237.
8. J. Biederman, "Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and juvenile mania," Journal
of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 35 (1996): 997-1008.
9. Deposition of Joseph Biederman, p. 158.
10. Margaret Williams, report on a sales call. May 17, 2002.
11. J. J. Zorc, "Expenditures for psychotropic medications in the United States in
1985," American Journal of Psychiatry 148 (1991): 644-47
12. "Top therapeutic classes by U.S. sales, 2008," IMS Health.
13. S. Giled, "Better but not best," Health Affairs 28 (2009): 637-48.
14. These calculations are based on Eli Lilly's annual 10-K reports filed with the SEC
from 1987 to 2000. Capitalization figures for 1987 and 2000 are based on prices in
the fourth quarter of each year.
15. J. Pereira, "Emory professor steps down," Wall Street Journal, December 23, 2008.
NOTES
391
16. C. Schneider, "Emory psychiatrist reprimanded over outside work," Atlanta
Journal-Constitution, June 11, 2009.
17. G. Harris, "Radio host has drug company ties," New York Times, November 22,
2008.
18. GlaxoSmithKline internal memo, "Seroxat/Paxil adolescent depression. Position
piece on the phase III studies," October 1998.
19. M. Keller, "Efficacy of paroxetine in the treatment of adolescent major depression,"
Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 40 (2001):
762-72.
20. E. Ramshaw, "Senator questions doctors' ties to drug companies," Dallas Morning
News, September 24, 2008.
21. L. Kowalczyk, "US cites Boston psychiatrist in case vs. drug firm," Boston Globe,
March 6, 2009.
22. G. Harris, "Lawmaker calls for registry of drug firms paying doctors," New York
Times, August 4, 2007.
23. G. Harris, "Researchers fail to reveal full drug pay," New York Times, June 8, 2008.
24. Avila v. Johnson & Johnson, deposition of Joseph Biederman, February 26, 2009,
119.
25. J. Biederman, Annual Report 2002: The Johnson & Johnson Center for Pediatric
Psychopathology at the Massachusetts General Hospital.
26. J. Olson, "Drug makers step up giving to Minnesota psychiatrists," Pioneer Press,
August 27, 2007.
27. Margaret Williams, reports on sales calls, April 20, 2001, and April 8, 2002.
28. Eli Lilly grant registry, 2009, 1st quarter.
29. E. Mundell, "U.S. spending on mental health care soaring," HealthDay, August 6,
2009.
30. T. Mark, "Mental health treatment expenditure trends, 1986-2003," Psychiatric
Services 58 (2007): 1041-48. Seven percent of national health expenditures in 2008
went to mental health services; by 2015, this figure is expected to rise to 8 percent.
Data on national health expenditures in 2008, and projected expenditures in 2015,
are from the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.
Chapter 16: Blueprints for Reform
1. MindFreedom, "Original statement by the fast for freedom in mental health," July
28, 2003.
2. Letter from James Scully to David Oaks, August 12, 2003.
3. Letter from MindFreedom scientific panel to James Scully, August 22, 2003.
4. APA statement on "diagnosis and treatment of mental disorders," September 26,
2003.
5. Letter from MindFreedom scientific panel to James Scully, December 15, 2003.
6. Interview with David Oaks, October 4, 2009.
7. J. Modrow, How to Become a Schizophrenic (Seattle: Apollyon Press, 1992), ix.
8. Interview with David Healy in Bangor, Wales, September 4, 2009.
9. D. Healy, "Psychiatric bed utilization," Psychological Medicine 31 (2001): 779-90;
392
NOTES
D. Healy, "Service utilization in 1896 and 1996," History of Psychiatry 16 (2005):
37-41. Also, Healy, unpublished data on readmission rates for first-episode psy¬
chosis, 1875-1924, and 1994-2003.
10. Interviews with Yrjo Alanen, Jukka Aaltonen, and Viljo Rakkolainen in Turku,
Finland, September 7, 2009.
11. V. Lehtinen, "Two-year outcome in first-episode psychosis treated according to an
integrated model," European Psychiatry 15 (2000): 312-20.
12. Interview with Jaakko Seikkula in Jyvaskyla, Finland, September 9, 2009.
13. J. Seikkula, "Five year experience of first-episode nonaffective psychosis in open-
dialogue approach," Psychotherapy Research 16 (2006): 214-28. Also see:
J. Seikkula, "A two-year follow-up on open dialogue treatment in first episode psy¬
chosis," Society of Clinical Psychology 10 (2000): 20-29; J. Seikkula, "Open dia¬
logue, good and poor outcome," Journal of Constructivist Psychology 14 (2002):
267-86; J. Seikkula, "Open dialogue approach: treatment principles and prelimi¬
nary results of a two-year follow-up on first episode schizophrenia," Ethical Human
Sciences Services 5 (2003): 163-82.
14. Interviews with staff at Keropudas Hospital in Tornio, Finland, September 10 and
11,2009.
15. Outcomes for 2002-2006 study and for spending in western Lapland on psychiatric
services from interviews with Jaakko Seikkula and Birgitta Alakare. See also the
published papers by Seikkula, op. cit.
16. J. Cullberg, "Integrating intensive psychosocial therapy and low dose medical treat¬
ment in a total material of first episode psychotic patients compared to treatment as
usual," Medical Archives 53 (1999): 167-70.
17. W. Buchan, Domestic Medicine (Boston: Otis, Broaders, and Co., 1846), 307.
18. National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence, "Depression," December
2004.
19. Interview with Andrew McCulloch in London, September 3, 2009.
20. F. Dimeo, "Benefits from aerobic exercise in patients with major depression," British
Journal of Sports Medicine 35 (2001): 114-17; K. Knubben, "A randomized, con¬
trolled study on the effects of a short-term endurance training programme in pa¬
tients with major depression," British Journal of Sports Medicine 41 (2007): 29-33;
A. Strohle, "Physical activity, exercise, depression and anxiety disorders," Journal of
Neural Transmission 116 (2009): 777-84.
21. J. Blumenthal, "Effects of exercise training on older patients with major depres¬
sion," Archives of Internal Medicine 159 (1999): 2349-56.
22. Ibid.
23. Interviews with Tony Stanton and staff at Seneca Center in San Leandro, California,
July 13 and 14, 2009.
24. Interviews with Keith Scott and Chris Gordon, Framingham, Massachusetts,
October 1, 2009.
25. Ibid.
26. Interview with Jim Gottstein in Anchorage, Alaska, May 10, 2009.
NOTES
393
27. M. Ford, "The psychiatrist's double bind/' American Journal of Psychiatry 137
(1980): 332-39.
28. Myers v. Alaska Psychiatric Institute, Alaska Supreme Court No. S-11021.
29. Interview with Susan Musante in Anchorage, Alaska, May 10, 2009.
Epilogue
1. E. Whipple, Character and Characteristic Men (Boston: Ticknor & Fields, 1866), 1.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
As I began reporting this book, I reached out to leaders of various
"consumer" groups for help in locating "patients" to interview. I
wanted to find people with different diagnoses and of various ages,
and before long I had a list of more than 100 people willing to tell
me their stories. I am deeply grateful to all those who helped me
find patients to interview, and to all of those who spoke to me about
their lives. In addition to those named in the book, I want to thank
the following people: Camille Santoro, Jim Rye, Sara Sternberg,
Monica Cassani, Brenda Davis, Lauren Tenney, Cheryl Stevens,
Ellen Liversidge, Howard Trachtman, Jennifer Kinzie, Kathryn Cas-
cio, Shauna Reynolds, Maggie McClure, Renee LaPlume, Chaya
Grossberg, Lyle Murphy, Oryx Cohen, Will Hall, Evelyn Kaufman,
Dianne Dragon, Melissa Parker, Amanda Green, Nicki Glasser, Stan
Cavers, Cindy Votto, Eva Dech, Dennis Whetsel, Diana Petrakos,
Bert Coffman, Janice Sorensen, Joe Carson, Rich Winkel, Pat Risser,
Susan Hoffman, Les Cook, Amy Philo, Benjamin Bassett, Antti Sep-
pala, Chris LaBrusciano, Kermit Cole, David Oaks, Darby Penney,
and Michael Gilbert.
At every turn, the people I interviewed were extraordinarily
gracious with their time. In Syracuse, Gwen Oates, Sean Oates,
396
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
Jason Smith, and Kelley Smith welcomed me into their homes. In
California, Tony Stanton organized two days of interviews with ad¬
ministrators, staff, and children at the Seneca Center. Throughout
this project, David Healy answered my inquiries, and when I inter¬
viewed him in North Wales, he and his wife, Helen, proved to be
gracious hosts. The architects of Open Dialogue therapy in Finland
collectively spent a week with me. I'm deeply indebted to Yrjo Ala-
nen, Jaakko Seikkula, and Birgitta Alakare for making my trip there
possible, and to Tapio Salo and his family for a wonderful evening
of conversation in Tornio.
As I worked on this book, I regularly drew sustenance from
friends and family. Thanks to Jang-Ho Cha, I was able to attend a
brain-cutting seminar at Massachusetts General Hospital. Matt
Miller, an associate professor at the Harvard School of Public
Health, proved to be an invaluable sounding board for thinking
about how medical therapies are evaluated and assessed. Cynthia
Frawley, my next-door office "neighbor," drew the many charts that
grace the book. And thanks to Joe Layden, Winnie Yu, and Chris
Ringwald for our regular conversations about the ups and downs of
the writer's life.
This is my fourth book, and I am now more convinced than ever
that writing a book —from the moment of first conception to the
day of publication —is best described as a collective enterprise. My
agent, Theresa Park, helped me shape the proposal and provided me
with invaluable guidance as I worked on the project. My editor,
Sean Desmond, pushed me to broaden the book's scope and its nar¬
rative arc, and when it came time to edit the manuscript, he im¬
proved it in innumerable ways. Every writer should be so lucky to
have an agent as supportive as Theresa Park and an editor as tal¬
ented as Sean Desmond. I am also indebted to Rick Willett for his
skillful copyediting; to Laura Duffy for her eye-catching cover; to
SongHee Kim for her wonderful layout; to Stephanie Chan for her
diligent management of the project; and to the many others at
Crown who contributed their talents to this book. And finally, I am
deeply grateful to Tina Constable for believing that the history told
in Anatomy of an Epidemic is one that deserves to be known.
INDEX
Page numbers of illustrations appear in
italics.
Aaltonen, Jukka, 337
Abbott Laboratories, 317
Ability (aripiprazole), 144-45, 212, 320n
acetylcholine, 61
Adderall (amphetamine), 252
Advocates, Inc., 214, 354
Age of Anxiety, The (Tone), 132
akathisia, 232, 249, 285, 301
Alakare, Birgitta, 341
Alanen, Yrjo, 337, 338, 339
Alaska Mental Health Assn., 355-57
Ambien (Zolpidem), 23
American Foundation for Suicide
Prevention, 327
American Medical Association, 39; drug
company alliances, 54-57
American Psychiatric Association (APA),
161-64, 168-69, 264; annual meeting,
4-5, 11, 115-18, 172-77; diagnostic
categories and market growth, 161-62,
168, 317; drug company alliances,
172-73, 273, 276-89, 278n, 298-99;
lithium approval and, 183; lobbying of
Congress, 277, 280; marketing of
antidepressants, 289-91; medical
(biological) model, 269-76, 304;
practice guide to depression, 180-81;
silencing of dissent, 304-7; Textbook of
Neuropsychiatry, 221; Textbook of
Psychiatry, 161, 225-26, 311-12;
Textbook of Psychopharmacology, 82,
322; Treatment of Psychiatric
Disorders, 272-73; wooing of media,
273-74
amitriptyline, 73, 74n
amphetamines, 64, 219, 228-29, 236. See
also Benzedrine; Ritalin
Andreasen, Nancy, 113-14, 275, 304, 332
Angier, Natalie, 291
anti-anxiety agents, 4, 51-53, 60, 65,
126-47; bipolar illness and, 142, 144,
148; disability numbers and, 146-47;
long-term effects, 136-38; side effects,
29, 140, 296-97; withdrawal from,
133-36, 134. See also benzodiazepine;
specific brand names
antibiotics, 41-42, 45, 51, 56, 57
antidepressants, 4, 60, 61, 62, 70, 73,
79-82, 148-71; bipolar disorder and,
175-77, 180, 181, 186-96, 193, 195;
case studies, 24-26, 148-50, 171,
196-204, 211-12; as cause of mental
illness, 26, 28, 30, 81-82, 157-60,
169-71, 197, 234; children given,
160-61, 229-32; disability numbers
and, 167-68, 168; hiding the evidence,
308-9, 312; marketing of, 74, 289-91 ;
nondrug outcomes vs. , 153-57,
164-69, 164n, 166, 166n, 167, 169,
185-96, 193, 195, 362; paradigm for
psychotropic drugs, 83-84; relapse risk
and, 158, 162, 169, 186; sales and use,
3, 320; side effects, 175-76, 191,
398 •
211-12; tricyclics, 153-55, 157, 158,
186, 198, 199, 229, 234, 240, 249,
285, 288 withdrawal psychosis, 250.
See also SSRIs; specific brand names
antipsychotics. See neuroleptics
Antipsychotics and Mood Stabilizers
(Stahl), 192
anxiety, 28, 51-53, 126-47; case studies,
139-45; disability and, 5, 140, 146-47,
209; drug-based treatment, 129-39;
hiding the evidence, 307-12; iatrogenic
effects of drugs, 209; social anxiety
disorder (SAD), 317; treatment before
Miltown, 127-29. See also
benzodiazepines; panic disorder
Asberg, Marie, 72-73
Ashton, Heather, 134-35, 137, 138
AstraZeneca, 174, 302, 324-25
Ativan (lorazepam), 18, 140-41, 212
attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD), 10, 216-29, 325-26; ADHD
to Bipolar Pathway, 238 , 238; bipolar
disorder and, 319; case studies,
251-53, 258-60; drug therapy, 31-34,
220, 236-38; etiology unknown,
220-21; hiding the evidence, 309; low
dopamine theory, 77-78, 221; silencing
of dissent, 307. See also Ritalin
Ayd, Frank, Jr., 64, 151-52
Badillo, George, 20-24, 120, 121
Baldessarini, Ross, 96-97, 158, 160, 184,
188, 191,193-94
Banks, Brandon, 200-202
Barrow, Jonathan, 255-56
Bayer, 41
Bayh, Birch, 267
Beard, George, 127
benzedrine, 219
benzodiazepine, 28, 29, 126-47, 202,
267-68; addiction, 129-30, 140-41;
brain damage, 137-38; case studies,
139-45; disability and, 131, 138; fall
from favor, 129-31, 265, 267-68;
hiding the evidence, 308-9, 312;
long-term effects, 136-38, 159;
short-term efficacy, 131, 132-33; side
effects, 213; withdrawal, 129-30,
133-36, 134. See also Klonopin;
Miltown
Berger, Frank, 51-52, 65, 128-29
Berrick, Ken, 348-49
Biederman, Joseph, 173, 232, 238, 242,
318-19, 319n, 325-26, 325n
Bipolar Child, The (Papolos), 233, 319
bipolar disorder, 14, 17, 172-204;
academics paid by drug companies,
323; anti-anxiety agents and, 142, 144,
INDEX
148; antidepressants and, 175-77, 180,
181, 186; case studies, 16-20, 24-30,
196-204, 247-60; DBSA and, 13;
diagnostic boundaries and, 242,
317-18; disability and, 5, 7, 15, 25,
178, 179, 188, 194, 193, 196, 210;
drug-based treatment and worsening
of, 177, 184-87; drug cocktails, 190,
191, 192, 200-202; drug side effects,
13, 26, 181, 189-91, 191n; as
epidemic, 179-80, 235; etiology
unknown, 275; Harrow long-term
study, 194-96, 195; hiding the
evidence, 307-12; juvenile bipolar
disorder, 10, 32, 33, 173, 217, 232-46,
255, 318-19, 325, 325n, 353, 358;
lithium and, 175, 182-86, 189-90,
198, 200; long-term outcomes, drug vs.
non-drug, 177-79, 185-96, 193, 195;
marketing of, 317-18; number of cases,
179, 182, 193, 195-96, 210; Patty
Duke and, 174; rapid cycling, 175,
186-87, 199, 237, 239, 243; "ultra,
ultra rapid cycling," 243
Blaming the Brain (Valenstein), 61, 78
Blau, Theodore, 270
Bleuler, Eugen, 90
Bockoven, J. Sanborne, 100, 118
Bola, John, 102-3
Bostic, Jeffrey, 324
Bowers, Malcolm, 71, 75
Boyer, Francis, 58-61
Bradley, Charles, 219, 222, 223-24, 233
brain, 61-64, 67-68, 68, 69-70, 69, 77,
80-81, 106-7; benzodiazepines and
GABA inhibition, 135-36, 139;
chemical imbalance theory and, 10, 17,
33, 61-64, 70-85; damage from
psychotropic drugs, 104, 106-7,
111-12, 113-14, 137-38, 159-60,
170, 189-90, 192; D1 and D2
receptors, 75-76, 82, 105, 106, 107,
113, 309; Ritalin and, 221-22
Breggin, Peter, 230, 284, 287, 304-5
Briggs, Monica, 24-26, 197
Brinkley, John R., 283-84, 295
Bristol-Myers Squibb, 172, 320n
Brodie, Bernard, 61-62
Broken Brain, The (Andreasen), 275
Bronowski, Jacob, 3
Buchan, William, 344-45
Burke, Tomie, 240
Burns, Geraldine, 139-41, 142
Carlat, Daniel, 313
Carlson, Gabrielle, 234-35
Carpenter, William, 100-101, 103-4, 118,
119, 209
INDEX
Carter Products, 59
catecholamines, 62, 63
Celexa (citalopram), 324
Cha, Jang-Ho, 67
chemical imbalance theory, 10, 17, 33,
61-85, 74n, 264; disproved, 77-79,
358; marketing of drug therapy and,
291, 319-20; societal belief in, 78
Child and Adolescent
Psychopharmacology Made Simple
(O'Neal), 218
children and adolescents: academics paid
by drug companies and, 323-26, 325n;
ADHD in, 10, 31-34, 218-29; ADHD
to Bipolar Pathway, 238; alternative
treatment, 347-53; apathy syndrome,
232; case studies, 31-34, 247-60,
313-15; delinquency, crime, and mental
illness, 227, 257; depression in, 10,
217, 229-32, 318; diagnosis of mental
disorders, 10-11, 216-18; drug
cocktails and, 32, 33, 173, 244-45,
249, 254, 258, 313-15, 320, 349-50,
353, 359; as drug company market,
318-19; drug therapy, 31-34, 160-61,
218, 220, 231-32, 238-42, 318; drug
therapy, long-term outcomes, 34, 35,
222-29, 243-45; drug therapy, side
effects, 32, 222-29, 231-32, 244n,
247-60; epidemic of mental illness in,
8-9, 239-46, 241, 246; foster children,
medicating of, 253-57, 347, 348, 357,
359; GAO figures on, 246; juvenile
bipolar disorder, 10, 32, 33, 173, 217,
232-45, 255, 318-19, 325, 325n, 353,
358; lawsuits brought on behalf of,
357; number receiving SSI for mental
illness, 3, 8, 241, 245-46, 246, 358;
rarity of pediatric mania, prior to drug
therapies, 233-34
Children and Adults with Attention Deficit
Hyperactivity Disorder (CHADD), 220,
221, 327
Chouinard, Guy, 105-7, 108, 119, 176-77
Ciba-Geigy,219, 220
Citizens Commission of Human Rights,
281
Clayborn, Sam, 254-56
Clemens, James, 79-82
clonidine, 253
Clozaril (clozapine), 250
Cochrane Collaboration, 96n
Cogentin (benztropine), 18
Cole, Jonathan, 95, 99, 104-5, 118,
152-53
Concerta (methylphenidate), 32, 252,
259
Costello, E. Jane, 216
* 399
Crane, George, 180
Creation of Psychopharmacology, The
(Healy), 78
DelBello, Melissa, 237, 244, 324-25
de Montigny, Claude, 81
Deniker, Pierre, 49-50, 77, 78, 105
Depakote (divalproex), 33, 200, 203, 249,
255,315
depression, 68, 68-69, 70 , 81-82,
148-71; case studies, 24-26, 148-50,
171, 211-12, 214-15; in children, 10,
217, 229-32, 318; disability and
unemployment, 5, 7, 15, 149, 166,
167-68, 168, 209-10 ; drug therapy,
13, 53-54, 162-64; drug therapy and
worsening, 157-64, 170; drug therapy
vs. nondrug outcomes, 153-57,
164-69, 164n, 166, 166n, 167, 362;
etiology unknown, 275; exercise
therapy, 344-47, 346; hiding the
evidence, 307-12; low-serotonin
hypothesis, 71-75, 74n, 77, 289;
marketing of, 291; number of cases
163-64, 210, 290; revision of
diagnosis, 161-64; Saint-John’s-wort
and, 156-57; as self-limiting in nature,
152-53; "selling of," 291; selling of
Prozac for, 289-95
Depression and Bipolar Support Alliance
(DBSA), 12-15, 25, 26, 29, 181, 198;
drug company alliances, 317-18
desipramine, 24, 197
Deutsch, Albert, 44, 91
Dewa, Carolyn, 148, 165-67, 166n, 167
dextroamphetamine, 252
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders {DSM), 5, 10, 128,
177, 219; // (DSM-II), 269; III
(DSM-III), 218, 220, 269-70, 271,
295; Ill-Revised (DSM-III Rev), 220;
IV (DSM-IV), 317
Dobbs, Dorothy, 285-86
Domestic Medicine (Buchan), 344-45
dopamine, 61, 62, 63-64, 68, 69, 69, 70,
71; ADHD and low dopamine theory,
77-78, 221; D2 receptors blocked by
antipsychotics, 75-76, 82, 114;
schizophrenia and, 63-64, 69, 70,
75-77, 78; supersensitivity psychosis,
105-7
drug cocktails, 18: for bipolar disorder, 28,
177, 190, 192, 196-97, 200-202;
children given, 32, 33, 173, 244-45,
249, 254, 258, 313-15, 320, 349-50,
359; iatrogenic illness and, 192,
353-54; side effects, 211-15
Drugs and the Brain (Snyder), 108-9
400 • INDEX
Duke, Patty, 174
Durham-Humphrey Amendment, 56, 57
Effexor, 155, 171,213
Ehrlich, Paul, 39-41
electroconvulsive therapy (ECT), 44, 103n,
157, 171, 180-81, 197, 281
Eli Lilly, 42, 47, 172, 187, 320; drug
revenues, 321-22; drug trials, skewing
of, 230-31, 285-86, 288-89; fraud
and, 284-85; grants and payments to
influence opinion, 293, 294,, 317-18
327-28; lawsuits against, 292; Prozac
and, 74, 79-81, 154-56, 239, 284-91,
286n, 292-95, 318, 320; Zyprexa and,
208, 301, 320
encephalitis lethargica, 50, 90-91, 219
Epidemiology of Depression, The
(Silverman), 151
Essential Psychopharmacology, 74
Evarts, Edward, 95
Faedda, Gianni, 240, 242, 243
Fava, Giovanni, 159-60, 161, 165, 181
Fieve, Ronald, 182
Fink, Paul, 272
Flugman, Hal, 141-43
fluoxetine. See Prozac
Flynn, Laurie, 280, 302
Food and Drug Administration (FDA),
55-56, 94; MedWatch Prozac
complaints, 287-88; MedWatch report
on Ritalin risk, 236-37; Paxil approved
for "social anxiety disorder," 317;
prescription-only requirement for
drugs, 55-56, 56n; Prozac hearing,
294; Risperdal trials and, 300
Ford, Betty, 131
Forest Laboratories, 324
Franklin, Jon, 274, 276
Frazer, Alan, 71, 72, 78
Frazier, Jean, 173
Frazier, Shervert, 290, 298
Freedman, Daniel, 295
Freud, Sigmund, 127, 128, 265
GABA, 135-36, 139
Gately, Theresa, 253-54
Geodon (Ziprasidone), 173, 176, 203, 213
Ghaemi, Nassir, 172, 175, 177, 187
GlaxoSmithKline: academics paid, 322-23;
fraud and, 324; freebies to
psychiatrists, 327; Paxil and, 317, 322,
323-24
Glenmullen, Joseph, 78, 284, 307
Gold, Mark, 274-75
Good News About Depression, The
(Gold), 274-75
Goodwin, Frederick, 175-76, 181-82,
186, 323
Gordon, Chris, 354
Gottstein, Jim, 355-5 7
Grassley, Charles, 322
Gressitt, Stevan, 138
Haarakangas, Kauko, 341
Hagler, Dennis, 14
Hala, Rhoda, 292
Halcion (triazolam), 18
Haldol (haloperidol), 17, 18, 21, 200, 267,
299-300
Harding, Courtenay, 109-10, 118, 119,
193n
Harrow, Martin, 115-18, 116, 111, 119,
174-75, 185, 193n, 194-96, 195, 209,
227,310, 310n, 311, 312
Hayes, Inc., 244
Healy, David, 74-75, 78, 230, 283, 284,
305-6, 307, 353
Hellander, Martha, 240
History of Psychiatry, A (Shorter), 4
Hoffman-La Roche, 52, 60, 126, 129,
138
Houtsmuller, Elisabeth, 244
How to Become a Schizophrenic
(Modrow), 333-34
Hubbard, L. Ron, 281
Hyman, Steve, 77, 83-84, 85, 222
imipramine, 60, 62, 65, 70-71, 74n, 153,
249, 287. See also tricyclics
Infinite Mind, The (radio show), 323
iproniazid, 53-54, 60, 62, 70-71, 84-85,
180
Jackson, Grace, 356
Jamison, Kay, 28
Janssen company, 299-302, 325
Jenner, Alec, 126
Jensen, Peter, 226, 311
Jones, Barry, 105-7, 108, 119, 176-77
Judd, Lewis, 162, 182, 187, 279, 280, 291
Kefauver, Estes, 56
Kendler, Kenneth, 78-79
Kennedy, Edward, 131, 267-68
Keropudas Hospital, Tornio, Finland,
339-44,340, 361-62
Kessler, Ronild, 217
Kim, Julie, 3151
Klein, Rachel, 213-14
Klerman, Gerald, 264, 270, 295-96, 298,
304
Kline, Nathan, 47, 53, 65, 153
Klonopin (clonazepam), 29, 141-46, 212,
315
INDEX
Kraepelin, Emil, 90, 151, 152, 169, 175,
178, 186, 193n, 194, 198
Kramer, Peter, 294
Kuhar, Michael, 276
Kuhn, Roland, 65
Kurtti, Mia, 342
Lader, Malcolm, 133-34, 135, 137-38, 147
Lamictal (lamotrigine), 145, 323
Lappen, Steve, 13, 14, 198-99
Laughren, Thomas, 230
LeFever, Gretchen, 307
Leonhard, Karl, 178
Levin, Cathy, 16-20, 120-21
Lexapro (escitalopram), 145, 324
Librium (chlordiazepoxide), 52, 60, 129,
145
life expectancy, 176, 211, 214-15, 354
Listening to Prozac (Kramer), 294
lithium, 25-26, 17, 33, 145,175, 182-86,
189, 190, 197, 198, 200, 205, 212
lobotomy, 44, 45, 49, 84, 281
Lord, Nancy, 286-8 7
Luvox (fluvoxamine), 239, 249
Mad in America (Whitaker), 16, 31 On
"magic bullet" medicine, 39-42, 47, 49,
51, 54-61, 65, 78, 84-85, 206-7,
263-64
Magic Bullets (Sutherland), 41
Maine Benzo Study Group, 138
Manic-Depressive Illness (Goodwin), 175,
323
Manic Depressive Illness (Winokur), 178
MAOIs (monoamine oxidase inhibitors),
153,171
March of Medicine (TV show), 57, 58
Marks, Isaac, 295, 298
Marsilid (iproniazid), 53, 84
McCulloch, Andrew, 345, 347
McGlashan, Thomas, 100-101,103-4, 1 i4
McWade, Mathew, 256
Mendels, Joseph, 71, 72, 78
Mental Health (Satcher), 4, 9, 78
mental illness, 353; biological psychiatry
and, 304; chemical imbalance theory,
10, 17, 33, 61-64, 67-85; in children,
5, 10,216-46, 241,246;
deinstitutionalization of patients, 93,
206; drugs and revolutionizing of
treatment, 4-5, 9; economics of, after
passage of Medicare and Medicaid, 93,
206; epidemic, past 50 years, 5-9, 7,
39-46, 169-71; epidemic, social
factors, 208-10; epidemic as iatrogenic,
9, 11, 30, 195, 195-96, 205-15,
239-46, 241, 353; etiology unknown,
78-79, 220, 332, 358; history of
• 401
treatment, 12, 42-46; hospitalized
mentally ill, 6, 6, 44, 91, 100, 205;
milieu therapy, 103n; as self-limiting,
100; social policy and, 91-92, 206;
spending on mental health services,
328. See also children and adolescents;
specific disorders
mental illness, reform of treatment,
331-59; Alaska Project, 355-57;
alternative treatment, 43, 332, 335-36;
"best" use paradigm, 332, 334-35,
353; children and, 347-53; David
Healy and, 334-36, 353; as
evidence-based, 353; exercise therapy,
344-47, 346; Lapland, open-dialogue
therapy, 339-44, 340; Lapland
need-adapted treatment, 336-39;
medication withdrawal programs,
353-54; MindFreedom International
hunger strike, 331-33; truth in research
and marketing, 332-33
mephenesin, 51, 52
meprobamate. See Miltown
Merck, 41, 55, 60
Miltown (meprobamate), 52, 59, 65, 84,
126, 128-29, 146
MindFreedom International, 331-34, 358
Modrow, John, 333
Moniz, Egas, 49
Moodswing (Fieve), 182
Mosher, Loren, 102-3, 107, 118, 119,
209, 265, 271, 279, 304, 305, 356
M-Power, 18, 148,214
Musante, Susan, 357
Myers, Faith, 356
Myers v. Alaska Psychiatric Institute, 356
Myth of Mental Illness, The (Szasz), 264
Nash, John, 204
National Alliance on Mental Illness
(NAMI), 174, 302; drug company
alliances, 279-80, 317, 327; hiding the
evidence, 311; silencing dissent, 305
National Institute of Mental Health
(NIMH), 46, 153; bias for drug
therapy, 64, 272, 279; bipolar child,
description of, 237; bipolar disorder,
rates, 179; bipolar disorder vs.
schizophrenia and cognitive function
study, 189-90; CATIE Trial, 303;
Center for Schizophrenia Studies,
271-72; chemical imbalance theory,
61-63; chronicity of depression and
drug treatment, 158; Collaborative
Program on the Psychobiology of
Depression, 162, 168; depressed
patients and probably outcome,
163-64; Depression Awareness,
402 •
Recognition and Treatment (DART)
program, 279, 289-91, 316; depression
study, "naturalistic" outcomes, 167,
168; dopamine hypothesis, 75, 77; drug
company alliances, 289-91, 295, 298,
299, 316-17, 323; Harrow long-term
study of schizophrenia, 115-18, 116 ,
117 , 174-75, 193n, 194-96, 195, 209,
227,310,311,312; hiding the
evidence, 307-12; Hyman paper,
83-84; imipramine trial, 154;
low-serotonin theory of depression,
73-75, 74n; marketing of
antidepressants and, 289-91;
neuroleptics testing, 94-98, 96n, 118;
Psychopharmacology Service Center,
95, 96; Ritalin studies, 224, 226-27;
silencing of dissent, 304-7; Soteria
Project and, 271-72, 279, 304;
STAR*D trial, 163; STEP-BD study,
243-44; studies of schizophrenia,
100-102, 101; study of first-episode
psychotic patients, 1946-1950, 92;
Thorazine trial, 60-61
National Mental Health Act, 45-46
National Mental Health Association, 327
National Science Foundation, 42
Nemeroff, Charles, 322-23
neuroleptics, 4, 14, 50, 61, 64, 89-125,
107, 183; American spending on,
yearly, 3; atypical antipsychotics, 13,
14, 15,16, 18, 19,120, 244, 295,
299-302,318,319, 325; Bayh
investigation, 267; blocking of D2
receptors, 75-76, 82, 113, 114; brain
damage and, 104, 106-7, 111-14, 115,
192; case for, 95, 96-99, 96n, 108; case
studies, 20-24, 121-25, 213-14; hiding
the evidence, 308-9; how they alter the
course of schizophrenia, 98-104, 101 ;
long-term outcomes, 29, 89, 98-120,
101 , 1 16 , 117 , 159, 356, 361-62; as
magic bullet medicine, 61; MRI studies,
112; NIMH trials, 94-99, 96n, 118;
paradigm for understanding, 83-84; as
psychosis-inducing, 64, 82, 99-102,
107, 108-14, 120, 250; review of the
evidence, 118-20; sales and use of,
320-21; side effects, 13, 19, 20, 22, 29,
99,104-5, 107, 108, 111-14, 122,
191, 191n, 211; societal belief in,
154-55; supersensitivity psychosis,
105-7, 109; unpopularity with
patients, 267; used in children, 244,
318, 319. See also Thorazine; Zyprexa
Neurontin (gabapentin), 145, 200
norepinephrine, 61, 62, 68
Norton, John, 278n
INDEX
Oates, Gwendolyn and Sean, 31-32, 257,
258-60
O'Neal, John, 218
One Flew Over the Cuckoo's Nest (film),
264-65, 267
Orr, Louis M., 39
Oxford Textbook of Clinical
Psychopharmacology and Drug
Therapy , 223
panic disorder, 295
Papolos, Demitri, 233, 237, 239-40, 242,
243
Parents of Bipolar Children, 240
Pasnau, Robert, 298
Paxil, 149, 155, 314, 315, 317, 322, 323,
334; used in children, 323-24
Peele, Roger, 272-73
peer recovery movement, 24, 26, 148, 214
Pelham, William, 227
Pfizer, 41, 57, 172, 176
phenelzine, 153
phenothiazines, 48-51
Piatt, Arthur, 268
Post, Robert, 75, 175, 176, 187
promethazine, 48-49
Prozac (fluoxetine), 4, 5, 7, 74, 79-82,
80n, 171, 240, 284-91, 286n; children
dosed, 32, 229, 239, 318; drug trials,
154-56, 230-31, 285-88; Eli Lilly
campaign to save, 292-95; fraud and,
284- 85; lawsuits against, 292;
marketing of, 282, 294; as model for
drug development, 299, 303; sales
figures, 289, 321; side effects, 230,
285- 88, 292, 320; silencing of dissent,
307; story told in medical journals,
288- 89; story told to the public,
289- 91, 358
Prozac Backlash (Glenmullen), 78, 307
Prozac Survivors Group, 292
Pseudoscience in Biological Psychiatry
(Ross), 74
psychiatry, 4, 11, 20, 127-28, 266:
academics paid by drug companies,
276-80, 278n, 288-89, 322-27;
"alternative" therapies, 265;
antipsychiatry movement, 264-66, 304;
biological psychiatry, 63, 263-82, 304;
categories of disorders, 128; charlatans
and, 283-84, 295; children, diagnosis
of mental disorders, 10-11, 216-18;
critics, discrediting, 280-82, 292-95,
304-7; depression, pre-drug therapy,
151-53; diagnostic boundaries
expanded, 209, 242; drug-based
treatment paradigm, 4-5, 59-60, 177,
265, 266-76, 270; drug company
INDEX
403
alliances, 94-95, 276-89, 293-302,
304, 322-27; false story told by,
358-59; financial incentives for drug
therapy, 313-28; hiding the evidence,
307-12; history of treatment, 3, 4,
42-46; "key opinion leaders" (KOLs),
322-26; "magic bullet" medicine and,
263-64, 267; marketing of drug
therapy, 283-312; median earnings,
1970s, 265; psychopharmacology
revolution, 4, 47-66, 78, 265, 361;
rebranding of, 316-21; social
psychiatrists, 265
psychopharmacology, 4, 39, 205-15;
biological psychiatry and, 263-82;
development of drug treatment
paradigm, 47-66; drug sales in 1967,
64; drug sales to children promoted,
218; expectations for drug therapy,
64-65; financial incentives, 313-28;
long-term safety of agents, 65, 211-15;
"magic bullet" medicine and, 58-61,
78, 84-85, 185; medical-related
disability and, 196; paradigm for
understanding psychotropic drugs,
83-84, 207; Prozac marketing, 288-95;
psychiatrists profiting from drugs, 57,
295-302; thought experiment, 207-8;
Xanax marketing, 295-99, 297;
"young lady/old hag" analogy, 205,
206, 361
psychotherapy, 103n
PsychRights, 355, 356
Putnam, Robert, 208
Rakkolainen, Viljo, 337, 338, 339
Rappaport, Maurice, 101, 101-2, 107,
118, 119, 209
Recognizing the Depressed Patient (Ayd),
151-52
reserpine, 61-62, 72
Rhone-Poulenc, 48, 49, 51
Risperdal (risperidone), 18, 32, 33, 113,
200, 213, 255, 259, 325; biased drug
trials, 299-300; marketing of,
299-302; side effects, 18-19, 113,
120-21, 301n
Ritalin (methylphenidate), 77, 219, 220,
221-29, 22In, 252; case studies,
31-34, 251-53, 255-56; as cause of
juvenile bipolar disorder, 234-38, 241,
242; hiding the evidence, 309, 311;
risks/side effects, 219, 224-29, 236-38,
252; silencing dissent, 306-7
Robinowitz, Carolyn, 5, 11, 172
Rosenbaum, Jerrold, 293, 304, 307
Ross, Colin, 74
Rubin, Harvey, 273
Sabshin, Melvin, 264, 265, 268, 270, 271,
272, 273, 277
Saint-John's-wort, 156-57
Salo, Tapio, 341, 342
Sances, Melissa, 148-50, 171
Satcher, David, 4, 9, 78, 245
Schildkraut, Joseph, 62-63, 68, 70, 71-72,
78
schizophrenia 63- 64, 90-91, 109,
112-13, 151, 193, 193n; atypical
antipsychotics and, 295, 299-305; case
studies, 20-24, 120, 121-25; cognitive
impairment in, 189-91; discharge rate,
93, 100, 103n; dopamine hypothesis,
63-64, 69, 70, 71, 75-77, 78, 236;
drug treatment as psychosis-inducing,
107, 108-14, 192; disability and
employment, 93, 99, 100, 109-10, 111,
115, 120; etiology unknown, 275;
Harrow long-term study, 115-18, 116,
117, 174-75, 193n, 194-96, 195, 209,
227, 310, 310n, 311, 312 ; hiding the
evidence, 307-12; how antipsychotics
alter the course of, 98-104, 101;
Lapland non-drug treatment, 336-44;
long-term outcomes drug treatment vs.
non-drug, 89, 90, 92-94, 98-120, 101,
103n, 116, 117, 209, 312, 335-36;
Mosher's theory of cause, 102; MRI
studies, 112, 113-14, 119; natural
history of disorder, 90-94, 92n;
neuroleptic risk-benefit profile, 104-5;
NIMH drug trials, 94-99, 96n, 118;
relapse studies, 97-98, 97n, 99, 104;
"revolving door syndrome," 99, 103;
Risperdal, marketing of, 299-302;
short-term drug success, 95, 96n,
97-98, 99; silencing of dissent, 304-7;
Soteria Project, 102-3, 265, 271-72,
279, 304, 355-57; supersensitivity
psychosis, 105-7, 109, 176-77; tardive
dyskinesia and, 111-12; Thorazine
and, 92, 93
Schuyler, Dean, 153, 290
Scientology, 280-82, 292-95
Scott, Keith, 354
Seeman, Philip, 75, 107, 112-13
Seikkula, Jaakko, 339-41, 340, 343,
361-62
Seneca Center, 347-53
Seroquel (quetiapine), 23, 145, 200, 302
serotonin, 61, 62, 68, 68-69, 71;
depression and, 68-69, 70, 71-75, 74n,
289; "reuptake" inhibitors, 62, 73, 74,
79-82, 80n
Serzone (nefazodone), 155
Sexton, Scott, 214-15
Shader, Richard, 129
404
Shame of the States , The (Deutsch), 44
Shorter, Edward, 4
Silver, Ann, 120
Silverman, Charlotte, 151
Simon, John, 326
Smith, Jason and Kelley, 33-34, 257-58
Smith Kline and French, 57, 58, 59-60,
267
Snyder, Solomon, 75, 108-9
Solomon, Harry, 100
Soteria House, 102-3, 265, 271-72, 279,
304, 355-57
Spitzer, Robert, 269-70
Squib, 41
SSRIs (selective serotonin reuptake
inhibitors), 74, 79-82, 155-57, 170,
181, 303; as cause of chemical
imbalance, 81-82, 170; children given,
160-61,229-32, 238-42, 318;
disability and, 167-68, 168; lawsuits
against, 230; sales and use, 160-61,
294; side effects, 170, 231-32, 305-6;
silencing of dissent, 305-6; suicide risk,
230, 285, 286, 287, 292, 305-6, 315.
See also Prozac
Stahl, Stephen, 131-32, 192
Stanton, Tony, 349, 350, 352, 353
Stevens, Andrew, 251-53
stimulants, 177, 180, 219; ADHD to
Bipolar Pathway, 238, 238. See also
Ritalin
Stip, Emmanuel, 89, 98, 118
Stotland, Nada Logan, 172
Stowe, Zachery, 323
Strober, Michael, 234-35
Suavitil (benactyzine), 60
suicide risk, 25, 230, 243, 285, 286, 287,
292, 305-6, 315
supersensitivity psychosis, 105-7, 109,
160, 176-77
Supplemental Security Income (SSI) or
Social Security Disability Insurance
(SSDI), 6, 18, 206; anxiety disorders, 7,
140, 146-47, 209; bipolar illness, 142,
196, 199, 256; cost of, 10; depression,
149, 209-10; "entitlement trap,"
208-9; number receiving for mental
illness, 3, 7-8, 210, 241, 245-46, 246,
358
Sutherland, Louis, 41
Szasz, Thomas, 264, 266, 268, 281
tardive dyskinesia, 19, 104, 105, 107,
108-9, 111-12, 304,313
Tegretol (carbamazepine), 18
INDEX
Thorazine (chlorpromazine), 4, 39, 58, 82,
84, 92-94, 105-7, 183, 200, 206, 267;
development of treatment, 49-51;
"magic bullet" medicine and, 58-59,
206- 7; NIMH trials, 60-61, 96-98,
96n; rise in disabled mentally ill, 120;
side effects, 50, 63, 104-5, 107;
supersensitivity psychosis, 105-7
Tohen, Mauricio, 187, 189
Tone, Andrea, 132
Touched by Fire (Jamison), 28
tricyclics. See antidepressants
Upham, Amy, 204, 211-12
Upjohn, 131; APA partnership, 316;
marketing of Xanax, 295-99, 297; paid
psychiatrists, 295-96, 298
Valenstein, Elliot, 61, 78
Valium (diazepam), 126, 130-33, 134,
145, 147, 149, 303
Van Rossum, Jacques, 64, 70, 75
Vierling-Clausen, Dorea, 27-30, 196-97
Viguera, Adele, 97n
Wagner, Karen, 323-24
Wallace Laboratories, 52, 59
Wayne, Kim, 350
weight gain and drug therapy, 13, 29, 122,
140, 191, 203, 207, 214, 244, 249,
250, 301
Weinberg, Jack, 269
Weinstein, Haskell, 57
Wellbutrin (bupropion), 145, 322
Whipple, Edwin Percy, 361
Winokur, George, 153, 178-79, 183, 188
World Health Organization (WHO):
depression study, 165, 166;
schizophrenia studies, 110-11, 118,
119, 308, 310n, 312; Paxil trials, 156
Xanax (alprazolam), 131, 213, 296-97,
297, 298, 303; marketing of, 295-99,
297, 303
Zajecka, John, 299, 302
Zarate, Carlos, 187, 189, 311
Zoloft (sertraline), 149, 156-57, 171, 249,
345-46, 346
Zubin, Joseph, 96, 98, 169
Zyprexa (olanzapine), 27, 29, 113, 200,
214-15, 249, 301, 315, 320, 321;
marketing, 301-2; thought experiment,
207- 8; weight gain, 13, 29, 214-15,
249, 250, 301
(continuedfrom front flap)
long-term studies—all of which point to the same
startling conclusion—been kept from the public?
In this compelling history, Whitaker also tells
the personal stories of children and adults swept
up in this epidemic. Finally, he reports on innova¬
tive programs of psychiatric care in Europe and the
United States that are producing good long-term
outcomes. Our nation has been hit by an epidemic
of disabling mental illness, and yet, as Anatomy
of an Epidemic reveals, the medical blueprints for
curbing that epidemic have already been drawn up.
ROBERT WHITAKER is
the author of Mad in America,
The Mapmaker's Wife, and On
the Laps of Gods, all of which won
recognition as "notable books" of
the year. His newspaper and
magazine articles on the men¬
tally ill and the pharmaceutical industry have gar¬
nered several national awards, including a George
Polk Award for medical writing and a National Asso¬
ciation of Science Writers Award for best magazine
article. A series he cowrote for the Boston Globe on
the abuse of mental patients in research settings was
named a finalist for the Pulitzer Prize in 1998.
ALSO AVAILABLE AS AN EBOOK
Jacket design: LAURA DUFFY
Jacket illustration: DIETRICH MADSEN/GETTY IMAGES
Author photograph: B. D. COLEN
Crown Publishers
New York
4/10
www.crownpublishing.com
Printed in the U.S.A.
Exceptional Praise for
ANATOMY OF AN EPIDEMIC
"In making a compelling case that our current psychotropic drugs are causing as much—if not
more—harm as good, Robert Whitaker reviews the scientific literature thoroughly, demonstrating
how much of the evidence is on his side. There is nothing unorthodox here A this case is solid and
evidence-backed. If psychiatry wants to retain its credibility with the public, it will now have to
engage with the scientific argument at the core of this cogently and elegantly written book."
—DAVID'HEALY, MD, PROFESSOR OF PSYCHIATRY, CARDIFF UNIVERSITY,
AND AUTHOR OF THE ANTIDEPRESSANT ERA AND LET THEM EAT PROZAC
"This is the most alarming book I've read in years. The approach is neither polemical nor ideologi¬
cally slanted. Relying on medical evidence and historical documentation, Whitaker builds his case
like a prosecuting attorney." —CARL ELLIOTT, M D , PHD, PROFESSOR,
CENTER FOR BIOETHICS, UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA, AND AUTHOR OF BETTER THAN WELL
• In Anatomy of an EpideMic, investigative reporter Robert Whitaker cuts through flawed science,
greed, and outright lies to reveal that the drugs hailed as the cure for mental disorders instead
worsen them over the long term. But Whitaker's investigation also offers hope for the future: solid
science backs nature's way of healing our mental ills through time and human relationships."
—DANIEL DORMAN, M D , CLINICAL ASSISTANT PROFESSOR OF PSYCHIATRY,
UCLA SCHOOL, OF MEDICINE, AND AUTHOR OF DANTE'S CURE: A JOURNEY OUT OF MADNESS
"Anatomy of an Epidemic is a splendidly informed, wonderfully readable corrective to the conven¬
tional wisdom about the biological Jjases-and biological cures—for mental illness. This is itself
a wise and necessary book—essential reading for all those who have experienced, or care for those
who have experienced, mental illness—which means all ofus!" —JAYNEUGEBOREN,
AUTHOR OF IMAGINING ROBERT, TRANSFORMING MADNESS, 1940, AND OTHERS
"Every so often a book comes along that exposes a vast deceit. Robert Whitaker has written that sort
of book. Scrupulously reported and written in compelling but unemotional style, this book shreds
the myth woven around today's psychiatric drugs." —NILSBRUZELIUS,
FORMER SCIENCE EDITOR FOR THE BOSTON GLOBE AND THE WASHINGTON POST
"A devastating critique. . . . One day, we will look back at the way we think about and treat mental
illness and wonder if we were all mad. Anatomy of an Epidemic should be required reading for both
patients and physicians." —SHANNON BROWNLEE,
SENIOR RESEARCH FELLOW, NEW AMERICA FOUNDATION, AND AUTHOR OF OVERTREATED
Leave a Comment